Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5b777bbd6c-ks5gx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-06-19T09:20:22.893Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Creating a language-rich environment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 June 2025

Sheila Degotardi
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Sydney
Andi Salamon
Affiliation:
University of Canberra
Tina Stratigos
Affiliation:
University of Sydney
Get access

Summary

From the moment of birth, infants are immersed in a world of communication. Attentive adults look into their eyes, smile and coo at them, and use touch, eye contact and simple sentences to connect. In return, newborns respond to the human voices that they have been hearing in utero by looking towards the adult’s face and moving their bodies and faces. They also respond neurologically, with parts of their brains associated with auditory language processing activated by human speech more so than by other human sounds such as humming or non-distinct speech. Within the first six months, they not only use cries, coos and facial expressions to communicate feelings and needs, but also engage in rudimental back and forth exchanges with attending caregivers. The desire to connect with others through language and communication is indeed a very strong and uniquely human trait.

Type
Chapter
Information
Intentional Practice with Infants and Toddlers
Pedagogies for Learning, Development and Wellbeing
, pp. 167 - 199
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Book purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Anderson, D. R. & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. The American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764204271506CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Australian Government Department of Education [AGDE]. (2022). Belonging, Being and Becoming: The Early Years Learning Framework for Australia (V2.0). Australian Government Department of Education for the Ministerial Council.Google Scholar
Brand, N., Djonov, E. & Degotardi, S. (2022). Toddler-educator decontextualised talk and its value for toddlers’ learning. Early Years, 44(2), 370–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2022.2150836Google Scholar
Bratsch-Hines, M. E., Carr, R., Zgourou, E., Vernon-Feagans, L. & Willoughby, M. (2020). Infant and toddler child-care quality and stability in relation to proximal and distal academic and social outcomes. Child Development, 91(6), 1854–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13389CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center on the Developing Child. (2019). 5 steps for brain-building serve and return. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/5-steps-for-brain-building-serve-and-return/. Center on the Developing Child, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Davis, B. & Torr, J. (2015). Educators’ use of questioning as a pedagogical strategy in long day care nurseries. Early Years, 36(1), 97111. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2015.1087974CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2020). Why do so many children who hear two languages speak just a single language? Zeitschrift für Interkulturellen Fremdsprachenunterricht, 25(1).Google Scholar
Degotardi, S. (2017). Joint attention in infant-toddler early childhood programs: Its dynamics and potential for collaborative learning. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 18(4), 409–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/1463949117742786CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degotardi, S. (2021). The language environment of infant childcare: Issues of quantity, quality, participation and context. In Saracho, O. (Ed.), Contemporary research on child care in early childhood education (pp. 85107). Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Degotardi, S. & Han, F. (2020). Quality of educator-infant conversational interactions among infants experiencing varying quantity of linguistic input. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 28(5). https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2020.1817245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degotardi, S., Torr, J. & Han, F. (2018). Infant educators’ use of pedagogical questioning: Relationships with the context of interaction and educators’ qualifications. Early Education and Development, 29(8), 1004–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1499000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degotardi, S., Torr, J. & Nguyen, N. (2016). Infant-toddler educators’ language support practices during snacktime. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood Education, 41(1), 5262. https://doi.org/10.1177/183693911604100407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Djonov, E. (2019). Bilingualism and multilingualism in early childhood education (Australia). Bloomsbury Education and Childhood Studies. https://doi.org/10.5040/9781350996298.0010Google Scholar
Duff, F. J., Reen, G., Plunkett, K. & Nation, K. (2015). Do infant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 56(8), 848–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eadie, P., Bavin, E. L., Bretherton, L., Cook, F., Gold, L., Mensah, F., Wake, M. & Reilly, S. (2021). Predictors in infancy for language and academic outcomes at 11 years. Pediatrics, 147(2), 112. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-1712CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauvain, M. (1995). Thinking in niches: Sociocultural influences on cognitive development. Human Development, 38(1), 2545. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1159/000278297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Warren, S. F., Oller, D. K., Russo, R. & Vohr, B. (2018). Language experience in the second year of life and language outcomes in late childhood. Pediatrics, 142(4). https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-4276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girolametto, L. & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with toddlers and preschoolers. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 33(2), 268–81. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2002/022)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleason, J. B., & Ratner, N. B. (2017). The development of language (9th ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2015). (Baby)Talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(5), 339–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415595345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hoff, E., Rowe, M. L., Tamis LeMonda, C. S. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Language matters: Denying the existence of the 30-million-word gap has serious consequences. Child Development, 90(1), 985–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. (2018). Time to talk: Implementing outstanding practice in speech, language and communication (2nd ed.). Critical Publishing.Google Scholar
Hackett, A., MacLure, M. & McMahon, S. (2021). Reconceptualising early language development: Matter, sensation and the more-than-human. Discourse: Studies in the cultural politics of education, 42(6), 913–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/01596306.2020.1767350Google Scholar
Halliday, M. (1975). Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language. Edward Arnold.10.1016/B978-0-12-443701-2.50025-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1993). Towards a language-based theory of learning. Lingustics and Education, 5(2), 93116. https://doi.org/10.1016/0898-5898(93)90026-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., Yust, P. K. S. & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–83. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (2018). “Languagizing” their world: Why talking, reading and singing are so important. Zero to Three, 38(3), 1218.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1994). Influences of mother and child on maternal talkativeness. Discourse Processes, 18(1), 105–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539409544886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hohm, E., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Schmidt, M. H. & Laucht, M. (2007). Language development at ten months: Predictive of language outcome and school achievement ten years later? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 149–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-006-0567-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, J., Torr, J., Degotardi, S. & Han, F. (2019). Educators’ use of commanding language to direct infants’ behaviour: Relationship to educators’ qualifications and implications for language learning opportunities. Early Years, 39(2), 190204. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2017.1368008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A. & Ferauld, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), 31–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00768.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J. & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalashnikova, M. & Burnham, D. (2018). Infant-directed speech from seven to nineteen months has similar acoustic properties but different functions. Journal of Child Language, 45(5), 1035–53. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000917000629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Brown, C. A., Lee, C. T., Cowperthwaite, B., Stutzman, S. S., Swansburg, M. L., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H. H., Zhang, K. & Wang, Z. (2009). Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 5971. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klette, T., Drugli, M. B. & Aandahl, A. M. (2018). Together and alone a study of interactions between toddlers and childcare providers during mealtime in Norwegian childcare centres. Early Child Development and Care, 188(3), 387–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1220943CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langeloo, A., Mascareño Lara, M., Deunk, M. I., Klitzing, N. F. & Strijbos, J.-W. (2019). A systematic review of teacher–child interactions with multilingual young children. Review of Educational Research, 89(4), 536–68. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654319855619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, W., Farkas, G., Duncan, G., Burchinal, M. R. & Vandell, D. (2013). Timing of high quality child care and cognitive, language, and preacademic development. Developmental Psychology, 49(8), 1440–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L. & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant’s role in mother-infant communication. Journal of Child Language, 13, 1529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive and language development. Child Development, 71, 960–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00202Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Awander, A., Spada, D., Baldoli, C., Poloniato, A., Lohmann, G. & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Neural language networks at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences – PNAS, 108(38), 16056–61. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1102991108Google ScholarPubMed
Rochat, P., Querido, J. G. & Striano, T. (1999). Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 950–7. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romberg, A. R. & Saffran, J. R. (2010). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Cognitive Science, 1(6), 906–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.78Google ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–74. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. & Snow, C. E. (2020). Analyzing input quality along three dimensions: Interactive, linguistic, and conceptual. Journal of Child Language, 47, 57. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M. (2007). On the development of the field of language development. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 115). Blackwell Publishing Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757833.ch1Google Scholar
Siraj-Blatchford, I. & Manni, L. (2008). ‘Would you like to tidy up now?’ An analysis of adult questioning in the English Foundation Stage. Journal of International Research and Development, 28(1), 522. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575140701842213Google Scholar
Smith, J., Eadie, T., Levickis, P., Bretherton, L. & Goldfeld, S. (2018). Predictive validity of verbal and non-verbal communication and mother–child turn-taking at 12 months on language outcomes at 24 and 36 months in a cohort of infants experiencing adversity: A preliminary study. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 53(5), 969–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C., Pan, A., Imbens-Bailey, A. & Herman, J. (1996). Learning to say what one means: A longitudinal study of children’s speech act use. Social Development, 5(1), 5684. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1996.tb00072.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1984). Parent-child interaction and the development of communicative ability. In Schiefelbusch, R. & Picker, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of communicative competence (pp. 69108). University Park Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (2017). The role of vocabulary versus knowledge in children’s language learning: A fifty year perspective. Journal for the Study of Education and Development, 40(1), 118. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1080/02103702.2016.1263449Google Scholar
Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27, 501–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soderstrom, M. & Wittebolle, K. (2013). When do caregivers talk? The influence of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments. PLoS ONE, 8(11). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taanila, A., Murray, G. K., Jokelainen, J., Isohanni, M. & Rantakallio, P. (2007). Infant developmental milestones: A 31-year follow-up. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47(9), 581–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.xGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Custode, S., Kuchirko, Y., Escobar, K. & Lo, T. (2019). Routine language: Speech directed to infants during home activities. Child Development, 90(6), 2135–52. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y. & Song, L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science: A journal of the American Psychological Society, 23(2), 121–6. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414522813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). Having intentions, understanding intentions, and understanding communicative intentions. In Zelazo, P. D., Astington, J. W. & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 6376). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Torr, J. (2020a). How ‘shared’ is shared reading: Book-focused infant-educator interactions in long day-care centres. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 20(4), 815–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798418792038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torr, J. (2020b). Shared reading as a practice for fostering early learning in an Early Childhood Education and Care centre: A naturalistic, comparative study of one infant’s experiences with two educators. Literacy, 54(3), 132–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/lit.12227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallotton, C. D. (2009). Do infants influence their quality of care? Infant communicative gestures predict caregivers’ responsiveness. Infant Behavior & Development, 32, 351–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandell, D., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Steinberg, L. & Vandergrift, N. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD study of child care and youth development. Child Development, 81(3), 737–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01431.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdon, S., McLeod, S. & Winsler, A. (2014). Language maintenance and loss in a population study of young Australian children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(2), 168–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A. & Fernauld, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, Z., Degotardi, S. & Djonov, E. (2021). Supporting multilingual development in early childhood education: A scoping review. International Journal of Educational Research, 110, 101894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2021.101894CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Dongxin, X., Gray, S. & Yapanel, U. (2009). Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics, 124, 342–9. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2267CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×