We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Communication and cognition are presented as deeply interrelated aspects of the mind, the means by which animals perceive, respond to, and understand each other as well as their world. This chapter reviews chemosensory, vibrational (acoustic and seismic), visual and tactile sense modalities, the various ways in which people have attempted to exploit these sensory channels to manage problematic behaviors, and the ways in which anthropogenic disturbances and pollutants can interfere with signaling. It then delves into domains such as self-awareness, personality, problem-solving, cooperation, social learning, and culture. The chapter considers intriguing adaptive hypotheses such as that of cognitive buffering, before provoking reflection on the downstream consequences of social disturbance and trauma. Drawing on experimental studies on elephants and a range of other species from honeybees to whales, the comparative perspective positions cognitive abilities within their broader ecological and evolutionary contexts, and highlights why it is crucial to account for phenomena such as social learning and culture in protecting and managing elephant populations.
Those at genetic risk for Alzheimer's Disease (AD) because of the ApoE ε4 allele show differences in activation during olfactory information processing and memory in areas such as MTL structures, entorhinal cortex, posterior cingulate, precuneus, and inferior parietal lobule, suggesting preclinical AD neuropathology and olfactory impairment as a biomarker for predicting later AD onset (Murphy, 2019). The effects of smoking on AD have varied, with early studies suggesting either no effect or protective effects, and recent studies suggesting smoking as a risk factor for AD but with the need for further investigation in preclinical stages. Therefore, this study focused on olfaction and smoking as risk factors for preclinical AD neuropathology by studying differences in fMRI BOLD signal changes in smokers and nonsmokers during olfactory tasks.
Participants and Methods:
Archival data from 25 non-demented older adults recruited from the UCSD Alzheimer's Disease Research Center who completed an Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog) and functional MRI scans at 3T, acquired during performance of an odor identification task. Odor Identification (OI) measured correct (hits) or incorrect (misses) identification of odors presented by an olfactometer to deliver the odor stimuli in short, controlled durations during fMRI scanning.
Results:
fMRI data were preprocessed using fMRIprep, smoothed at 4mm, scaled, and first level analyses were conducted using 3dDeconvolve in AFNI with time points corresponding to hits and misses as regressors. Differences between smokers and nonsmokers revealed smokers show a larger difference in BOLD signal change from hits minus misses at five significant clusters (p = 0.01 with the minimum cluster size [voxels] at 42). Peak areas of significant clusters included the right precuneus, right calcarine gyrus, left inferior parietal lobule, left superior parietal lobule, and left middle occipital gyrus. Analyses suggested a greater difference in activity between hits and misses in smokers compared to nonsmokers, with more activity during hits.
Conclusions:
Differences in activation between smokers and nonsmokers during an olfactory identification task, with greater activity in smokers during hits, suggests greater effort to correctly identify an odor. These findings of hyperactivation in areas (such as the precuneus and inferior parietal lobule) are similar to findings of hyperactivation during odor memory observed in studies of ε4 carriers during preclinical stages. Results provide further insight into smoking as a risk factor for AD. Moreover, results suggest the risk of smoking could potentially be reflected in altered activity in olfactory information processing networks in preclinical stages of AD. The study highlights the need for research to further understand the role smoking plays in the development of AD and the use of olfaction as a biomarker to aid in disease detection, prevention, and stage-associated treatments.
Olfactory function declines during normal aging; however, accelerated olfactory decline is observed in neurodegenerative diseases, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Moreover, olfactory deficits in pre-clinical AD are associated with future cognitive decline. Odor identification and memory deficits have been consistently reported in early stage AD indicating its potential sensitivity to AD pathophysiology in olfactory and limbic structures, yet few studies of olfaction have incorporated structural measures in a well-characterized cohort of older adults. In the current study we examined the association between odor identification impairment, cognition, and medial temporal lobe (MTL) sub-regions in cognitively unimpaired and impaired older adults.
Participants and Methods:
We enrolled 140 participants (age=72.25±6.54, 56% female, years of education=16.30±2.63, 82% Caucasian, 15% Black/AA, 3% Multiracial) from the Penn Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center Clinical Cohort. Participants completed the Sniffin’ Sticks Odor Identification Test (SS-OIT), cognitive testing (NACC UDS2 or UDS3 and additional cognitive tests), and MRI scans (3T Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma MRI scanner). For the SS-OIT, participants were presented with 16 odorants using felt-tipped pen dispensers and asked to identify each odor from four multiple-choice options. Scores range from 0 to 16. Additionally, cognitive domains were created by averaging z-scores from tests within each domain: attention, memory, language, executive function, and visuospatial. This cohort was divided into participants with unimpaired cognition (n=96) and impaired cognition (MCI, dementia; n=44) using established normative data and consensus diagnosis. Linear regressions were performed to examine the association between SS-OIT score, each cognitive domain, and MTL measurements for unimpaired and impaired groups. For all analyses, we controlled for age, race, sex, education, smoking status, and hypertension and additionally for MOCA score and intracranial volume with MTL measurements.
Results:
In the unimpaired group, SS-OIT significantly associated with language (p<.05). In the impaired group, SS-OIT significantly associated with language and memory (p<.05). In the unimpaired group, SS-OIT significantly associated with right anterior hippocampal volume (p<.05). In the impaired group, significant associations were found between SS-OIT and right anterior hippocampal volume (p<.05) and left hippocampal mean thickness (p<.05). Additionally, SS-OIT significantly associated with left and right entorhinal cortex volume (p<.05) and mean thickness (p<.05).
Conclusions:
This study reveals that lower odor identification performance is related to lower performance on measures of cognition and atrophy in MTL sub-regions in unimpaired and impaired older adults. Our findings support prior results demonstrating relationships between olfactory function, cognition, and MTL sub-regions. Specifically, olfactory function and episodic memory have been shown to follow similar patterns of decline in the course of AD, potentially reflecting AD pathology in shared regions of the MTL subserving episodic memory and olfactory function. Our findings demonstrate that reductions in both cortical thickness and grey matter volume of MTL regions are linked to olfactory deficits in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s dementia. Future steps will include the analysis of longitudinal cognitive and imaging indices and the incorporation of fluid biomarker data.
One of the greatest challenges of the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) epidemic is identifying the disease prior to substantial neurological compromise. The established biomarkers of AD, such as measures of cognitive impairment, hippocampal atrophy, and CSF measures of beta amyloid and tau, used in research and drug trials are less indicative of AD pathology in preclinical, non-demented, populations. Olfactory dysfunction, a well-established sensory impairment of AD found to correlate strongly with tau burden and hippocampal volume measures, has shown to be a promising preclinical biomarker for AD progression. Several studies have found either impaired odor identification or odor memory at baseline to predict 5-year follow-up cognitive decline and conversion from MCI to AD, but less is known about how olfactory performance reflects the integrity of associated brain regions such as the hippocampus. The present analysis aims to explore the value of psychophysical olfactory assessment as biomarker measure in preclinical AD studies and drug trials by investigating its relationships with structural measures of the hippocampus.
Participants and Methods:
A sample consisted of non-demented older adults (age >75), recruited from the UCSD Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center as part of a ongoing olfactory biomarker study. Participants completed the AD Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale-13 (ADAS-Cog-13), San Diego Odor Identification Test (SDOIT), tests of odor recognition memory (ORMem) and odor associative memory (OAM), and MRI derived hippocampal volumes and average hippocampal occupancy (Avg HOC). Left and right hippocampal volumes were adjusted for each participant’s estimated intracranial volume. Bivariate correlations were calculated for ADAS-Cog-13 and SDOIT total scores, performance scores for odor recognition and odor associative memory tests, and the three hippocampal measures (bilateral volumes and average occupancy).
Results:
ADAS-Cog-13 score did not show significant correlations with either hippocampal measure at the .05 level. SDOIT scores were significantly correlated with the measure of Avg HOC (p<.05). ORMem false positive responses were significantly correlated with Avg HOC (p<.01) and right hippocampal volume (p<.05). ORMem miss responses and OAM errors were both correlated with left (p<.05) and right (p<.01) hippocampal volumes.
Conclusions:
These results demonstrate that psychophysical assessments of odor identification and odor memory can better reflect the integrity of the hippocampus in nondemented older adults, compared to the neuropsychological ADAS-Cog-13. This is congruent with olfactory dysfunction preceding cognitive-memory decline in AD cases and provides support for the utility of psychophysical olfactory assessment along with other established AD biomarkers in research and drug trials in preclinical populations.
Acknowledgements:
Supported by NIH grant # R01AG062006-04 from the National Institute on Aging to CM. Special thank you to the staff and participants of the UCSD ADRC, especially Christina Gigliotti, and Aaron Jacobson at the UCSD Center for fMRI.
Olfaction is a critical sensory function and changes in the ability to detect smells could affect quality of life by diminishing appreciation of food, drink, and other aroma-based experiences, increase danger of hazardous exposures, and cause a loss of employment. Additionally, decrements in olfaction have been related to onset of some neurodegenerative conditions. Olfactory impairments in military populations are highly prevalent and often attributed to the long-term effects of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) and chronic psychiatric disorders. The main goal of this investigation was to examine olfactory function in a cohort of combat veterans using a quantitative smell test.
Participants and Methods:
Participants underwent a neurological examination using a revised version of the Neurological Outcome Scale for Traumatic Brain Injury. Olfactory function was examined using a set of essential oil vials with common odors. Based on the number of correctly identified odors, the following grading system was employed: no deficit; mild; moderate; severe deficit; and absence of smell detection. All study assessments were performed prior to March of 2020 (onset of COVID-19 pandemic). In addition, participants completed performance validity testing (PVT) and screening for ongoing substance misuse using the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test and Drug Abuse Screening Test-10. Lifetime history of brain injury, combat-related extracranial injuries, and deployment characteristics were assessed using structured interview. All available medical records were reviewed.
Results:
Participants were 38 veterans with a deployment-related mTBI who passed the PVT and did not have ongoing substance misuse issues. Olfactory examination revealed normosmia in 20 participants and various degrees of deficit in 18 (11= mild; 4=moderate; and 3=severe). The groups did not differ in demographics, post-injury interval, or current clinical (non-psychiatric) conditions. Participants with hyposmia frequently reported being exposed to a higher number of blasts and being positioned closer to the nearest primary blast, and more often endorsed a period of loss of consciousness after the most serious mTBI. In addition, they more often reported tympanic membrane perforation, extracranial injuries, and histories of both blast and blunt force mTBI. Comorbid diagnoses of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, chronic headaches, and pain were more common among these participants as well.
Conclusions:
Several blast exposure and specific injury-related characteristics increase the likelihood of long-term olfactory impairments, comorbid psychiatric conditions, and chronic pain among veterans with a history of deployment-related mTBI. Notably, none of the participants with hyposmia had a clinical diagnosis of olfactory dysfunction or were receiving service-connected disability for a loss of sense of smell at the time of their assessment. Multidisciplinary rehabilitation care provided to combat veterans with history of mTBI and/or PTSD should include olfactory examination using both quantitative and qualitative smell tests, education regarding the adversities related to loss of smell, management of current psychiatric symptoms, and follow-up assessments. The lack of a comparison group without a history of mTBI and the small sample size were the main limitations of this investigation.
Olfactory dysfunction can influence nutritional intake, the detection of environmental hazards, and quality of life. Prior research has found discordance between subjective and objective measures of olfaction. In people living with HIV (PLWH), olfactory dysfunction is widely reported; however, few studies have examined concordance between subjective olfactory self-ratings and performance on an objective psychophysical measure of olfaction and associated factors in men living with HIV (MLWH).
Participants and Methods:
MLWH (n=51, mean age=54 years, 66.7% Black) completed two subjective olfaction ratings (two 5-point Likert scales), the Smell Identification Test (SIT), cognitive measures (HVLT-R, TMT), and self-report questionnaires assessing smell habits, mood, cognitive failures, and quality of life. Participants were categorized into one of four groups: true positives (TP; impaired subjective olfaction and objective olfaction dysfunction), false negatives (FN; intact subjective olfaction and objective olfaction dysfunction), false positives (FP; impaired subjective olfaction and objective normosmia), and true negatives (TN; intact subjective olfaction and normosmia). Established formulas were used to calculate the sensitivity and specificity of subjective olfaction, and t-tests and ANOVA were used to examine potential demographic, clinical, and cognitive factors contributing to discordance between subjective and objective olfaction dysfunction.
Results:
Across both subjective self-report items, 35.3% reported olfactory dysfunction, whereas 60.8% had objective olfaction dysfunction on the SIT (score < 33). Black MLWH had significantly higher rates of subjective (Black 41.2% vs. White 35.3%) and objective (Black 73.5% vs. White 35.3%) olfactory dysfunction (X2(1)=9.22, p=.002). We found discordance between subjective and objective olfaction measures, with 29.4% of the sample having objective olfaction dysfunction and not recognizing it (FN). In comparison, 3.9% with self-rated olfaction impairment had normal objective olfaction scores (FP). Additionally, there was concordance in subjective self-reports compared with objective olfaction, with 35.3% correctly identifying normal olfaction (TN) and 31.4% correctly identifying olfactory dysfunction (TP). Those unaware of olfaction dysfunction (FN) reported using less scented products in daily life on the Smell Habits Questionnaire. Although the FN group had faster TMT scores, these findings were no longer significant after the removal of three outliers in the TP group (e.g., time to complete greater than 350 seconds).
Conclusions:
Our findings cohere with work in healthy older adults, traumatic brain injury, and Parkinson’s disease, documenting that subjective olfaction may inadequately capture the full range of a person’s olfactory status. We extend these findings to a sample of MLWH, in which discordance rates ranged from 35-61% for subjective and objective olfactory dysfunction. Unawareness of olfactory dysfunction in MLWH was associated with less daily smell habits and paradoxically faster TMT performance. A higher number of smell habits in the TP group indicate that more frequent odor exposure may increase sensitivity to olfactory declines. Future studies with larger samples will be helpful in understanding the full nature of these relationships. Lastly, given that one-third of the sample had discordance between subjective and objective olfaction, objective olfaction measures may be useful to consider in the neuropsychological assessment and standard clinical care for PLWH.
To analyse variations in the n-butanol threshold and odour identification scores of the Connecticut Chemosensory Clinical Research Centre test in various grades of olfactory dysfunction and in different nasal conditions leading to olfactory loss.
Method
Retrospective observational study.
Results
All grades of olfactory dysfunction were predominantly noted among males. In chronic rhinosinusitis, anosmia or severe hyposmia was seen in 87.5 per cent of patients without polyps in comparison with 68 per cent of patients with polyps. In addition, 90 per cent of patients with atrophic rhinitis and post-traumatic loss had anosmia, but only 30.7 per cent of patients with allergic rhinitis had anosmia. Pepper was the most affected smell for all the nasal diseases except atrophic rhinitis, in which asafoetida and baby powder smells were affected more.
Conclusion
In most inflammatory sinonasal conditions, odour identification is relatively preserved even when the threshold is maximally affected. In patients with comparable olfactory dysfunction based on the Connecticut Chemosensory Clinical Research Centre test score, a relatively preserved suprathreshold odour identification score may predict better prognosis.
The aim of this study was to identify the potential electrophysiological biomarkers of human responses by comparing the electroencephalogram brain wave changes towards lavender versus normal saline in a healthy human population.
Method
This study included a total of 44 participants without subjective olfactory disturbances. Lavender and normal saline were used as the olfactory stimulant and control. Electroencephalogram was recorded and power spectra were analysed by the spectral analysis for each alpha, beta, delta, theta and gamma bandwidth frequency upon exposure to lavender and normal saline independently.
Results
The oscillatory brain activities in response to the olfactory stimulant indicated that the lavender smell decreased the beta activity in the left frontal (F7 electrode) and central region (C3 electrode) with a reduction in the gamma activity in the right parietal region (P4 electrode) (p < 0.05).
Conclusion
Olfactory stimulants result in changes of electrical brain activities in different brain regions, as evidenced by the topographical brain map and spectra analysis of each brain wave.
There are instances when animals are introduced and expected to live alongside unfamiliar conspecifics within zoos, laboratories and wildlife sanctuaries. These pairings of unfamiliar animals may result in stress, trauma, or even death, in addition to reduced confidence in data resulting from these subjects. For species that communicate relatedness, sex, social status, and emotional state through olfactory cues (eg pheromones), one means of counteracting aggression may involve a period of partial separation — where animals are close enough to become acquainted — while a permeable barrier maintains separation. For our study, we evaluated the use of a novel, autoclavable, wire-mesh partition to separate potential aggressors. We tested different pairs of 24 wild-type male Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus), previously kept in social isolation for seven days. Each control pair were merged directly into one cage, while pairs from the experimental groups underwent three pre-exposure sessions that lasted two to four days. We used continuous video recordings to assess five common threat displays: lateral threat, keep down, upright posture, chase, and clinch attack. We used two types of bedding: new (unscented) bedding and recently used bedding that conveyed scents from both merged rats. We found that rats subjected to pre-exposure demonstrated lower aggression levels across three of the five metrics (lateral threats, upright postures, and keep downs). We conclude that permeable partitions show promise as a humane mechanism to mix new individuals into preexisting colonies. Further research may explore whether partitions could be helpful with other species that communicate social information by pheromones or direct visual inspection.
Recent work has demonstrated that human body odour alters with changing emotional state and that emotionally laden odours can affect the physiology and behaviour of people exposed to them. Here we review these discoveries, which we believe add to a growing recognition that the human sense of smell and its potential role in social interactions have been underappreciated. However, we also critically evaluate the current evidence, with a particular focus on methodology and the interpretation of emotional odour studies. We argue that while the evidence convincingly indicates that humans retain a capacity for olfactory communication of emotion, the extent to which this occurs in ordinary social interaction remains an open question. Future studies should place fewer restrictions on participant selection and lifestyle and adopt more realistic experimental designs. We also need to devote more consideration to underlying mechanisms and to recognise the constraints that these may place on effective communication. Finally, we outline some promising approaches to address these issues, and raise some broader theoretical questions that such approaches may help us to answer.
Smelling and other sensations that are often considered solely physiological phenomena are in fact deeply influenced by culture and history, and without understanding the ancient sensory landscape, our knowledge of the past inevitably remains limited. This paper explores the olfactory nuisances in one Pompeian city block (IX,3) and its immediate neighbors. I examine the area's stenches by tracing and mapping the sources of smells, focusing on those that in previous scholarship have been considered to render ancient towns foul smelling. The analysis contests the views of malodorous Roman urban space presented in previous studies and suggests that the smellscape of urban Pompeii was not a constant reek but milder and manageable. However, the analysis also reveals that social hierarchies and power relations played a part in Pompeian odor control, and the olfactory landscape was not the same for all inhabitants.
To assess the effectiveness of the nasal airflow inducing manoeuvre or ‘polite yawn’ technique in improving olfaction and quality of life in laryngectomised patients.
Methods
Using a prospective study design, 42 patients scheduled to undergo laryngectomy at a tertiary care centre were subjected to olfaction testing before surgery and two weeks following the surgery. The nasal airflow inducing manoeuvre was taught, and the olfaction test was repeated with the patient performing the nasal airflow inducing manoeuvre. Quality of life was assessed using the Appetite, Hunger and Sensory Perception questionnaire with calculation of scores after the patient had learnt the nasal airflow inducing manoeuvre.
Results
There was a significant reduction in the composite olfaction score, from a mean (standard deviation) baseline value of 4.01 (1.39) to 0.44 (0.51), two weeks after surgery (p < 0.001). After practising the nasal airflow inducing manoeuvre, the olfaction scores increased to 3.05 (1.32) (p < 0.001). Appetite, Hunger and Sensory Perception questionnaire scores ranged from 52 to 110 (normal range, 29–145), suggesting an improvement in the quality of life of patients.
Conclusion
The nasal airflow inducing manoeuvre, an inexpensive, simple, patient-friendly manoeuvre, can be used in the olfaction rehabilitation of patients undergoing laryngectomy.
Impaired olfaction may be a biomarker for early Lewy body disease, but its value in mild cognitive impairment with Lewy bodies (MCI-LB) is unknown. We compared olfaction in MCI-LB with MCI due to Alzheimer’s disease (MCI-AD) and healthy older adults. We hypothesized that olfactory function would be worse in probable MCI-LB than in both MCI-AD and healthy comparison subjects (HC).
Design:
Cross-sectional study assessing olfaction using Sniffin’ Sticks 16 (SS-16) in MCI-LB, MCI-AD, and HC with longitudinal follow-up. Differences were adjusted for age, and receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were used for discriminating MCI-LB from MCI-AD and HC.
Setting:
Participants were recruited from Memory Services in the North East of England.
Participants:
Thirty-eight probable MCI-LB, 33 MCI-AD, 19 possible MCI-LB, and 32HC.
Measurements:
Olfaction was assessed using SS-16 and a questionnaire.
Results:
Participants with probable MCI-LB had worse olfaction than both MCI-AD (age-adjusted mean difference (B) = 2.05, 95% CI: 0.62–3.49, p = 0.005) and HC (B = 3.96, 95% CI: 2.51–5.40, p < 0.001). The previously identified cutoff score for the SS-16 of ≤ 10 had 84% sensitivity for probable MCI-LB (95% CI: 69–94%), but 30% specificity versus MCI-AD. ROC analysis found a lower cutoff of ≤ 7 was better (63% sensitivity for MCI-LB, with 73% specificity vs MCI-AD and 97% vs HC). Asking about olfactory impairments was not useful in identifying them.
Conclusions:
MCI-LB had worse olfaction than MCI-AD and normal aging. A lower cutoff score of ≤ 7 is required when using SS-16 in such patients. Olfactory testing may have value in identifying early LB disease in memory services.
Piscine mast cells or eosinophilic granule cells (EGCs) of fish are equivalent to the mammalian mast cells. Recently, a better understanding of EGCs functions is allowed because of the growing interest in fish models. Herein, we present a trial to furnish data regarding the distribution of the EGCs in the fish olfactory organ, an issue that has not been reported so far. Regarding their distribution, two kinds of EGCs had been identified. An intra-epithelial one was detected in the olfactory epithelium lining of the olfactory lamellae. The stromal one was identified in the connective tissue core of the olfactory lamellae and in the lamina propria underlying the olfactory epithelium. Some were detected in the capillary lumen. The cytoplasm of the EGCs reacted strongly with the MMP-9 antibody. Stimulating a migration perspective for the olfactory EGCs which was confirmed by their location in the blood capillaries. Several EGCs could be verified in close relation, some underneath the epineurium, with the nerve fiber. Mutually, this verifies the existence of intra-epithelial and stromal migrating EGCs in the catfish olfactory organ and their inclusion in the olfactory immune response. Additionally, this provides evidence for an immune–nervous interaction to influence both the immune reactions and the nervous scheme in catfish.
Autism-spectrum disorders (ASD) are characterized by deficits in social domains, associated with abnormal socioemotional perception. Although olfaction provides access to socioemotional cues, little is known about the perception of emotional odors considering their social meaning in ASD.
Objectives
To investigate the subjective responses to emotional body odors (BOs) versus non-social, common odors (COs) in ASD.
Methods
Eleven ASD and 49 typically developed (TD) adults were asked to smell negative, positive, and neutral BOs (axillary sweat from healthy individuals exposed to fearful, happy, and neutral film-clips) and COs, and to rate each odor on perceived pleasantness, intensity, familiarity and arousal. Odors were presented for 5 sec. Analyses were performed with linear mixed-effect models with fixed factors (group × odor type × valence) and covariates (e.g., age; intensity for arousal/familiarity; familiarity for pleasantness). Post-hoc comparisons were Bonferroni-corrected.
Results
Odors were perceived as significantly more intense (p=.044) and pleasant (p<.001) in ASD than TD. Distinct response patterns were found in ASD and TD. First, positive BOs and COs were similarly arousing and pleasant in ASD (p>.05), but not in TD (p<.001). Second, positive and neutral COs were equally arousing, familiar and pleasant in ASD (p>.05), but not in TD (p<.001). No differences were observed between BOs in ASD and TD (p>.05).
Conclusions
ASD is associated with abnormal subjective responses to emotional odors, which could contribute to the social communication difficulties characterizing ASD. Since emotional BOs elicit psychological responses in others, analyses on subjective and automatic responses will allow a better understanding of the role of olfaction in ASD.
Ants appeared in the Jurassic and diversified into a multitude of new forms during the Cretaceous, around 100 million years ago. Today, ants are ecologically dominant in most terrestrial ecosystems. In tropical rain forest, ant biomass is four times greater than the biomass of all the vertebrates. The trait common to all ant species is sociality: they are all social insects that live in colonies. As in human societies, the size of ant societies varies enormously, from just a few individuals to tens of millions. Ants show extraordinary adaptations. They evolved the ability to build complex nest structures; they cultivate fungi for food and milk aphids, thus practicing agriculture and animal farming; they have nurseries and cemeteries, they cooperate. The key to their evolutionary success is efficient division labour in which the colony behaves as an organism. To achieve this remarkable social organization, ants rely on effective communication. Even though they use several different channels, such as the visual, acoustic and tactile, chemical communication is the most widespread way to exchange messages in an ant colony. Ants have developed multicomponent body odours, a myriad of exocrine glands and refined chemosensory abilities.
Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus-2 is a formidable virus, responsible for coronavirus disease 2019 and endowed with marked neurotropism. The damage it causes to the nervous system is manifold. The main neurological manifestation is anosmia. Olfactory damage is often transient, but there are no data reflecting an observational period of several months.
Objective
This study evaluated the trend of anosmia in patients affected by coronavirus disease 2019 in the eight months following diagnosis.
Methods
Fifty-five subjects who presented with symptoms suggestive of coronavirus disease 2019 and who developed anosmia, between the end of February and the beginning of March 2020, were investigated. The patients were interviewed after eight months to determine functional recovery and assess the degree of recovery.
Results
Ninety-one per cent of the population reported olfactory recovery and, of these, 53 per cent had total recovery after eight months. Females and younger age groups seem slightly advantaged in functional recovery. The elderly population appears to have excellent prospects for full functional recovery.
Conclusion
Anosmia represents a frequent neurological manifestation during coronavirus disease 2019. Fortunately, it is transient in most cases, and only a small percentage of patients affected by it report long-term functional deficits.
Chronic rhinosinusitis is associated with altered mucociliary clearance and olfaction. The study aimed to analyse the reversibility of impairment and endoscopic factors predicting changes in mucociliary clearance and olfactory parameters.
Methods
This prospective study included patients undergoing functional endoscopic sinus surgery for medically refractory chronic rhinosinusitis. Pre- and post-operative measurements of mucociliary clearance, olfactory thresholds, and identification scores were recorded.
Results
Of the 96 patients, 65.6 per cent had polyposis and 80.2 per cent underwent primary surgery. Improvements in mucociliary clearance and olfaction scores were seen in all patients, with greater reversibility of impairment in patients with polyposis and in those who underwent revision surgery. The presence of polyps correlated significantly with changes in mucociliary clearance and olfaction.
Conclusion
The study highlights improvements in mucociliary clearance, olfactory thresholds and identification scores after functional endoscopic sinus surgery in chronic rhinosinusitis with or without nasal polyposis, as well as for primary and revision surgeries. Adequate post-operative care and prevention of polyps recurrence help to improve mucociliary clearance and olfaction scores.
It is unclear whether olfactory deficits improve after remission in depressed patients. Therefore, we aimed to assess in drug-free patients the olfactory performance of patients with major depressive episodes (MDE) and its change after antidepressant treatment.
Methods
In the DEP-ARREST-CLIN study, 69 drug-free patients with a current MDE in the context of major depressive disorder (MDD) were assessed for their olfactory performances and depression severity, before and after 1 (M1) and 3 (M3) months of venlafaxine antidepressant treatment. They were compared to 32 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HCs). Olfaction was assessed with a psychophysical test, the Sniffin’ Sticks test (Threshold: T score; Discrimination: D score; Identification: I score; total score: T + D + I = TDI score) and Pleasantness (pleasantness score: p score; neutral score: N score; unpleasantness score: U score).
Results
As compared to HCs, depressed patients had lower TDI olfactory scores [mean (s.d.) 30.0(4.5) v. 33.3(4.2), p < 0.001], T scores [5.6(2.6) v. 7.4(2.6), p < 0.01], p scores [7.5(3.0) v. 9.8(2.8), p < 0.001)] and higher N scores [3.5(2.6) v. 2.1(1.8), p < 0.01]. T, p and N scores at baseline were independent from depression and anhedonia severity. After venlafaxine treatment, significant increases of T scores [M1: 7.0(2.6) and M3: 6.8(3.1), p < 0.01] and p scores [M1: 8.1(3.0) and M3: 8.4(3.3), p < 0.05] were evidenced, in remitters only (T: p < 0.01; P: p < 0.01). Olfaction improvement was mediated by depression improvement.
Conclusions
The olfactory signature of MDE is restored after venlafaxine treatment. This olfaction improvement is mediated by depression improvement.
The leaf skeletonizer, Eutectona machaeralis (Walker) (Lepidoptera: Crambidae), is a severe insect pest of teak trees (Tectona grandis L.f.) in China. To provide some basic evidence for future semiochemical-based management strategies of E. machaeralis, the morphology, ultrastructure, and distribution of antennal sensilla of adults were observed under scanning and transmission electron microscopy. The shape and structure of antenna were similar between males and females, both being filiform. However, the antennal length of males was significantly longer than that of females. Eight morphological sensilla types were observed in both sexes: Böhm's bristles, sensilla trichodea, sensilla basiconica, sensilla chaetica, sensilla styloconica, sensilla coeloconica, sensilla auricillica, and sensilla squamiformia. Significant sexual dimorphism of the sensilla dimensions was found, especially in sensillar length. The putative and potential functions of the different sensilla types are discussed based on the fine structures of the cuticular walls and dendrites of the different sensilla types. We expect these results to help lay a solid foundation for future functional research and develop further investigations of E. machaeralis.