Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:20:29.906Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can Appeals for Peace Promote Tolerance and Mitigate Support for Extremism? Evidence from an Experiment with Adolescents in Burkina Faso

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2022

Allison N. Grossman
Affiliation:
Immigration Policy Lab, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA. Twitter: https://twitter.com/allisonnamias
William G. Nomikos*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA. Twitter: https://twitter.com/wnomikos
Niloufer A. Siddiqui
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Rockefeller College of Public Affairs and Policy, University at Albany-State University of New York (SUNY), Albany, NY 12222, USA. Twitter: https://twitter.com/nilosiddiqui
*
*Corresponding author. Email: wnomikos@wustl.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Recent efforts to improve attitudes toward outgroups and reduce support for extremists in violent settings report mixed results. Donors and aid organizations have spent millions of dollars to amplify the voices of moderate religious figures to counter violent extremism in West Africa, the Middle East, and South Asia. Despite this investment, we know little about whether such messaging persuades the primary recruits of violent extremist organizations: at-risk youth in fragile settings. In this paper, we consider whether pro-peace religious messaging can promote social cohesion among school-age respondents in Burkina Faso. Using a survey experiment, we find little evidence that such messages affect reported attitudes or behaviors toward religious extremism and find instead that it can have the unintended effect of increasing intolerance toward ethnic others. Our findings carry lessons about the inadvertent priming of ethnic identities that can result in a backlash effect among certain societal segments.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Experimental Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

Can pro-peace messaging and appeals for tolerance improve attitudes and behaviors toward religious and ethnic outgroups and reduce support for violent extremism? We consider this question in the context of increasing Islamist influence among disaffected youth in conflict zones and fragile settings. To attract new adherents, many Islamist armed groups present their violent tactics as the solution to longstanding injustice and social exclusion. Such groups exploit pre-existing divisions, deepening antagonism among social groups. Increasing tolerance and rebuilding social cohesion are seen as necessary correctives in order to facilitate long-term peace and stability. To this end, domestic governments and foreign donors have funded projects to rebuild trust and tolerance toward out-groups in countries as diverse as Iraq, Nigeria, Afghanistan, and Liberia (Scacco and Warren Reference Scacco and Warren2018; Mousa Reference Mousa2020; Hartman, Blair, and Blattman Reference Hartman, Blair and Blattman2021). However, evidence that these programs achieve their desired results is mixed, particularly for populations most vulnerable to radicalization (Findley Reference Findley2018).

Drawing upon recent studies emphasizing the importance of religious messaging, this article tests whether religious appeals for peace shift at-risk youths’ attitudes and behavior toward out-groups and violent extremism in a setting with a growing Islamist extremist insurgency (McClendon and Riedl Reference McClendon and Riedl2015; Masoud, Jamal, and Nugent Reference Masoud, Jamal and Nugent2016; Condra, Isaqzadeh, and Linardi Reference Condra, Isaqzadeh and Linardi2019). International collaboration with local, moderate religious leaders has become an increasingly popular strategy to disseminate appeals for tolerance and counteract extremist narratives (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Littman, Nugent, Wolfe, Bukar, Crisman, Etim, Hazlett and Kim2021). We report the results of a pre-registered survey experiment, fielded in twelve schools located in rural and periurban Burkina Faso, in which we randomly assigned respondents to listen to audio excerpts of a sermon delivered by a nationally recognized imam that invokes Islamic teachings and appeals to a shared Burkinabé identity.

We find that participants’ self-reported ethnic identity conditions their responses to pro-peace messages. Although we anticipated that the treatment would have a uniformly positive effect on pro-social attitudes and behaviors, our results run contrary to our theoretical expectations. The treatments did not meaningfully shift most respondents’ support for religious extremism, attitudes toward religious out-groups, or civic participation. However, we find that appeals intended to promote peaceful coexistence reduced tolerance and trust of other ethnic groups. Further analysis shows that this result was driven by respondents who identify as Mossi, the country’s dominant ethnic group and one more likely to be at the receiving end of Islamist violence. Drawing upon our field research in Burkina Faso, we attribute the negative treatment effects to a backlash among this ethnic community.

Our findings suggest that pro-peace messaging may unintentionally prime ethnic identity in settings where religious extremism is associated with particular ethnic groups. This study thus contributes to a growing literature which raises questions about the effectiveness of commonly used policy measures aimed at increasing social cohesion and reducing violence, particularly when international interventions fail to tailor programs to local contexts (Autesserre Reference Autesserre2015; Barma Reference Barma2016; Swedlund Reference Swedlund2017; Campbell Reference Campbell2018).

Theoretical motivation

Where armed groups invoke religious doctrine to justify violence and recruit supporters, policymakers increasingly amplify the voices of moderate religious leaders as credible sources of pro-peace messaging. Many of these interventions draw on research that finds that religious leaders can encourage pro-social attitudes and behaviors by the nature of their divinely ordained authority, by promising divine rewards, or by drawing on scripture to persuade the faithful (Masoud, Jamal, and Nugent Reference Masoud, Jamal and Nugent2016; Condra, Isaqzadeh, and Linardi Reference Condra, Isaqzadeh and Linardi2019). Spreading the messages shared by these leaders is part of a broader strategy to counter violent extremism by winning the hearts and minds of potential recruits (Berman, Felter, and Shapiro Reference Berman, Felter and Shapiro2020). These, and other, international interventions seek to increase tolerance of out-groups and promote social cohesion to counter the hardening of cleavages and prejudice that result from sustained inter-group violence (Scacco and Warren Reference Scacco and Warren2018).

In conflict settings that increase the salience of specific social identities, priming cross-cutting cleavages may also provide another mechanism to bring together in-group and out-group members. Emphasizing superordinate identities, such as a shared religion or national identity to which both the in-group and out-group belong, has been shown to positively alter attitudes (Charnysh, Lucas, and Singh Reference Charnysh, Lucas and Singh2015; Siegel and Badaan Reference Siegel and Badaan2020; Kalin and Siddiqui Reference Kalin and Siddiqui2020).

However, there are many reasons to be cautious about the generalizability of these findings. While moderate religious leaders may theoretically be seen as impartial, in practice many such actors are politically engaged and may be perceived as such, undermining their credibility (Nomikos Reference Nomikos2022). Alternatively, religious leaders may be persuasive because they are part of dense, local networks; their messages may lose efficacy when removed from their context. Similarly, religious sermons facilitate behavioral change due, in part, to the collective social setting in which they are delivered (McClendon and Riedl Reference McClendon and Riedl2015). Even where studies have demonstrated that religious messages shift attitudes, recipients were persuaded only in some domains (Masoud, Jamal, and Nugent Reference Masoud, Jamal and Nugent2016). Moreover, extremist rhetoric may be convincing primarily because it is seen as challenging the status quo or because it is often accompanied by material inducements (Mitts, Phillips, and Walter forthcoming).

Finally, it is unclear whether these findings apply to adolescents, a primary target for recruitment by Islamist groups (Bizina and Gray Reference Bizina and Gray2014). Evidence across Africa suggests that armed groups recruit youth who feel left behind by society and locked out of alternative opportunities (Blattman Reference Blattman2009; Debos Reference Debos2011; Trisko Darden Reference Trisko Darden2019), relying on family or social networks as well as extensive propaganda and indoctrination campaigns to do so (Beber and Blattman Reference Beber and Blattman2013). For these youth, supporting a militant cause can provide a sense of purpose and belonging, offer economic stability, or strengthen group-based identity. Separate from joining militant groups, both direct exposure to violence and indirect exposure − growing up in violent environs, having family members and friends threatened by violence − affect young people’s attitudes and behavior, such as their willingness to engage in risk-taking behavior (Pat-Horenczyk et al. Reference Pat-Horenczyk, Peled, Miron, Brom, Villa and Chemtob2007; Blattman Reference Blattman2009).

Which types of development programs are most effective among adolescents in such settings remains an open question. An initiative focused on providing secondary education and civic engagement opportunities to Somali youth in Somaliand, for example, found mixed results on youth support for extremism (Trisko Darden Reference Trisko Darden2019). Whether pro-peace messaging, and specifically religious authoritative appeals, affect adolescents is even more unclear. Some studies have found that community members perceived as credible can prevent youth radicalization (Norman and Mikhael Reference Norman and Mikhael2017). Another study of youth in Ghana, however, found that religious organizations and churches, particularly in their role as messengers of theology, are not “significant socialization agents” in “informing notions of citizenship” and influencing political engagement (Kuperus and Asante Reference Kuperus and Asante2021). Whether such findings travel to settings with a growing Islamist insurgency is a question we take up here.

Research context

To test whether pro-peace appeals from religious leaders can counter support for violent extremism and promote intergroup tolerance, we fielded a survey experiment in Burkina Faso, a landlocked West African country of about 21 million people and the site of ongoing violence perpetrated by multiple Islamist extremist groups. Attacks by religious extremist groups have killed almost 2,500 individuals and displaced 500,000 others since January 2016 (see Figure 1). Footnote 1 These groups have proliferated in the context of a broader regional Islamic revival that has resulted in a “democratization of religious interpretation” centered on modern communication methods (Madore Reference Madore2013; Idrissa Reference Idrissa2017). Religious shows are regularly broadcast on the radio and audio recordings of sermons circulate widely. While this provides extremist preachers a podium through which to spread their messages, it also presents opportunities for moderate leaders’ counter-programming.

Figure 1 Fatalities and internally displaced persons in Burkina Faso: 2010–2020.

Islamist extremist groups have capitalized on longstanding grievances to recruit members of marginalized ethnic communities, primarily the Fulani, to mobilize against a central government dominated by the Mossi ethnic group. As Human Rights Watch notes, this situation has led to “a dramatic and dangerous increase in ethnic tension” (2019). The Mossi suffer disproportionately from armed Islamist violence, and state security forces target Fulani civilians for their perceived support of militants. Many militants operate in Fulani-inhabited areas, entrenching the perception that this group is to blame for shattering decades of perceived harmony and peaceful coexistence. Assumptions that all Burkinabé citizens share this idealized view of the past have influenced local and international policy response.

Research design

We administered an in-person survey experiment with 1,457 students in 12 schools in February–March 2020. Footnote 2 Given the age of our sample and the instability of the context, we strove to minimize any potential harm to participants. Following recently developed best practices for experiments in Africa (Davis Reference Davis2020), we collaborated with local stakeholders to develop and implement the study. This collaboration allowed us to secure approvals from school principals and teachers and consent from parents before students were approached to participate. Footnote 3 In an initial visit to Ouagadougou in March 2019, we also conducted interviews with academics, religious leaders, security analysts, and NGOs to assess the ethics and feasibility of studying radicalization and violent extremism in a quickly changing context. These interviews helped us ensure that our study design was both locally relevant and culturally appropriate.

The survey was administered in partnership with a local nongovernmental organization whose enumerators conducted face-to-face interviews with each respondent. We piloted the survey in January 2020 to further test the appropriateness and validity of the question language and approaches to measurement. All participants consented to be a part of the study, and only one participant refused to complete all parts of the questionnaire. We attribute the high response rate to the support of the local communities, principals, teachers, and parents. Footnote 4

We sampled schools from two regions, Ouagadougou and Kenedougou. Ouagadougou, the country’s capital, presents a peri-urban environment where youth participate in broad social networks that can be used for recruitment, while Kenedougou offers a rural context where extremists can leverage pre-existing grievances to recruit based on social identity. All students in the sample were enrolled in either sixth or seventh grade, with ages ranging from 12 to 18 and a median age of 14.

Respondents were randomly assigned to a control condition or one of four treatment conditions in which enumerators played an audio recording of Alidou Ilboudo, a prominent Burkinabé imam and radio host, speaking in 2019 to encourage intergroup tolerance. Footnote 5 Enumerators asked respondents to listen closely to the speech prior to playing the recording; respondents could then ask to listen again or pose clarifying questions. As such, we consider this a stronger treatment than if the excerpt had simply been read aloud by the enumerator or if the respondents read it themselves in an online survey; prior studies have detected statistically significant effects using both of these approaches. Footnote 6

We divide our outcomes into three categories. First, we measured respondents’ attitudes toward out-groups along two dimensions: their willingness to have other ethnic or religious groups as neighbors and their level of trust in others. Second, we asked respondents direct questions about their support for religious extremism, including whether they thought the use of violence was ever justified, though we acknowledge the possibility that these questions are too extreme to capture the nuances of lower levels of sympathy for extremist groups. Footnote 7 Third, a behavioral measure of civic participation at the end of our survey asked respondents to record a brief voice memo to express “support for peace and tolerance in your community” and send it to one of a number of local organizations or authorities. Footnote 8

We estimate the average treatment effect for each outcome with an OLS estimator given by ${Y_i} = {\beta _0} + {\beta _1}{Z_i} + {\varepsilon _i}$ where Y i is binary indicator of whether a given respondent i agreed or strongly agreed with a statement about an outgroup, violent extremism, or sent a recorded message. Footnote 9 Z i is the binary indicator for whether a respondent received the treatment or control. Given the large number of outcomes in which we are interested, we implement a parametric multiple comparison correction of our estimates for robustness (reported in full in Appendix E). We also specify the primary outcome variables in our pre-analysis plan as a design-based corrective for the multiple comparisons problem (Olken Reference Olken2015; Ofosu and Posner Reference Ofosu and Posner2021). Footnote 10

Results

We summarize the main results of our study in Figure 2. We find no evidence that the pro-peace message had any effect on attitudes about extremism (panel (b)) or the behavioral measure of civic participation (panel (c)). We also find that treatment effects on respondents’ attitudes toward members of religious out-groups were modest in magnitude and statistically indistinguishable from zero. However, we find that religious appeals toward peace reduce tolerance of ethnic others (panel (a)). Compared to the control, the audio of the imam’s speech reduced respondents’ tolerance of members of other ethnic groups as neighbors by 0.037 (95% CI = [−0.080, −0.004]) and respondents’ trust of members of other ethnic groups by 0.065 [−0.128, −0.001]. That respondents exhibited a reduction in tolerance along two different measures of attitudes towards ethnic out-groups suggests that the treatment did move respondents − just not in the manner expected. Footnote 11

Figure 2 Treatment effects (90% and 95% confidence intervals).

We find evidence that the salience of ethnic cleavages in response to religious extremism in Burkina Faso explains these results. We analyze heterogeneous effects by whether our respondents belonged to the dominant Mossi ethnic group or not. Footnote 12 Figure 3 reports the treatment effect of the pro-peace message on attitudes toward out-groups, attitudes about extremism, and civic participation by ethnicity. Footnote 13

Figure 3 Treatment effects by ethnicity (90% and 95% confidence intervals).

The results confirm that the Mossi ethnic group, which makes up 34 percent of our sample, largely drives our overall findings. First, as panel (a) of the figure shows, the pro-peace message had a negative treatment effect on attitudes toward out-groups among Mossi but not others. Among the Mossi, the treatment reduced tolerance of members of other ethnic groups as neighbors by 0.061 [−0.126, 0.003] and decreased trust of other ethnic groups by 0.169 [−0.277, −0.062]. Footnote 14 Moreover, whereas the treatment also reduced trust of other religious groups among Mossi by 0.083 [−0.190, 0.023], it increased trust of other religious groups among non-Mossi by 0.054 [−0.023, 0.132]. Footnote 15

Our analysis is less clear about the effect of pro-peace messaging on attitudes toward extremism and civic activity. As panel (b) shows, we find no evidence that the effect of the treatment on attitudes about extremism vary by ethnicity. However, this may be due to floor and ceiling effects; 90% of the respondents expressed concern about extremism and only 3% said violence could be justified. It is possible that at least some of these responses were driven by social desirability bias. However, given the randomized nature of the treatment, we should not expect this to influence the internal validity of the experiment itself.

Finally, we find some preliminary evidence that Mossi respondents in the treatment group were less likely to engage in pro-peace civic activity than those in the control group. As panel (c) shows, the treatment decreased the willingness of Mossi respondents to record and send a message to governing authorities in support of peace by 0.083 [−0.174, 0.008]. By contrast, we find that the treatment increased the willingness of non-Mossi to send a message, although this was not statistically significant at conventional levels. While we caution against drawing too much from these suggestive findings, this heterogeneity is noteworthy given that only a minority of Mossi respondents in any treatment condition were willing to send a message.

Discussion

In this section, we explore possible explanations for the null treatment effects observed in attitudes toward extremism or the behavioral measure of civic participation, as well as the negative treatment effects for tolerance and trust of ethnic others, observed particularly among Mossi respondents.

First, although the speech used in the treatments was originally delivered in a communal setting, the audio treatment was delivered individually, which may have blunted its impact. Since people often receive religious messages during collective services, they are most effective when connected to grassroots networks and enhanced by group dynamics. Nonetheless, studies using weaker treatments – enumerators reading a quote aloud to individual respondents – have found meaningful and statistically significant effects on similar outcome measures. We designed the experiment to maximize its external validity by choosing short excerpts to which we asked respondents to listen closely. While our experiment cannot evaluate the effectiveness of sustained interventions, it can shed some light on the potential (in)effectiveness of radio as a medium and of speeches as a format for narratives promoting tolerance among this demographic. Inconsistent reception, poor audio quality, and the myriad distractions of real life make it even less likely that young people listen attentively outside of the research context.

Second, it is possible that the treatments were simply not strong enough to elicit an effect on some outcome measures, or that students did not understand the message. To probe students’ understanding of the speech’s content, we administered a short phone survey to a different sample of 252 students who attend four of the schools in Kenedougou from which our original sample was drawn. Footnote 16 Students received the same audio treatments as those in the original sample and then responded to questions that assessed their understanding of the content of the speech. About 85% of the respondents understood that the speech was calling for peace and arguing against the use of violence, and a similar percentage correctly identified terrorism as the overall theme of the speech. These results suggest that the majority of students grasped the primary message of the speech. An open-ended question which asked about their understanding of the intention or purpose of the speech further supports these findings.

Another potential explanation for our null results is that participants simply were not paying attention. However, this is unlikely to account for the results we present here. Given that a vast majority of the respondents in the follow-up, phone survey comprehended the overall content of the treatment, we expect that an even higher percentage in the original, face-to-face survey would have paid attention to the treatment, even if they did not entirely understand its nuances. We instructed our enumerators to repeat any questions should the respondents need them to do so, including cases where respondents were not paying close attention, and to replay the treatment if need be. To document this protocol, we asked enumerators to note if they had any difficulties with any of the respondents. They recorded only 9 out of 1457 respondents as not paying attention during the survey. We drop these respondents in the results presented in the article but include them in supplementary analyses presented in Appendix D.5. We also rerun our analysis without the youngest respondents in our sample since these students might be the least likely to pay attention (see Appendix D.6). Our findings remain robust to either check.

Finally, and most importantly, the robust negative treatment effects we observe among Mossi respondents suggest that pro-peace messaging intended to counter Islamist extremism may have unintended consequences for other salient social cleavages. Although the current conflict in Burkina Faso is often framed as a consequence of growing religious extremism in the broader Sahel region, it has explicit ethnic overtones in the local context. To extend influence and gain adherents, extremist groups exploit existing tensions, such as those over access to land, which often follow ethnic lines. Consequently, ethnic groups who are perceived to be closely allied with militant groups, such as Fulani herders, face social stigma and distrust (International Crisis Group 2020). In interviews we conducted in Ouagadougou, interlocutors said that due to the increasing number of terrorist attacks, people treat non-coethnics with suspicion where they would have previously welcomed them.

The speech excerpts that the treatment groups received emphasized the importance of condemning terrorist violence and invoked a shared past wherein ethnic communities lived peacefully together – albeit, in an unequal manner. In so doing, the speech may have inadvertently primed Mossi respondents to recall negative ideas about groups that they blame for terrorist violence. Highlighting how violence has fractured existing social ties or is religiously unacceptable may result in a backlash against those groups perceived as sympathetic toward extremist groups. When majority group members develop stereotypes about the ethnic composition of terrorist groups, these stereotypes facilitate the logical fallacy that if all terrorists are members of a certain minority, all members of that minority support terrorism (Glaeser Reference Glaeser2005). These findings resonate with McCauley’s argument that religious and ethnic identity are mobilized in different ways and priming these identities may inspire different priorities (Reference McCauley2014).

Conclusion

In this article, we show that pro-peace messaging from religious leaders can have little effect on support for extremism and civic participation among youth in Burkina Faso and in fact may lead to a reduction in tolerance of ethnic out-groups. These findings are more likely to generalize to other ranked ethnic societies, where marginalized ethnic groups may be more susceptible to appeals by extremist groups to counter the status quo. In these contexts, members of marginalized groups may disproportionately align with extremists to seek protection or greater power. In response, dominant groups interpret this alignment as a threat to the existing social and political order and blame marginalized communities for violence. In the Sahel, members of certain ethnic groups are assumed to be terrorists and blamed for violence. For example, across Mali, Burkina Faso, and Niger, extremist groups target Fulani youth for recruitment. This has deepened social divisions and led members of dominant ethnic groups to regard all Fulani individuals with suspicion. These findings are consistent with a growing body of literature that documents how social identity conditions recipient views and behaviors toward foreign aid in fragile settings (Lyall, Shiraito, and Imai Reference Lyall, Shiraito and Imai2015; Arriola and Grossman Reference Arriola and Grossman2021).

Given the increase in violent ethnic reprisals in Burkina Faso in recent years, our findings imply that pro-peace messages should be tailored to the audience they seek to persuade. For the Mossi, who are the least likely to support religious extremism, but who may hold intolerant views toward groups they see as more susceptible to extremism, messages that seek to discourage extremism may instead make peaceful coexistence more difficult. As such, our results caution against broad, pro-peace messaging campaigns that do not account for variations in social relationships that condition recipients’ interpretation of pro-tolerance messages.

Supplementary Material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2022.1

Data Availability

Our pre-analysis plan is available via Evidence in Governance and Politics at https://osf.io/gp78nhttps://osf.io/gp78n. The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article are available at the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network, at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/TOBMYV (Nomikos, Grossman, and Siddiqui Reference Nomikos, Grossman and Siddiqui2021).

Acknowledgments

For comments on previous versions of the project, we thank Vivek Ashok, Natália Bueno, Guadalupe Tuñón, Thomas Zeitzoff, John McCauley, Justine Davis, Laura Jakli, Elizabeth Nugent, Daniel Masterson, Andrea Morris, Luwei Ying, and Christopher Lucas. We are grateful for help from our colleagues in the Innovation for Poverty Action office in Burkina Faso, especially Adama Sankoudouma and Dounialou Rose Yara.

Funding

Funding for this project was provided in part by the UK Foreign, Commonwealth & Development Office, awarded through Innovation for Poverty Action’s Peace & Recovery Program. We also acknowledge the financial support of the Weidenbaum Center on the Economy, Government, and Public Policy at Washington University in St. Louis and the Innovation for Poverty Action’s Peace and Recovery Program.

Ethics Statement

The survey received approval from the Institutional Review Board of Washington University in St. Louis (Protocol no. 202001048). Our research adheres to APSA’s Principles and Guidance for Human Subjects Research. More information about ethics is provided in the supplementary appendix B.2.

Conflicts of Interest

We report no conflicts of interest. All errors and omissions are our own.

Footnotes

This article has earned badges for transparent research practices: Open Data and Open Materials. For details see the Data Availability Statement.

1 Fatalities data from the Armed Conflict Location & Event Data Project (Raleigh et al. Reference Raleigh, Linke, Hegre and Karlsen2010) and displacements data from the Internal Displacement Monitoring Centre.

2 The data along with the replication code are available on the Journal of Experimental Political Science dataverse (Nomikos, Grossman and Siddiqui Reference Nomikos, Grossman and Siddiqui2021).

3 See Appendix B.2 for expanded discussion of ethics.

4 For nonresponse, see Appendix C.3.

5 We randomly varied the speech excerpts provided in the message and whether the respondent was told the identity of the speaker. Respondents were balanced across all treatments (see Appendix C.2), and few differences were found between individual treatment groups (see Appendix D.3). The results discussed in this paper are the pooled treatment effects.

6 We were careful not to fabricate a new speech, but rather adopted an existing message intended in its original form to promote peace and tolerance and made at a public gathering.

7 We chose these measures primarily to ensure comparability with Afrobarometer and Pew surveys, where these questions are frequently used. See Appendix C.4.

8 We also asked respondents about their national identities and attitudes toward foreigners for which we found no treatment effects. We omit these for space considerations but report the results in Appendix D.

9 Although answers to each question ranged on a five-point Likert scale we re-coded each outcome as binary for ease of interpretation. See Appendix D.2 for robustness check with other coding.

10 We include the preanalysis plan with a discussion of deviations in Appendix F.

11 This result is not robust to multiple comparison corrections (Appendix E.1).

12 Our preanalysis plan specified analysis of heterogeneous effects by ethnic group (see Appendix F).

13 Given the religious nature of the treatment, we expected it to have a greater effect among Muslim respondents, which our supplementary analyses, presented in Appendix D.4, confirm. Mossi Muslims in particular appear to drive the effect, but we are unable to draw definitive conclusions because there were few non-Muslim Mossi in our sample (n = 2).

14 This estimate is robust to multiple comparison corrections (see Appendix E.2).

15 We report this result because in some of the model specifications we pre-registered, this result is statistically significant at conventional levels (see Appendix D).

16 The students were similar to our initial Kenedougou sample along most covariates, differing primarily in expressing greater religiosity (see Appendix C.1). We conducted the survey over the phone because of the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic.

References

Arriola, Leonardo R. and Grossman, Allison Namias. 2021. Ethnic Marginalization and (Non) Compliance in Public Health Emergencies. Journal of Politics 83(3): 807–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Autesserre, Severine. 2015. Peaceland: Conflict Resolution and the Everyday Politics of International Intervention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barma, Naazneen. 2016. The Peacebuilding Puzzle: Political Order in Post-Conflict States. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beber, Bernd and Blattman, Christopher. 2013. The Logic of Child Soldiering and Coercion. International Organization 67(1): 65104.Google Scholar
Berman, Eli, Felter, Joseph H. and Shapiro, Jacob N.. 2020. Small Wars, Big Data: The Information Revolution in Modern Conflict. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bizina, Margarita and Gray, David H.. 2014. Radicalization of Youth as a Growing Concern for Counter-Terrorism Policy. Global Security Studies 5(1): 72–9.Google Scholar
Blair, Graeme, Littman, Rebecca, Nugent, Elizabeth R., Wolfe, Rebecca, Bukar, Mohammed, Crisman, Benjamin, Etim, Anthony, Hazlett, Chad and Kim, Jiyoung. 2021. Trusted Authorities can Change Minds and Shift Norms During Conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118(42): e2105570118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blattman, Christopher. 2009. From Violence to Voting: War and Political Participation in Uganda. American Political Science Review 103(2): 231–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Susanna P. 2018. Global Governance and Local Peace: Accountability and Performance in International Peacebuilding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charnysh, Volha, Lucas, Christopher and Singh, Prerna. 2015. The Ties that Bind: National Identity Salience and Pro-social Behavior toward the Ethnic Other. Comparative Political Studies 48(3): 267300.Google Scholar
Condra, Luke, Isaqzadeh, Mohammad and Linardi, Sera. 2019. Clerics and Scriptures: Experimentally Disentangling the Influence of Religious Authority in Afghanistan. British Journal of Political Science 49(2): 401–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, Justine M. 2020. Manipulating Africa? Perspectives on the Experimental Method in the Study of African Politics. African Affairs 119(476): 452–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Debos, Marielle. 2011. Living by the Gun in Chad: Armed Violence as a Practical Occupation. The Journal of Modern African Studies 49(3): 409–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Findley, Michael G. 2018. “Does Foreign Aid Build Peace?Annual Review of Political Science 21: 359–84.Google Scholar
Glaeser, Edward L. 2005. “The Political Economy of Hatred.The Quarterly Journal of Economics 120(1): 4586.Google Scholar
Hartman, Alexandra C., Blair, Robert A. and Blattman, Christopher. 2021. “Engineering Informal Institutions: Long-run Impacts of Alternative Dispute Resolution on Violence and Property Rights in Liberia.The Journal of Politics 83(1): 381–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idrissa, Rahmane. 2017. The Politics of Islam in the Sahel: Between Persuasion and Violence. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
International Crisis Group. 2020. “Burkina Faso: Stopping the Spread of Violence.” International Crisis Group (287).Google Scholar
Kalin, Michael and Siddiqui, Niloufer. 2020. National Identity, Religious Tolerance, and Group Conflict: Insights from a Survey Experiment in Pakistan. Conflict Management and Peace Science 37(1): 5882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperus, Tracy and Asante, Richard. 2021. Christianity, Citizenship, and Political Engagement among Ghanaian Youth. African Studies Quarterly 20(2): 3761.Google Scholar
Lyall, Jason, Shiraito, Yuki and Imai, Kosuke. 2015. “Coethnic Bias and Wartime Informing.The Journal of Politics 77(3): 833–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madore, Frédérick. 2013. Imams, Islamic Preachers and Public Space in Ouagadougou (Burkina Faso) since the 1990s: Toward New Intergenerational Relationships and a Muslim Public Sphere. In 2013 Africa Conference - Social Movements, Political Expression and Religion in Africa.Google Scholar
Masoud, Tarek, Jamal, Amaney and Nugent, Elizabeth. 2016. Using the Qur’ān to Empower Arab Women? Theory and Experimental Evidence from Egypt. Comparative Political Studies 49(12): 1555–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, John F. 2014. The Political Mobilization of Ethnic and Religious Identities in Africa. American Political Science Review, 801–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClendon, Gwyneth and Riedl, Rachel Beatty. 2015. Religion as a Stimulant of Political Participation: Experimental Evidence from Nairobi, Kenya. The Journal of Politics 77(4): 10451057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitts, Tamar, Phillips, Gregoire and Walter, Barbara. forthcoming. Studying the Impact of ISIS Propaganda Campaigns. Journal of Politics.Google Scholar
Mousa, Salma. 2020. “Building Social Cohesion between Christians and Muslims Through Soccer in Post-ISIS Iraq.Science 369(6505): 866–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nomikos, William G. 2022. Peacekeeping and the Enforcement of Intergroup Cooperation: Evidence from Mali. Journal of Politics 84(1): 194–208.Google Scholar
Nomikos, William, Grossman, Allison and Siddiqui, Niloufer. 2021. “Replication Data for: Can Appeals For Peace Promote Tolerance and Mitigate Support for Extremism? Evidence from an Experiment with Adolescents in Burkina Faso.” URL: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/TOBMYV CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, Julie and Mikhael, Drew. 2017. “Youth radicalization is on the rise. Here’s what we know about why.” Washington Post Monkey Cage.Google Scholar
Ofosu, George K. and Posner, Daniel N.. 2021. “Pre-Analysis Plans: An Early Stocktaking.Perspectives on Politics 117.Google Scholar
Olken, Benjamin A. 2015. “Promises and Perils of Pre-analysis Plans.Journal of Economic Perspectives 29(3): 6180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pat-Horenczyk, Ruth, Peled, Osnat, Miron, Tomer, Brom, Daniel, Villa, Yael and Chemtob, Claude. 2007. Risk-taking Behaviors among Israeli Adolescents Exposed to Recurrent Terrorism: Provoking Danger Under Continuous Threat? American Journal of Psychiatry 164(1): 6672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raleigh, Clionadh, Linke, Andrew, Hegre, Håvard and Karlsen, Joakim. 2010. Introducing ACLED: An Armed Conflict Location and Event Dataset. Journal of Peace Research 47(5): 651–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scacco, Alexandra and Warren, Shana S.. 2018. Can Social Contact Reduce Prejudice and Discrimination? Evidence from a Field Experiment in Nigeria. American Political Science Review 112(3): 654–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, Alexandra A. and Badaan, Vivienne. 2020. #No2Sectarianism: Experimental Approaches to Reducing Sectarian Hate Speech Online. American Political Science Review 114(3): 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swedlund, Haley J. 2017. The Development Dance: How Donors and Recipients Negotiate the Delivery of Foreign Aid. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trisko Darden, Jessica. 2019. “Tackling Terrorists’ Exploitation of Youth.” Washington, DC: American Enterprise Institute pp. 121.Google Scholar
Watch, Human Rights. 2019. We Found Their Bodies Later That Day: Atrocities by Armed Islamists and Security Forces in Burkina Faso’s Sahel Region. New York, NY: Human Rights Watch.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Fatalities and internally displaced persons in Burkina Faso: 2010–2020.

Figure 1

Figure 2 Treatment effects (90% and 95% confidence intervals).

Figure 2

Figure 3 Treatment effects by ethnicity (90% and 95% confidence intervals).

Supplementary material: Link

Grossman et al. Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Grossman et al. supplementary material

Grossman et al. supplementary material

Download Grossman et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 387.7 KB