Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5b777bbd6c-f9nfp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-06-24T11:59:36.453Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Introduction

from I - Background

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2021

Mario L. Small
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Brea L. Perry
Affiliation:
Indiana University, Bloomington
Bernice Pescosolido
Affiliation:
Indiana University, Bloomington
Edward B. Smith
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois

Summary

Network analysis is ubiquitous. It has shaped how researchers and society as a whole understand issues as diverse as the spread of disease, the precursors of loneliness, the rise of protest movements, the causes of social inequality, the flows of air traffic, the rise of social media, and much more (McAdam and Paulsen 1993; Wasserman and Faust 1994; Watts and Strogatz 1998; Watts 1999; Barabási 2002; Christakis and Fowler 2009; Wellman et al. 2021).1 This influence is due to the remarkable flexibility and power of network analysis. A network is simply a set of nodes and the ties between them, and a node can be anything – an individual, an organization, a website, a computer server, an airport, a nation, or any entity with the capacity to connect in any fashion to another entity. The ability to think of any relationship in network terms has proved remarkably generative for researchers.

Type
Chapter
Information
Personal Networks
Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis
, pp. 3 - 26
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Introduction

Network analysis is ubiquitous. It has shaped how researchers and society as a whole understand issues as diverse as the spread of disease, the precursors of loneliness, the rise of protest movements, the causes of social inequality, the flows of air traffic, the rise of social media, and much more (Reference McAdam and PaulsenMcAdam and Paulsen 1993; Reference Wasserman and FaustWasserman and Faust 1994; Reference Watts and StrogatzWatts and Strogatz 1998; Reference WattsWatts 1999; Reference BarabásiBarabási 2002; Reference Christakis and FowlerChristakis and Fowler 2009; Reference Wellman, Hampton, Quan-Haase, Harper, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithWellman et al. 2021).Footnote 1 This influence is due to the remarkable flexibility and power of network analysis. A network is simply a set of nodes and the ties between them, and a node can be anything – an individual, an organization, a website, a computer server, an airport, a nation, or any entity with the capacity to connect in any fashion to another entity. The ability to think of any relationship in network terms has proved remarkably generative for researchers.

Social scientists have traditionally conducted network studies in one of two ways (see Reference Wasserman and FaustWasserman and Faust 1994; Reference KadushinKadushin 2012; Reference Borgatti, Everett and JohnsonBorgatti, Everett, and Johnson 2013; Reference Crossley, Bellotti, Gemma Edwards, Everett and TranmerCrossley et al. 2015; Reference Perry, Pescosolido and BorgattiPerry, Pescosolido, and Borgatti 2018; Reference McCarty, Lubbers, Vacca and MolinaMcCarty et al. 2019). Those adopting a sociocentric perspective examine a set of actors and all realized or potential ties between them; those adopting an egocentric one, a set of actors and their relations of any type that are of interest to the researcher. Sociocentric researchers know how everyone in their study is connected to everyone else. For example, they might study all students in a given high school, all board members in the Fortune 500, or even all users of a social media site, and thus know the entirety of connections within the chosen context. However, they will not know what connections any of those people have outside of, respectively, that high school, set of board members, or social media site. Egocentric researchers know how everyone in their study is connected to anyone (with a given role or type of connection) in their personal network universe – but not their connections to others in the study. They might study a representative sample of 3,000 Americans or a set of sixty undocumented immigrants from various countries residing throughout Spain, and thus know the Americans’ or immigrants’ connections to their confidants, co-workers, dearest friends, or anyone else specified by the researcher. However, the researcher will likely not know whether those 3,000 persons or sixty individuals are connected to one another.

The power of sociocentric analysis comes from capturing the totality of social structure. In a high school, a researcher can understand, among other things, the total number of cliques in the school, the degree to which particular students connect cliques not otherwise connected, and the extent to which popular students are tied to other popular ones. In a board member network, the researcher can capture the extent to which companies in different industries are nonetheless connected, the degree to which any two members in the entire network are exchangeable without altering the structure of the interlock, and the power of particular members as a function of the connectedness of the boards they have joined. In a social media network, the researcher can determine the level of a person’s influence, the speed with which an idea spreads, and the probability that any two persons will have a friend in common. This power does come at a cost. Logistically, the researcher would ideally need to have data on every person or node in the network.Footnote 2 But more fundamentally, the researcher is forced to assume that any persons outside that network – any students in a different school, or board members in slightly smaller companies, or friends and family who use a different social media platform – do not affect relations (or their consequences) within the observed network (see Reference Laumann, Marsden, Prensky, Freeman, White and RomneyLaumann, Marsden, and Prensky 1989; Reference Perry and RothPerry and Roth 2021). Across the majority of social relations, that assumption is implausible.

The power of egocentric analysis comes from capturing the entire set of people’s personal connections of any type. In a sample of 3,000 Americans, a researcher can understand, among other things, how many friends or confidants Americans have, how connected those friends are to one another, whether any given friend plays more than one role, whether different Americans have personal networks of different structure or composition, and the relationship between Americans’ networks and their income, education, political beliefs, health, or any other personal characteristic or condition. In a study of sixty immigrants in Spain, a researcher can understand, for example, whether connections in the country of origin differ in number, quality, or structure from those in Spain, whether immigrants turn to different kinds of people for different kinds of needs, and whether those from Latin America use their national or international connections differently from those from Northern Africa, among many other questions. This power has its own costs. Logistically, to fully understand the structure and composition of the networks of those studied, the researcher must collect a great deal of data from them, and since questions must be asked about relations between every pair of persons named, for every additional name the time required for data collection expands rapidly. More fundamentally, the researcher can only capture the totality of the network structure surrounding individuals, rather than the broader set of connections – including those between friends and friends of friends – that the individual may be unaware of. This is, in part, why egocentric analysis is known as the study of personal networks.

While sociocentric and egocentric perspectives can differ dramatically, in practice researchers do collect both kinds of data and adopt both analytic approaches. For example, a researcher may study all students in a school, ask about their connections to other students, and also ask about their connections to anyone else (Harris et al. 2009; Reference SmallSmall 2017). But each perspective inevitably asks different kinds of questions and can call for distinct analytical techniques. Sociocentric research has exploded in recent decades; it is the perspective that has most directly informed network science in other disciplines (e.g., Reference BarabásiBarabási 2002).

This volume is dedicated to understanding the history, present, and future of egocentric or personal network analysis. Egocentric analysis conceives of each individual, or ego, as embedded in a personal community of alters, a community partially of ego’s creation and nearly unique to them, whose composition and structure have consequences. The strengths of this approach are well suited for many of the important questions in mainstream social science. Particularly influential in research on social inequality, social networks have provided powerful insights about the precursors of upward mobility, educational achievement, social support, health inequities, poverty, immigration, and social movements, to name a few (e.g., see Reference Fernandez and WeinbergFernandez and Weinberg 1997; Reference Fernandez and Fernandez-Mateo.Fernandez and Fernandez-Mateo 2006; Reference SmithSmith 2007; Reference SmallSmall 2009b, Reference Small2017, Reference Small, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and Smith2021; Reference EricksonErickson 2021; Reference Lubbers, Molina, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithLubbers and Molina 2021; Reference Smith, Sanders, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSmith and Sanders 2021; Reference Tindall, Stoddart, McLevey, Jasny, Fisher, Earl, Diani, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithTindall et al. 2021). Our goal in this volume is to take stock of these contributions – within and outside network analysis – which have been substantive, methodological, and theoretical in nature.

Before proceeding, we must address the role of theory in egocentric research. Social network analysis, unlike rational choice theory or structuralism, is not explicitly a theory of society. Rather, it is a perspective, or set of perspectives, on how to study social relations and their consequences for individuals and groups. Nonetheless, that statement must be qualified in two ways. First, network analysts have produced numerous theories, both descriptive and causal, about human behavior and social organization. While the theories have ranged widely, they have shared a relational orientation toward the world, the belief that ties, most often between individuals but also between organizations, cultural objects, and other social entities, represent an important indicator for understanding human nature and behavior (Reference Emirbayer and GoodwinEmirbayer and Goodwin 1994). Second, adopting that belief means, implicitly or explicitly, assuming something about the ontological status of relations, as opposed to individuals, groups, classes, or other entities. Most often, it means assuming that ties are ontologically real entities (but see, e.g., Reference Laumann, Marsden, Prensky, Freeman, White and RomneyLaumann et al. 1989). And that assumption, in turn, results in epistemological decisions about how best to study such relations. So, egocentric analysis, while not a theory per se, inevitably requires adopting theoretical positions on the nature of society and human behavior and on how to study them.

Many practitioners of network analysis today spend little time thinking about these matters – and, in fact, one need not think about them to conduct an effective empirical study of, say, how social networks affect upward mobility among working class Americans. But the methods, analytical habits, and even vocabulary of egocentric researchers today reflect many theoretical decisions made one or more generations ago by sociologists, anthropologists, and psychologists who, proposing new ways of understanding society, first grappled with such questions. Knowing something about that history is essential to understanding where egocentric research is today – and where it is headed.

A Selective History of Ego-Network Research

Early Notions

In many ways, the study of the relations between people and those they are connected to has been at the center of social thought from its beginnings. As Reference SmallSmall (2017: 11) has recently written, “Network-related thinking is as old as thinking itself. Aristotle proposed that the strength of ties among friends derives from their similarity and repeated interaction. Hume argued that people will tend to associate with those with similar characteristics. And Adam Smith proposed that friends made better confidants than acquaintances.” Nevertheless, the ideas that most directly informed egocentric research, and network research more generally as it is practiced today only began to be systematized in the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries, most notably by German sociologist Georg Simmel.

An unusually creative thinker, Simmel proposed a number of ideas that network researchers adopted in later years. In his Web of Group Affiliations, Reference SimmelSimmel (1964[1955]) proposed that society had evolved over the course of modernity: the reasons people associate with others had shifted from those imposed on individuals and based on “organic” criteria (such as age or kinship or village membership) to those entered into freely and based on “rational” interests (such as occupations, clubs, etc.).Footnote 3 These patterns of relations translated into different social structures in premodern and modern society. The premodern form is local and best described as a set of concentric circles, wherein the individual feels a strong sense of self but fears outsiders and has limited access to mobility and information. The modern form resembles a set of intersecting circles, wherein the individual belongs to multiple different associations with only partly overlapping memberships, producing a web of affiliations – in essence, a network – that affects trust, solidarity, personality, conflict and more (see Reference Blau and SchwartzBlau and Schwartz 1997; Reference Pescosolido and RubinPescosolido and Rubin 2000). This work provided an intellectual foundation for thinking of society as what is now known as a two-mode network, in which people were connected to one another by virtue of their membership in common associations (see Reference BreigerBreiger 1974). It also offered a way to think about how social relations, and people’s relations to their networks, have changed over the last century, including how social media has and has not altered personal networks (Reference Pescosolido and RubinPescosolido and Rubin 2000; Reference WellmanWellman 2018; see Reference Wellman, Hampton, Quan-Haase, Harper, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithWellman et al. 2021).

One of Simmel’s core beliefs was that the form and content of social relations could be separated, and that formalization in this sense was key to understanding society (Reference SimmelSimmel 1971). A simple but consequential way he applied this idea involved numbers. Simmel believed that the mere number of individuals involved in a relation, or interaction, affected its nature. Among other things, he argued that the interaction between two people (a dyad) would be qualitatively different from that among three (a triad) with respect to the operation of trust, conflict, intimacy, alliances, power, authority, and more. For example, the relationship between two conflicting parties is affected by the arrival of a third. And because the differences are structural, rather than substantive, they can be seen regardless of context, in relations as different as that of a child vis-à-vis husband and wife, or ancient Rome vis-à-vis Athens and Sparta. In this spirit, Simmel also wrote perceptively about the nature of secrets and strangers, arguing that the newcomers to a set of relations may find themselves hearing more than others, as they have no local allegiances to compromise their objectivity. His contributions would lay the foundations for dyadic and triadic analysis, and for the study of intimacy, secrecy, brokerage, and much more (e.g., Reference GranovetterGranovetter 1974; Reference Gould and FernandezGould and Fernandez 1989; Reference Wasserman and FaustWasserman and Faust 1994; Reference Burt, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and EdwardBurt 2021; Reference HollsteinHollstein 2021).

A less well-known feature of Simmel’s work (1997) is that he also anticipated future research on the relationship between social networks and physical space. He proposed that the physical distance between people affected their social relations, foreseeing future arguments about importance of propinquity in networks (Reference Festinger, Schachter and BackFestinger, Schachter, and Back 1950; Reference BlauBlau 1977; Reference ReagansReagans 2011; Reference Small and AdlerSmall and Alder 2019). He also argued that “fixed locations” in space, such as local organizations in neighborhoods, were important, because they anchored social activity toward a common point. He offered as an example the church: “This fixed point in space becomes a pivotal point for the relationships and the cohesion of the faithful, so that communal, rather than isolated, religious forces are developed” (Reference SimmelSimmel 1997: 147; see Reference Small and AdlerSmall and Adler 2019). This idea would reappear decades later in studies of how the focus of activity affects social networks (Reference FeldFeld 1981, Reference Feld1982).

While Simmel’s conceptual writing at the turn of the twentieth century laid the theoretical foundations for a great deal of subsequent research, much of the growth in ego-network analysis over the course of that century was produced by empirical researchers either answering substantive questions or grappling with methodological problems. Not all of these scholars dealt directly with Simmel’s ideas (which would have to wait until translations in the 1950s and a resurgence of network research in the 1970s to be taken on explicitly and credited properly). But the social scientists who conducted what we now recognize as ego-network analysis addressed equally important questions, at times echoing Simmel’s concerns with modernity, with the role of the individual in society, with group affiliation, and with the formalization of social relations. Moreover, subsequent generations of researchers often pushed for a much more systematic, evidence driven, and scientifically rigorous understanding of how personal networks matter. These survey researchers, ethnographers, and experimenters, working in sociology, anthropology, and psychology were at times unaware of research in other disciplines, but nonetheless set the foundation for today’s egocentric research.

Survey Researchers and Mathematical Sociologists

The idea of sampling individuals and giving them questionnaires to understand something about society as a whole blossomed over the course of the twentieth century, and was instrumental to the growth of egocentric research. Among the most important innovators in survey design was Paul Lazarsfeld, a Columbia University mathematical sociologist who over a series of studies on voting behavior and cultural taste in the 1940s and 1950s began to examine how individuals were influenced by others (Reference Lazarsfeld, Berelson and GaudetLazarsfeld, Berelson, and Gaudet 1944[1968]; Reference Katz and LazarsfeldKatz and Lazarsfeld 1955; Reference Centola, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithCentola 2021). Probably his most important study for egocentric analysis was Reference Katz and LazarsfeldKatz and Lazarsfeld (1955), which attempted to understand how the political opinions of residents of Decatur, IL were affected by social influences. Rather than asking respondents in general terms whether they tended to trust others’ views, the authors asked respondents to name those who had an influence on their opinions: “Do you know anyone around here who keeps up with the news and whom you can trust to let you know what is really going on?” (1955: 140). This kind of question, which later became known as a “name generator,” was a crucial innovation, as it allowed the authors to know exactly who had been influential.

This approach also allowed them to collect precise data on those individuals who had been named. The authors asked about the characteristics of each named person, such as whether they were neighbors, friends, or work colleagues. Additional questions of this kind later came to be known as “name interpreters.” The resulting data allowed the authors to paint a kind of picture of the social circle that affected how individuals made decisions. While Lazarsfeld did not ask respondents for the connections among those they named – that insight would wait a few decades to arrive – his decision to elicit names and their characteristics became foundational to how the majority of ego network data are collected in surveys.

Lazarsfeld’s contributions were more than methodological. Working with Robert Merton, he developed and tested a set of propositions about the friendship formation process that centered around the idea that the process would encourage people to connect with others who shared their characteristics. In so doing, they coined the term “homophily,” or the tendency of people to associate with others who are similar to them (Reference Lazarsfeld and MertonLazarsfeld and Merton 1954; Reference McPherson, Smith-Lovin and CookMcPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook 2001; Reference McPherson, Smith-Lovin and RawlingsMcPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Rawlings 2021).

At Columbia, Lazarsfeld and Merton taught several future network researchers, including Charles Kadushin. Though Kadushin would produce a number of works in the sociocentric tradition, two would be important to later egocentric research. Based on data from 1,500 patients in psychiatric clinics in New York City, Reference KadushinKadushin (1969) found that the decision to seek a psychiatrist was a deeply networked process because there was a network of elites in the city with strong connections to a subset of the psychiatric field. Additionally, whether a person made the decision was affected by connections to that network and, more generally, the extent to which alters helped recognize their problems and reinforce the need for professional help. Recent work on mobilization echoes some of these ideas (Reference SmallSmall 2017; Reference PescosolidoPescosolido 2021). In addition, for a study of network formation, Kadushin tried to understand the psychological motives underpinning the development of different network structures. He argued that fundamental psychological needs matter, particularly safety and efficacy: “Safety corresponds to and is generated by networks of cohesion, while efficacy comes about from separation and corresponds to networks with structural holes and is typical of brokerage situations” (Reference KadushinKadushin 2002: 77).

Perhaps Lazarsfeld’s most influential student was James Coleman, a mathematical sociologist who would later become known for his work on educational inequality in the United States (1966), on social capital (1988), and on rational actor theory (1994). But beginning in the 1950s, he devoted himself to the development of mathematical sociology, and the role of formalization in personal networks formed a core part in that process (Reference ColemanColeman 1958, Reference Coleman1964).Footnote 4 In one of the first studies of diffusion, Reference Coleman, Katz and MenzelColeman, Katz, and Menzel (1957) examined how networks affected doctors’ adoption of new drugs. The authors interviewed nearly all the doctors in a particular specialty in a community, asked them name-generator questions about other doctors they consulted or socialized with, and then tracked, based on pharmacy records, the date at which they first prescribed a new drug. The authors found that popular doctors prescribed earlier, that well-integrated doctors in general prescribed more quickly, and that the rate of diffusion for integrated doctors followed a simple model in which social contagion – the influence of other doctors in a given doctor’s personal network – played a role.Footnote 5 Because the authors had data on nearly the entire population of doctors in that field, they could use ego-data collected from a standard survey to understand the whole network in a natural, unbounded community sociocentrically. While later researchers questioned whether what the authors observed was spread among physicians, or rather spread to physicians via marketing from drug companies, or a two-step diffusion process, the role of networks in medical innovation was clear, and the study remains a mainstay of understanding drug adoption (Reference Van den Bulte and LilienVan den Bulte and Lilien 2001; Reference Chami, Kontoleon, Bulte, Alan Fenwick, Kabatereine, Tukahebwa and DunneChami et al. 2017).

In the 1950s and 1960s, the number of surveys using name generators to study association or influence grew in number. An especially important one was Edward Laumann’s intervention in the 1966 Detroit Area Study (DAS), a representative study of residents of the city. Concerned with what he called “microstructure,” or the “pattern of social relations” surrounding an individual (Reference LaumannLaumann 1973: 4), Laumann believed that the structure of a person’s immediate friendship network was important to how a city was socially organized. The survey asked a name generator – "think of the three men who are your closest friends and whom you see most often[; they] can be relatives or non-relatives, as you wish” – and name interpreter questions that included age, occupation, education, religious background, and political orientation. Laumann then also asked whether each pair among three best friends were “good friends with one another” (1973: 264–268) – a move that made possible the characterization of the structure of this friendship network. For example, the researchers could examine the density of the network, as in a standard sociocentric study. Finally, the 1966 DAS was also probably the first to trace social networks out one step. That is, a subsample of respondents was asked for the address and contact information of the named friends, who were then contacted and asked about their closest friends, their characteristics, and the characteristics of the closest friends. This additional survey provided an opportunity to assess the reliability of the initial reports, which documented that people could be trusted to accurately report basic characteristics of their network ties.Footnote 6

Other ego-network researchers would build on this early survey work in multiple ways. Many of them were trained at Harvard University under the influence of Harrison White. A theoretical physicist turned sociologist, White wrote influential papers on blockmodeling and vacancy chains that are central to the sociocentric network analysis tradition, in addition to major works on network influences in markets, identity, social interactions, and control (Reference WhiteWhite 1970, Reference White2008[Reference White1992]; Reference Boorman and WhiteBoorman and White 1976; Reference White, Boorman and BreigerWhite, Boorman, and Breiger 1976). White’s students, who would include Nancy Howell Reference LeeLee (1969), Barry Reference WellmanWellman (1979), Mark Reference GranovetterGranovetter (1973), Ronald Reference BreigerBreiger (1974), Kathleen Reference CarleyCarley (1991), Scott Reference BoormanBoorman (1975), Peter Reference BearmanBearman (1991; Reference Bearman and ParigiBearman and Parigi 2004), Bonnie Reference EricksonErickson (2021), and others, would go on to make significant contributions to ego-network theory and methods (see Reference FreemanFreeman 2004: chapter 8; Reference EricksonErickson 2021; Reference MischeMische 2021; Reference Wellman, Hampton, Quan-Haase, Harper, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithWellman et al. 2021).

In the late sixties, Lee found over 100 women with unwanted pregnancies who had obtained an abortion at a time when they were not yet legal. She asked how they found out who would perform them, tracing the networks involved in doing so, and thus conducting one of the early modern studies of how people mobilize their networks when needed (Reference LeeLee 1969). Reference WellmanWellman (1979) employed an egocentric network perspective to answer a longstanding question in sociology – whether modernity had destroyed “community” (see Reference DurkheimDurkheim 1933[1893]; Reference WirthWirth 1938; Reference ToenniesToennies 1957[1887]; Reference SimmelSimmel 1976[1903]). In a multi-faceted survey of residents of East York, Toronto in 1968, he asked respondents to report on their “intimates” or close friends and families, asking a battery of questions about where the alters lived and the kinds of support they provided. He found that modernity and urbanization had not destroyed community, but rather had “liberated” it from its physical confines, as people maintained intimate ties well beyond their local neighborhood (Reference Wellman and WortleyWellman and Wortley 1990; Reference Wellman, Hampton, Quan-Haase, Harper, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithWellman et al. 2021). He also found that the diversity of contemporary personal networks meant people turned to different intimates for different needs (Reference Wellman and WortleyWellman and Wortley 1990; Reference Wellman, Hampton, Quan-Haase, Harper, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithWellman et al. 2021). In 1969, Reference GranovetterGranovetter (1974) studied employed professionals in Newton, MA using both surveys and in-depth interviewing to examine the role of people’s network ties in the job search. He found that acquaintances were more important than close friends, a finding that helped inform one of the most influential papers in social science. “The Strength of Weak Ties” proposed that weak ties are more likely than strong ties to be bridges, and thus are better sources of new information on job availability (Reference GranovetterGranovetter 1973; Reference FernandezFernandez 2021).

In the late 1970s, another Harvard graduate, but not a student of White’s, Claude Reference FischerFischer (1982) conducted a large comprehensive survey of the personal communities of Northern Californians to address the impact of urbanization on social relations.Footnote 7 Importantly, his survey contained not one but multiple name generators, and thus could capture much more comprehensively than almost any survey before it the personal networks of a representative sample of respondents (Reference FischerFischer 1982; Reference FischerFischer 2021). He found that popular fears were largely unfounded; personal networks were maintained in robust though somewhat different ways in both small communities and cities. The East Yorker and Northern California Studies, and their recent restudies, remain, to this day, two of the foundational studies of community-based networks.

One of Fischer’s name generator questions turned out to be important in ways few might have predicted. He asked respondents whom they turned to when they had “personal matters” to discuss. That question captured the attention of Ronald Burt, a former student of Coleman’s and then colleague of Fischer’s at Berkeley, who contended that a nationally representative view of social networks was important and thought the General Social Survey could serve as a platform. Since asking a large number of name generator questions, as Fischer had, was cost prohibitive for the omnibus GSS, Burt analyzed the Northern California data and became convinced that the single most important question to ask was the following: “From time to time, most people discuss important matters with other people. Looking back over the last six months, who are the people with whom you discussed matters important to you?” (Reference BurtBurt 1984). To this day, and despite controversy, the question remains the most frequently used egocentric name generator (see Reference Bearman and ParigiBearman and Parigi 2004; Reference FischerFischer 2009, Reference Fischer2012; Reference Perry and PescosolidoPerry and Pescosolido 2010; Reference BrashearsBrashears 2011; Reference Smith, Menon and ThompsonSmith et al. 2012; Reference Paik and SanchagrinPaik and Sanchagrin 2013; Reference SmallSmall 2013, Reference Small2017).

Network analysis continued to explode in the 1970s and 1980s (Reference MischeMische 2021). Ronald Reference BreigerBreiger (1974), echoing Simmel, proposed influential techniques for studying the duality of persons and groups, the connections between people made by their membership in the same organizations, and between organizations made by the people who join them. Scott Reference FeldFeld (1981) proposed that “foci of activity” are important for the formation of social ties (Reference Feld, Knighton and AlecFeld et al. 2021). Reference BurtBurt (1995), a student of Coleman’s, examined the implications of Granovetter’s idea that only weak ties could be bridges. Burt developed the notion of structural holes – or unconnected spaces in a network between sets of connected nodes – and, particularly relevant for this volume, systematized and demonstrated the advantages that brokers gain from being positioned near those holes (see also Reference MarsdenMarsden 1983; Reference GouldGould 1989; Reference Gould and FernandezGould and Fernandez 1989; Reference Burt, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and EdwardBurt 2021).

Reference ColemanColeman (1988), writing three decades after his study of diffusion, would pen an influential paper both inside and outside of network analysis. Defining “social capital” as the set of resources – such as trust or information – contained in people’s networks, he proposed that social capital was as essential as human capital for upward mobility (Reference Volker, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithVolker 2021; also Reference Loury, Wallace and AnnetteLoury 1977, Reference PortesPortes 1998). A rational choice theorist deeply grounded in sociology, Coleman saw personal networks as incorporating the role of the social in how individuals make decisions. Nan Reference LinLin (2001), who had earlier used the idea of “social resources” to make a similar point, would go on to systematize social capital research, and introduce the important distinction between “access” and “mobilization” in social capital, or between the advantages one has, passively, by virtue of one’s network and those one has to act upon to realize (Lin and Bian, 2021). Also centered on people’s decisions, Bernice Reference PescosolidoPescosolido (1992), echoing Mayer, Boswell, and the “action set” theorists, would develop a model of how people mobilize their networks for support in which existing social structure played a role, but activation at the time of event or crises was equally important (Reference PescosolidoPescosolido 2021).

Ethnographers and Anthropologists

Much of the work of survey researchers was directly shaped by that of ethnographers who, beginning in the 1940s and 1950s, had observed social interaction in villages, neighborhoods, and cities and come to see the benefits of understanding social relations from a network perspective. In sociology, a classic ethnography of the 1940s had quietly relied on network thinking. William Foote Reference WhyteWhyte’s (1943) study of Italian American street corner men in a Boston neighborhood examined the status hierarchy inherent in the structure of relations he observed among the men. Whyte used sociograms – pictures of the networks as individuals with lines connecting them – to depict graphically the relationships among them, proposing that the structure of relations affected the allegiances and orientations of the group. Whyte’s work influenced several generations of ethnographers (e.g., Reference HerbertGans 1962) who would themselves go on to shape survey researchers’ thinking on egocentric networks (see extended discussion of Gans’ 1962 work in Reference GranovetterGranovetter 1973).

In anthropology, networks in one or another way had long been part of the researcher’s toolkit. Levi-Strauss, of the structural tradition, had proposed that the cultural rituals and practices observed in the field reflected an underlying structure of relationships and at times represented exchange systems or kin relations using directed graphs (Reference Levi-StraussLevi-Strauss 1969[194]; e.g., pp. 300, 377). Indeed, Alfred Reference Radcliffe-BrownRadcliffe-Brown (1952), an enormously influential figure in the first half of the twentieth century, explicitly described social structure as “a network of actually existing social relationships” (1952: 190).

But anthropology’s greatest direct influence on modern network analysis came from what became known as the Manchester School. Max Gluckman, a South African structural anthropologist and founding chair of the University of Manchester’s Social Anthropology Department in 1949, assembled a group that would become known for their sophisticated research on social networks. Many, forth like Gluckman himself, were Africanists of the southern or central regions of the continent. In a study of legal and interpersonal relations among the Lozi in Barotseland, now Zambia, Gluckman found that many relationships between individuals served multiple different interests or roles; he termed the phenomenon “multiplexity,” which became staple in the modern study of social ties (Reference GluckmanGluckman 1955: 19; see Reference VerbruggeVerbrugge 1979: 1287).

Gluckman’s student, John A. Reference BarnesBarnes (1954) was probably the first modern network anthropological theorist. Based on fieldwork in the 1950s in Bremnes, a Norwegian fishing town of 4,600, Barnes proposed that social relations could be understood as a “social field,” or what he called a “network.” His conception of the network, like that of future egocentric researchers, began with the individual: “Each person is, as it were, in touch with a number of other people, some of whom are directly in touch with each other and some of whom are not. Similarly, each person has a number of friends, and these friends have their own friends; some of any one person’s friends know each other, others do not. I find it convenient to talk of a social field of this kind as a network” (1954: 43). He explained how to visualize this network: “The image I have is of a set of points some of which are joined by lines. The points of the image are people, or sometimes groups, and the lines indicate which people interact with each other” (1954: 43).

Over the 1960s, many other anthropologists would follow (Reference MitchellMitchell 1969; Reference Boissevain and MitchellBoissevain and Mitchell 1973). Elizabeth Reference BottBott (1955, Reference Bott1957) studied the personal networks of husbands and wives in a set of London families, and found that the conjugal roles were related to the structural characteristics of each spouse’s network, particularly in what she called the “connectedness,” or the density, of the network (see Reference Bidart, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithBidart 2021). Adrian Reference Mayer and BatonMayer (1966) made at least two important contributions. Adopting Barnes’ language, he proposed that researchers should distinguish much more clearly the two separate levels at which social relations can be observed: the “network” and the “set,” or what today we call the whole network and the egonetwork. In addition, based on research on political behavior he observed in Bhopal, India, he proposed that the “action set,” the people with whom ego interacts around a set of activities, provides an accurate picture about how social networks are used in practice. David M. Reference Boswell and Mitchell.Boswell (1969: 245) built on this idea. Studying how bereavement and burials were managed after death in Lusaka, the capital of now Zambia, he showed that the network that gets mobilized during a crisis cannot be fully observed beforehand; it can only be identified during the crisis itself (see Reference Small, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSmall 2021).

Perhaps no Manchester anthropologist was more important than J. Clyde Mitchell, who synthesized discoveries, compiled several collections of work, and charted the theoretical and methodological questions – qualitative and quantitative – that the field needed to answer (Reference Mitchell1969; Reference Boissevain and MitchellBoissevain and Mitchell 1973; Reference Borgatti, Halgin, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithBorgatti and Halgin 2021). For example, he showed anthropologists that networks can be presented equivalently in matrix form and graph form, made a case for distinguishing density from centralization, identified several of the most important characteristics of social ties (e.g., frequency and duration), and outlined several methodological and conceptual issues that network researchers continue to contend with (see Reference MitchellMitchell 1969). Mitchell’s idea of “anchorage” would be essential to crystallizing egocentric network analysis as a distinct perspective.Footnote 8 While a network can be observed from many perspectives, he noted that several fieldworkers had begun with the individual, tracing those connections out, anchoring the network in the person. “This has led to the specification of this type of network,” he explained, “as egocentered[,] though the term ‘personal network’ may be more acceptable” (1969: 13).Footnote 9

In the 1970s and 1980s, a different team of anthropologists and their collaborators would emerge as important. H. Russell Bernard, along with oceanographer Peter Killworth, produced a series of studies that examined the limits of memory, recall, and perception, and the impact of these limits on our ability to understand networks (Reference McCartyMcCarty 2021). They found repeatedly, and in different contexts, that when asked to recall whom they were in touch with or had come into contact with, respondents often could not do so with much accuracy (Reference Bernard, Killworth and SailerBernard, Killworth, and Sailer 1979; Reference Bernard, Killworth, Kronenfeld and SailerBernard et al. 1984). These works helped inform a later preoccupation with cognition that provided much more clarity on what people can and cannot be relied upon to tell researchers about their networks – and, more broadly, how cognition affects our network interactions (Reference KrackhardtKrackhardt 1990; papers by Reference Smith, Brands, Brashears and KleinbaumSmith et al. 2020 and Reference Sun, Brashears, Smith, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSun, Brashears, and Smith 2021).Footnote 10

Psychologists and Psychiatrists

While anthropologists were developing ideas essential to egocentric analysis, so were psychologists. Their work contributed to sociometric methods, cognition, diffusion, and memory, among other topics. While we cannot do the work justice here, a few contributions are especially notable (see Reference FreemanFreeman 2004 for a history of this work). Perhaps no one was more important to the early development of network analysis than Jacob L. Reference MorenoMoreno (1934), a Romanian psychiatrist, public intellectual, and prolific writer, who, in a pioneering study, mapped the relationships among individuals with directed graphs, laid out a lexicon for network language still used today, and coined the term sociometry. In 1937, Moreno founded Sociometry, a journal for the systematic analysis of social relations (it later became Social Psychology Quarterly). While Moreno was a creative thinker, many attribute the formal aspects of his work to Helen Jennings, a psychologist with the scientific training to conduct systematic data collection and analysis, who, among other things, helped collect the network data in a school for girls that was one of the foundations of Moreno’s major work (e.g., Reference MorenoMoreno 1934; Reference JenningsJennings 1943).Footnote 11

Beginning in the 1940s, a set of psychologists working in a different tradition became concerned with cognitive distress, and the extent to which individuals found themselves in balanced or imbalanced states. Fritz Reference HeiderHeider (1946), as Simmel had before him, believed that people and their attitudes were related. He proposed that two persons were in a balanced state if they had the same attitude – like or dislike – toward an object, and that “the state of imbalance will produce tension” (1946: 108) in the relation. Dorwin Reference Cartwright and HararyCartwright and Frank Harary (1956) generalized the idea, and defining it in graph theoretic terms, both clarified and formalized it even further. Theodore Reference NewcombNewcomb (1956) showed that the idea helped explain how people became friends with others, based on a naturalistic, longitudinal study of seventeen college students who agreed to be observed over their first year at the University of Michigan. The ideas of this group of psychologists would become a pillar of triadic analysis.

Finally, in a highly creative, multi-faceted study, with wide-ranging implications, Reference Festinger, Schachter and BackLeon Festinger, Stanley Schachter and Kurt Back (1950) examined how the configuration of space affected friendship formation and the diffusion of information, based on a study of World War II veterans who were taking up residence as students at MIT (Reference Martin and KwonMartin and Kwon 2021). Echoing Simmel’s point about the importance of physical propinquity, the authors found that the physical arrangement of housing units shaped friendship formation, with ties most likely among those whose units were closest. These friendships affected “the flow of information and opinions,” and the latter, in turn, contributed to conformity in the group (1950: 151). The work helped inspire large literatures on the role of space in the formation of social ties, on diffusion, and on group processes (Reference Small and AdlerSmall and Adler 2019; Reference Martin and KwonMartin and Kwon 2021).

Consolidation and Expansion

Research in sociology, anthropology, and psychology, only some of which we described above, helped set the foundation for a period of remarkable productivity, not only in egocentric research but also in network analysis more broadly, as the perspective became a formal field. By 1977, Wellman had founded the International Network for Social Network Analysis and a bulletin, Connections; by 1979, the journal Social Networks was established.

Beginning in the 1990s, there was a different kind of expansion. Natural and physical scientists began to turn to network analysis to understand a wide range of phenomena, from cellular organization to global communications. Some studies were methodological or theoretical in nature, developing models to understand network structure across multiple contexts; others were empirical, using large-scale datasets or computationally intensive methods to understand network phenomena (e.g., Reference Watts and StrogatzWatts and Strogatz 1998; Reference WattsWatts 1999; Reference BarabásiBarabási 2002, Reference Barabási2009; Reference NewmanNewman 2010). While social network analysis had made its way into the study of inequality, health, education, and poverty, this new “network science” became a major interest among natural scientists, computer scientists, engineers, and their supporting institutions, such as the National Science Foundation. By 2006, a National Research Council Committee had called “network science” “the study of network representations of physical, biological, and social phenomena leading to predictive models of these phenomena” (National Academies Press, 2006). Network science and the study of complex systems have expanded well outside and even independent of the social sciences (Reference Luke and StamatakisLuke and Stamatakis 2012). The impact of this emerging turn on egocentric research remains unknown (see Reference Pescosolido, Olafsdottir, Sporns, Perry, Meslin, Grubesic, Martin, Koehly, Pridemore, Vespignani, Foroud, Shekar and NealPescosolido et al. 2016 for more detail).

The Current Era

The first two decades of the twenty-first century also witnessed an explosion in egocentric analysis, as researchers pushed the boundaries of what had become traditional network analysis. Well versed in the study of social structure, the importance of effective survey sampling, the boundary specification problem, the value of linear algebra and graph theory, the multiple approaches to conceiving of tie relations, and the advantages and disadvantages of individual, dyadic, triadic, and full network analyses, they sought not only to adapt these perspectives to new kinds of questions in social inequality, but also to break free of the limits of the ideas they had inherited. They studied job finding, social support, health management, immigration, social media access, and more. They eschewed conformity to convention and pushed egocentric analysis in new directions (Reference Hampton and ChenHampton and Chen 2021; Reference PerryPerry 2021; Reference Shepherd, Garip, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithShepherd and Garip 2021; Reference Small, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSmall 2021; Reference Smith, Sanders, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSmith and Sanders 2021; Reference Sun, Brashears, Smith, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSun et al. 2021). Those directions can be summed up in five themes, which represent both a connection to tradition and an emerging agenda.

First is the contrast between constraint and agency. For years, the predominant concern of sociocentric network analysts, and some egocentric researchers, was to examine how network structure imposed constraints upon action, how what people were able to do was affected by their network position, by the structure of opportunity inherent in their relations, by the resources they had or did not have access to by virtue of their connections, and so on (e.g., Reference WhiteWhite 1971). Agency, particularly in egocentric analysis, did play a role in the works of several early researchers. For example, Reference LeeLee’s (1969) work on how people used their networks to find doctors willing to perform abortions, Reference PescosolidoPescosolido’s (1991) work on how people muddle through the act of using their networks to manage illness, and Reference LinLin’s (2001) work on the “mobilization” of social capital assumed that people at times needed to exercise agency to take advantage of their network position. But a new generation of scholars have taken this work further, conceiving of agency as decision-making and imagining the consequences of seeing the network of relations as following the actions of ego, rather than determining them, of seeing networks as something ego must decide to take advantage of, rather than an advantage passively received (Reference SmithSmith 2007; Reference MarinMarin 2012; Reference Perry and PescosolidoPerry and Pescosolido 2015; Reference SmallSmall 2017, Reference Small, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and Smith2021; Reference PescosolidoPescosolido 2021; see also Reference KadushinKadushin 1969; Reference Emirbayer and GoodwinEmirbayer and Goodwin 1994; Reference Bruch and FeinbergBruch and Feinberg 2017; Reference HuckfeldtHuckfeldt 2021; Reference Lin and BianLin and Bian 2021).

Second is the contrast between maps and events. The starting point of both sociocentric and egocentric network analysis has been mapping the network, capturing either the total network structure or the personal networks of individuals. From that point, the researcher could describe its structure, study its origins, or identify its consequences. Yet as early as the 1960s, a few researchers had proposed a different starting point – not the structure of the network but the event of calling it up. Reference Mayer and BatonMayer (1966) and Reference Boswell and Mitchell.Boswell (1969) made a case for the action set because the set of individuals involved in addressing one crisis event – say, a funeral – would not necessarily be the network involved in another. A similar idea emerged years later as Reference PescosolidoPescosolido (1992) and others proposed an event-focused approach to data collection, as the crisis situation would call for people who otherwise would not necessarily interact. Others found problems with the GSS’s core discussion name generator by asking people not whom they typically talked to but whom they had talked to the last time it had happened (Reference Bearman and ParigiBearman and Parigi 2004; Reference SmallSmall 2013; Reference BrashearsBrashears 2014). Similarly, Reference SmallSmall (2017) recently argued that, when people need support when facing a crisis or problem, beginning with that event, rather than the stable personal network, is likely to uncover the existence of many actors that the researcher had not thought of. Among several new egocentric researchers, the dictum that one must begin by mapping the network is less a universal principle than one tool in a varied toolbox, a tool that, like others, has important limitations (see Reference Small, Small, Perry, Pescosolido and SmithSmall 2021).

Third is the contrast between structure and context. As early as Simmel’s work, a foundation of network analysis has been a distinction between form and content, between the nodes and ties that constitute a network and the particular conditions of the context in which those relations manifest themselves. This foundation has been the reason network analysts feel comfortable comparing, say, friends Amy, Beth, and Cathy to countries Mexico, Canada, and the United States – a triad is a triad, and balance issues play a role regardless of the context. It is the reason the raw data reported by network analysts has traditionally been little more than adjacency matrices – the appendix of Reference Wasserman and FaustWasserman and Faust’s (1994) classic work contains page after page of 1s and 0s. The structure of nodes and ties are the data. But a new generation of scholars have leaned heavily into context, showing that the context of social interaction is essential to the operation of network processes. Context, to these scholars, has been spatial (Reference ChuaChua 2012; Reference Doreian and ContiDoreian and Conti 2012; Reference Spillane, Shirrell and SweetSpillane et al. 2017; Reference Small and AdlerSmall and Adler 2019; also Reference DoreianDoreian 1980), organizational (Reference SmallSmall 2009b; Reference MazelisMazelis 2017; Reference Stretesky, Defeyter, Long, Sattar and CrilleyStretesky et al. 2020), urban (Reference StackStack 1974; Reference SmithSmith 2007), cultural (Reference VaiseyVaisey 2009; Reference LizardoLizardo 2017), online (Reference Rainie and WellmanRainie and Wellman 2012; Hampton and Chen 2021), and more.

Fourth is the contrast between network perception and reality. Egocentric research has traditionally depended on asking people to report their connections. But as research by Reference Bernard, Killworth and SailerBernard, Killworth and Sailer (1979) has shown, such reports can be suspect, not only because people’s recollections are flawed but also because they are sensitive to the process of elicitation (see Reference SchwarzSchwarz 1999). Still, for many earlier researchers the questions were merely how to effectively capture the real network. A new generation of researchers have pushed this question further, asking whether traditional methods for eliciting egocentric networks might also provide insight into the cognitive and psychological processes by which people encode, store, and retrieve social information (Smith, Menon, and Thompson 2012; Reference BrashearsBrashears 2013; Sun, Brashears, and Smith, this volume). New research in this area has examined how these cognitive, or “micro,” processes impact people’s networking behavior and access to social capital (e.g., Reference Parkinson, Kleinbaum and WheatleyParkinson, Kleinbaum, and Wheatley 2018).

Fifth is the contrast between stasis and dynamics. Just as early sociocentric studies were limited, due to computational capacity, to networks with a few dozen nodes, so were sociocentric and egocentric studies limited to capturing static snapshots. Researchers had neither the resources to collect nor the capacity to analyze dynamic processes extensively. There were some exceptions – Reference Coleman, Katz and MenzelColeman, Katz, and Menzel (1957), for example, studied diffusion over time by cataloguing the date on which doctors prescribed a drug. But by and large the data and analysis constraints on empirical research meant that static perspectives were common. The growth in computational capacity and improvements in dynamic modeling have changed network research, giving rise to analyses that begin with the individual and model structural change based on actor-based processes (Reference SnijdersSnijders 1996). More recently, researchers have pushed even further, conceptualizing personal networks as inherently dynamic systems that are responsive to biographical changes and periods of crisis or elevated need in the lives of individuals (Reference Burt and MerluzziBurt and Merluzzi 2016; Reference PerryPerry 2021).

Still, the outline of ideas we have presented in these pages only scratches the surface of new work. The pieces that follow take seriously the work of the past, but are fully oriented toward the future. They show that the consolidation of network analysis in the last decades of the twentieth century and early ones of the twenty-first have paved the way for an increasingly creative, far-reaching, and powerful new wave of egocentric research.

Footnotes

1 With origins in the social sciences, network analysis and its approach to relations have spread to so many different disciplines that it has spawned its own field, “network science,” which is concerned with uncovering basic patterns in network structure and process across domains and contexts. The field now has PhD granting capacity (see, e.g., Northeastern University’s Network Science Institute) and at least one major new journal, Network Science.

2 For example, if the researcher is missing data on the ties of an actor or set of actors who are especially well-connected, the inferences about the structural characteristics of the network may be wrong. Researchers have not yet fully addressed the implications of missingness in sociocentric data.

3 The exact title and the date of this essay’s appearance is unknown. Typically, the date is listed as 1922, the date of Reinhart Bendix’ well-known translation. Yet, the essay appears as early as 1908 in Simmel’s treatise Soziologie. Reference Levine, Carter and GormanLevine, Carter, and Gorman (1976: 1114) argue that Bendix’s translation of the original title, “Die Kreuzung sozialer Kreise,” as “web of group affiliations” is inaccurate. They argue that the proper translation is the “Intersection of Social Circles.” Reference LawrenceLawrence (1976) translates the original German into “The Intersection of Social Spheres.” Yet, the literal translation is “The Crossroads of Social Circles,” and Simmel did not use “uberschneidung,” the German word for “intersection.”

4 Reference ColemanColeman (1964) wrote a massive volume that served as an introduction to mathematical sociology, and presented ideas not only in diffusion but also in the study of social structure, in both graphic and matrix form, using examples from his previous research on adolescent networks, among others. These works helped convince researchers at the time that studying the network structure of individuals could be done rigorously.

5 The authors concluded with a methodological note, proposing “a marriage between sociometric techniques and survey research,” which would allow the formalization of relations only studied previously in small group contexts (1957: 269).

6 Laumann innovated in other ways, and would use the survey data to propose smallest space analysis (a type of multidimensional scaling) for representing social structure, to develop more refined ways of measuring social distance, and to understand the role of networks in the diffusion of disease and health conditions; he would also expand on the implications of the boundary specification problem (Reference Laumann, Marsden, Prensky, Freeman, White and RomneyLaumann et al. 1989; Reference Laumann, Ellingson, Mahay, Paik and YoumLaumann et al. 2004; Reference Cornwell, Benjamin, Philip Schumm and GraberCornwell et al. 2009; Reference Perry and RothPerry and Roth 2021).

7 Fischer’s dissertation adviser was Lee Rainwater; Fischer did not take courses from White, either (Fischer, personal communication, July 13, 2020).

8 This work shaped how Laumann and others conceived of their survey research. Though Laumann, a Harvard sociologist trained by Talcott Parsons, had Harrison White on his dissertation committee, the Manchester anthropologists were arguably more important. He attributed the inspiration of his notion of the microstructure (1973 to Reference MitchellMitchell’s (1969)) “personal network” and Reference BarnesBarnes’s (1954) “first order zone.”

9 As a group, the network researchers of the Manchester School were also methodologists. They would work not only to formalize many of their ideas quantitatively but also to anticipate several of the problems network analysts would face for years, including how to specify the boundaries of a network (e.g., Reference Barnes and Mitchell.Barnes 1969; see Reference Laumann, Marsden, Prensky, Freeman, White and RomneyLaumann et al 1989; Perry and Roth 2021) and how to quantify a node’s centrality (e.g., Reference MitchellMitchell 1969; Reference Kapferer and Mitchell.Kapfrerer 1969). In addition, they would also contribute to ethnographic research methods, developing situational analysis and the extended case method (Reference EpsteinEpstein 1967; Reference Evens and HandelmanEvens and Handelman 2006; see also Reference BurawoyBurawoy 1998, 2009; Reference SmallSmall 2009a).

10 Other anthropologists would make important contributions in the sociocentric tradition. Especially notable is Douglas White (Reference White and HararyWhite and Harary 2001; Reference White and JohansenWhite and Johansen 2005), a complexity researcher and anthropologist who would innovate and increase communication across disciplines in multiple ways.

11 The authors also published, in Sociometry, perhaps the first statistical analyses of sociometric choices. For that paper, the authors asked Lazarsfeld to produce a mathematical model to account for the choice process.

References

Barabási, Albert-László. 2002. Linked: The New Science of Networks. New York: Perseus Books Group.Google Scholar
Barabási, Albert-László. 2009. “Scale-Free Networks: A Decade and Beyond.Science 325(5939): 412–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, John A. 1954. “Class and Committees in a Norwegian Island Parish.Human Relations 7(1): 3958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, John A. 1969. “Networks and Political Process,” pp. 5176 in Social Networks in Urban Situations: Analyses of Personal Relationships in Central African Towns, edited by Mitchell., J. C. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Bearman, Peter S. 1991. “Desertion as Localism: Army Unit Solidarity and Group Norms in the U.S. Civil War.Social Forces 70(2): 321–42.Google Scholar
Bearman, Peter, and Parigi, Paolo. 2004. “Cloning Headless Frogs and Other Important Matters: Conversation Topics and Network Structure.Social Forces 83(2): 535–57.Google Scholar
Bernard, H. Russell, Killworth, Peter D., and Sailer, Lee. 1979. “Informant Accuracy in Social Network Data IV: A Comparison of Clique-Level Structure in Behavioral and Cognitive Network Data.Social Networks 2(3): 191218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernard, H. Russell, Killworth, Peter, Kronenfeld, David, and Sailer, Lee. 1984. “The Problem of Informant Accuracy: The Validity of Retrospective Data.Annual Review of Anthropology 13: 495517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bidart, Claire. 2021. “Commentary on Bott’s ‘Family and Social Network.’” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Blau, Peter M. 1977. Inequality and Heterogeneity: A Primitive Theory of Social Structure. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Blau, Peter M., and Schwartz, Joseph. 1997. Crosscutting Social Circles: Testing a Macrostructural Theory of Intergroup Relations. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Boissevain, Jeremy, and Mitchell, Clyde. 1973. Network Analysis: Studies in Human Interaction. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Boorman, Scott A. 1975. “A Combinatorial Optimization Model for Transmission of Job Information Through Contact Networks.The Bell Journal of Economics 6(1): 216–49.Google Scholar
Boorman, Scott A., and White, Harrison C.. 1976. “Social Structure from Multiple Networks. II. Role Structures.American Journal of Sociology 81(6): 1384–446.Google Scholar
Borgatti, Stephen P., Everett, Martin G., and Johnson, Jeffrey C.. 2013. Analyzing Social Networks. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Borgatti, Stephen, and Halgin, Daniel S.. 2021. “On J. Clyde Mitchell’s ‘The Concept and Use of Social Networks.’” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boswell, David M. 1969. “Personal Crises and the Mobilization of the Social Network,” pp. 245–96, in Social Networks in Urban Situations: Analyses of Personal Relationships in Central African Towns, edited by Mitchell., J. C. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Bott, Elizabeth. 1955. “Urban Families: Conjugal Roles and Social Networks.Human Relations 8(4): 345–84.Google Scholar
Bott, Elizabeth. 1957. Family and Social Network. London: Tavistock Publications.Google Scholar
Brashears, Matthew E. 2011. “Small Networks and High Isolation? A Reexamination of American Discussion Networks.Social Networks 33(4): 331–41.Google Scholar
Brashears, Matthew E. 2013. “Humans Use Compression Heuristics to Improve the Recall of Social Networks.Scientific Reports 3(1): 1513.Google Scholar
Brashears, Matthew E. 2014. “‘Trivial’ Topics and Rich Ties: The Relationship Between Discussion Topic, Alter Role, and Resource Availability Using the ‘Important Matters’ Name Generator.Sociological Science 1: 493511.Google Scholar
Breiger, Ronald L. 1974. “The Duality of Persons and Groups.Social Forces 53(2): 181–90.Google Scholar
Bruch, Elizabeth, and Feinberg, Fred. 2017. “Decision-Making Processes in Social Contexts.Annual Review of Sociology 43: 207–27.Google Scholar
Burawoy, Michael. 1998. “The Extended Case Method.Sociological Theory 16(1): 433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burawoy, Michael. 2009. The Extended Case Method: Four Countries, Four Decades, Four Great Transformations, and One Theoretical Tradition. Oakland, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Burt, Ronald S. 1984. “Network Items and the General Social Survey.Social Networks 6(4): 293339.Google Scholar
Burt, Ronald S. 1995. Structural Holes: The Social Structure of Competition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Burt, Ronald S. 2021. “Structural Holes, Capstone, Cautions, and Enthusiasms.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice and Edward, B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Burt, Ronald S., and Merluzzi, Jennifer. 2016. “Network Oscillation.” Academy of Management Discoveries 2(4): 368–91.Google Scholar
Carley, Kathleen. 1991. “A Theory of Group Stability.American Sociological Review 56(3): 331–54.Google Scholar
Cartwright, Dorwin, and Harary, Frank. 1956. “Structural Balance: A Generalization of Heider’s Theory.Psychological Review 63(5): 277–93.Google Scholar
Chami, Goylette F., Kontoleon, Andreas A., Bulte, Erwin, Alan Fenwick, Narcis B. Kabatereine, Edridah M. Tukahebwa, and David W. Dunne, . 2017. “Diffusion of Treatment in Social Networks and Mass Drug Administration.Nature Communications 8(1): 1929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chua, Vincent. 2012. “Contacts and Context.Social Networks 34(4): 534–8.Google Scholar
Centola, Damon. 2021. “Influencers, Backfire Effects, and the Power of the Periphery.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Christakis, Nicholas A., and Fowler, James H.. 2009. Connected: The Surprising Power of Our Social Networks and How They Shape Our Lives. London: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Coleman, James S. 1958. “Relational Analysis: The Study of Social Organizations with Survey Methods.Human Organization 17(4): 2836.Google Scholar
Coleman, James S. 1964. Introduction to Mathematical Sociology. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Coleman, James S. 1966. Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, DC: National Center for Educational Statistics.Google Scholar
Coleman, James S. 1988. “Social Capital in the Creation of Human Capital.American Journal of Sociology 94: S95120.Google Scholar
Coleman, James S. 1994. Foundations of Social Theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Coleman, James, Katz, Elihu, and Menzel, Herbert. 1957. “The Diffusion of an Innovation Among Physicians.Sociometry 20(4): 253–70.Google Scholar
Cornwell, , Benjamin, L. Philip Schumm, Edward O. Laumann, and Graber, Jessica. 2009. “Social Networks in the NSHAP Study: Rationale, Measurement, and Preliminary Findings.The Journals of Gerontology: Series 64B(1): i4755.Google Scholar
Crossley, Nick, Bellotti, Elisa, Gemma Edwards, Martin G. Everett, Johan Koskinen, and Tranmer, Mark. 2015. Social Network Analysis for Ego-Nets: Social Network Analysis for Actor-Centred Networks. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Doreian, Patrick. 1980. “Linear Models with Spatially Distributed Data: Spatial Disturbances or Spatial Effects?Sociological Methods & Research 9(1): 2960.Google Scholar
Doreian, Patrick, and Conti, Norman. 2012. “Social Context, Spatial Structure and Social Network Structure.Social Networks 34(1): 3246.Google Scholar
Durkheim, Emile. 1933[1893]. Division of Labor in Society. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Emirbayer, Mustafa, and Goodwin, Jeff. 1994. “Network Analysis, Culture, and the Problem of Agency.American Journal of Sociology 99(6): 1411–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, Arnold L. 1967. The Craft of Social Anthropology. London and New York: Tavistock Publications.Google Scholar
Erickson, Bonnie. 2021. “The Problem of Culture Flows in Weak Ties.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evens, Terence M. S., and Handelman, Don. 2006. The Manchester School: Practice and Ethnographic Praxis in Anthropology. New York: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Feld, Scott L. 1981. “The Focused Organization of Social Ties.American Journal of Sociology 86(5): 1015–35.Google Scholar
Feld, Scott L. 1982. “Social Structural Determinants of Similarity among Associates.American Sociological Review 47(6): 797801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feld, Scott L., Knighton, Devin T., and Alec, McGail. 2021. “Reflections On ‘The Focused Organization of Social Ties’ and Its Implications for Bonding and Bridging.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernandez, Roberto M. 2021. “Strength in Weak Ties in the Labor Market: An Assessment of the State of Research.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernandez, Roberto M., and Fernandez-Mateo., Isabel 2006. “Networks, Race, and Hiring.American Sociological Review 71(1): 4271.Google Scholar
Fernandez, Roberto, and Weinberg, Nancy. 1997. “Sifting and Sorting: Personal Contacts and Hiring in a Retail Bank.American Sociological Review 62(6): 883902.Google Scholar
Festinger, Leon, Schachter, Stanley, and Back, Kurt. 1950. Social Pressures in Informal Groups: A Study of Human Factors in Housing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, Claude S. 1982. To Dwell Among Friends: Personal Networks in Town and City. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, Claude S. 2009. “The 2004 GSS Finding of Shrunken Social Networks: An Artifact?American Sociological Review 74(4): 657–69.Google Scholar
Fischer, Claude S. 2012. “The Loneliness Scare Is Back.” Available at: https://madeinamericathebook.wordpress.com/2012/04/24/the-loneliness-scare-is-back/ [last accessed February 14, 2021].Google Scholar
Fischer, Claude S. 2021. “From the Northern California Community Study, 1977–78, to UCNets, 2015–20.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, Linton. 2004. The Development of Social Network Analyis: A Study in the Sociology of Science. North Charleston, SC: BookSurge, LLC.Google Scholar
Gans, Herbert J. 1962. The Urban Villagers: Group and Class in the Life of Italian Americans. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Gluckman, Max. 1955. Custom and Conflict in Africa. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gould, Roger V. 1989. “Power and Social Structure in Community Elites.Social Forces 68(2): 531–52.Google Scholar
Gould, Roger V., and Fernandez, Roberto M.. 1989. “Structures of Mediation: A Formal Approach to Brokerage in Transaction Networks.Sociological Methodology 19: 89126.Google Scholar
Granovetter, Mark. 1973. “The Strength of Weak Ties.American Journal of Sociology 78(6): 1360–80.Google Scholar
Granovetter, Mark. 1974. Getting a Job: A Study of Contacts and Careers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hampton, Keith N., and Chen, Wenhong. 2021. “Studying Social Media from an Ego-Centric Perspective.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, , Kathleen M., Carolyn T. Halpern, Eric A. Whitsel, Jon M. Hussey, Joyce Tabor, Pamela P. Entzel, and J. Richard Udry. 2009. “The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health: Research Design [WWW document].” Available at: https://addhealth.cpc.unc.edu//documentation/study-design [last accessed February 14, 2021].Google Scholar
Heider, Fritz. 1946. “Attitudes and Cognitive Organization.The Journal of Psychology 21(1): 107–12.Google Scholar
Herbert, Gans. 1962. The Urban Villagers. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hollstein, Betina. 2021. “Georg Simmel’s Contribution to Social Network Research.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huckfeldt, Robert. 2021. “Individuals, Groups, and Networks: Implications for the Study and Practice of Democratic Politics.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jennings, Helen Hall. 1943. Leadership and Isolation: A Study of Personality in Interpersonal Relations. New York: Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
Kadushin, Charles. 1969. Why People Go to Psychiatrists. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Kadushin, Charles. 2002. “The Motivational Foundation of Social Networks.Social Networks 24(1): 7791.Google Scholar
Kadushin, Charles. 2012. Understanding Social Networks: Theories, Concepts, and Findings. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kapferer, Bruce. 1969. “Norms and the Manipulation of Relationships in a Work Context,” pp. 181244 in Social Networks in Urban Situations: Analyses of Personal Relationships in Central African Towns, edited by Mitchell., J. C. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Katz, Elihu, and Lazarsfeld, Paul. 1955. Personal Influence: The Part Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communications Glencoe, IL: Free Press.Google Scholar
Krackhardt, David. 1990. “Assessing the Political Landscape: Structure, Cognition, and Power in Organizations.Administrative Science Quarterly 35(2): 342–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laumann, Edward O. 1973, Bonds of Pluralism: The Form and Substance of Urban Social Networks. New York: J. Wiley.Google Scholar
Laumann, Edward O., Marsden, Peter V., and Prensky, David. 1989. “The Boundary Specification Problem in Network Analysis,” pp. 1834 in Research Methods in Social Network Analysis, edited by Freeman, Linton C., White, Douglas R., and Romney, Antone Kimball. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Laumann, Edward O., Ellingson, Stephen, Mahay, Jenna, Paik, Anthony, and Youm, Yoosik, eds. 2004. The Sexual Organization of the City. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, Peter A. 1976. Georg Simmel: Sociologist and European. Sunbury-on-Thames: Nelson.Google Scholar
Lazarsfeld, Paul, Berelson, Bernard, and Gaudet, Hazel. 1944[1968]. The People’s Choice: How the Voter Makes Up His Mind in a Presidential Campaign. Oxford: Duell, Sloan & Pearce.Google Scholar
Lazarsfeld, Paul F., and Merton, Robert K.. 1954. “Friendship as a Social Process: A Substantive and Methodological Analysis.Freedom and Control in Modern Society 18(1): 1866.Google Scholar
Lee, Nancy Howell. 1969. The Search for an Abortionist. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levi-Strauss, Claude. 1969[1949]. The Elementary Structures of Kinship. London: Eyre and Spottiswoode.Google Scholar
Levine, Donald N., Carter, Ellwood B., and Gorman, Eleanor Miller. 1976. “Simmel’s Influence on American Sociology.American Journal of Sociology 81(4): 813–45.Google Scholar
Lin, Nan. 2001. Social Capital: A Theory of Social Structure and Action. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lin, Nan, and Bian, Yanjie. 2021. “Social Capital: An Update.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lizardo, Omar. 2017. “Improving Cultural Analysis: Considering Personal Culture in Its Declarative and Nondeclarative Modes.American Sociological Review 82(1): 88115.Google Scholar
Loury, Glen. 1977. “A Dynamic Theory of Racial Income Differences,” pp. 153–88 in Women, Minorities, and Employment Discrimination, edited by Wallace, Phillis and Annette, LaMond. Lexington, MA: Heath.Google Scholar
Lubbers, Miranda J., and Molina, José Luis. 2021. “Personal Networks and Migration Trajectories.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luke, Douglas A., and Stamatakis, Katherine A.. 2012. “Systems Science Methods in Public Health: Dynamics, Networks, and Agents.Annual Review of Public Health 33(1): 357–76.Google Scholar
Marin, Alexandra. 2012. “Don’t Mention It: Why People Don’t Share Job Information, When They Do, and Why It Matters.Social Networks 34(2): 181–92.Google Scholar
Marsden, Peter. 1983. “Restricted Access in Networks and Models of Power.American Journal of Sociology 88(4): 686717.Google Scholar
Marsden, Peter V., Fakete, Maleah, and Baum, Derick. 2021. “Egocentric Network Studies within the General Social Survey: Measurement Methods, Substantive Findings, and Methodological Research.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, John Levi, and Kwon, Hyunku. 2021. “Festinger, Schachter, and Back’s Social Pressures in Informal Groups.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, Adrian. 1966. “The Significance of Quasi-Group in the Study of Complex Societies,” pp. 94122 in The Social Anthropology of Complex Societies, edited by Baton, Michael. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mazelis, Joan Maya. 2017. Surviving Poverty: Creating Sustainable Ties among the Poor. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
McAdam, Doug, and Paulsen, Ronnelle. 1993. “Specifying the Relationship between Social Ties and Activism.” American Journal of Sociology 99(3): 640–67.Google Scholar
McCarty, Christopher. 2021. “Implications of Informant Accuracy Research for Ego Networks.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCarty, Christopher, Lubbers, Miranda J., Vacca, Raffaele, and Molina, José Luis. 2019. Conducting Personal Network Research: A Practical Guide. Milton Keynes: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
McPherson, Miller, Smith-Lovin, Lynn, and Cook, James M.. 2001. “Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks.Annual Review of Sociology 27: 415–44.Google Scholar
McPherson, Miller, Smith-Lovin, Lynn, and Rawlings, Craig. 2021. “The Enormous Flock of Homophily Researchers: Assessing and Promoting a Research Agenda.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mische, Ann. 2021. “On Parachutes and Lion-Taming.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, James Clyde. 1969. Social Networks in Urban Situations: Analyses of Personal Relationships in Central African Towns. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Moreno, Jacob L. 1934. Who Shall Survive? A New Approach to the Problem of Human Interrelations. Washington, DC: Nervous and Mental Disease PubCo.Google Scholar
National Research Council. 2006. Network Science. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Newcomb, Theodore M. 1956. “The Prediction of Interpersonal Attraction.American Psychologist 11(11): 575–86.Google Scholar
Newman, Mark. 2010. Networks. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Paik, Anthony, and Sanchagrin, Kenneth. 2013. “Social Isolation in America: An Artifact.American Sociological Review 78(3): 339–60.Google Scholar
Parkinson, Carolyn, Kleinbaum, Adam M., and Wheatley, Thalia. 2018. “Similar Neural Responses Predict Friendship.Nature Communications 9(1): 332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, Brea L. 2021. “Personal Network Dynamics: Organizing Principles of Stability and Change from Complex Systems Theory.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perry, Brea L., and Pescosolido, Bernice A.. 2010. “Functional Specificity in Discussion Networks: The Influence of General and Problem-Specific Networks on Health Outcomes.Social Networks 32(4): 345–57.Google Scholar
Perry, Brea L., and Pescosolido, Bernice A.. 2015. “Social Network Activation: The Role of Health Discussion Partners in Recovery from Mental Illness.Social Science and Medicine 125: 116–28.Google Scholar
Perry, Brea L., Pescosolido, Bernice A., and Borgatti, Stephen P.. 2018. Egocentric Network Analysis: Foundations, Methods, and Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perry, Brea L., and Roth, Adam R.. 2021. “On the Boundary Specification Problem in Network Analysis: An Update and Extension to Personal Social Networks.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pescosolido, Bernice. 1991. “Illness Careers and Network Ties: A Conceptual Model of Utilization and Compliance.Advances in Medical Sociology 2: 161–84.Google Scholar
Pescosolido, Bernice. 1992. “Beyond Rational Choice: The Social Dynamics of How People Seek Help.American Journal of Sociology 97(4): 1096–138.Google Scholar
Pescosolido, Bernice. 2021. “Confronting How People Cope with Crisis: From the Social Organization Strategy Framework to the Network Episode Model to the Network Embedded Symbiome.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Mario L. Small, Brea Perry, Bernice Pescosolido, and Edward B. Smith. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pescosolido, Bernice A., and Rubin, Beth A.. 2000. “The Web of Group Affiliations Revisited: Social Life, Postmodernism, and Sociology.American Sociological Review 65(1): 5276.Google Scholar
Pescosolido, Bernice, Olafsdottir, Sigrun, Sporns, Olaf, Perry, Brea, Meslin, Eric, Grubesic, Tony, Martin, Jack, Koehly, Laura, Pridemore, William, Vespignani, Alessandro, Foroud, Tatiana, and Shekar, Anantha. 2016. “The Social Symbiome Framework: Linking Genes-to-Global Cultures in Public Health Using Network Science,” pp. 2548 in Handbook of Applied System Science, edited by Neal, Zachary. Abingdon: Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
Portes, Alejandro. 1998. “Social Capital: Its Origins and Applications in Modern Sociology.Annual Review of Sociology 24(1): 124.Google Scholar
Radcliffe-Brown, Alfred. 1952. Structure and Function in Primitive Society. London: Cohen and West.Google Scholar
Rainie, Harrison, and Wellman, Barry. 2012. Networked: The New Social Operating System. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reagans, Ray. 2011. “Close Encounters: Analyzing How Social Similarity and Propinquity Contribute to Strong Network Connections.Organization Science 22(4): 835–49.Google Scholar
Schwarz, Norbert. 1999. “Self-Reports: How the Questions Shape the Answers.American Psychologist 54(2): 93105.Google Scholar
Shepherd, Hana, and Garip, Filiz. 2021. “The Context of Network Inequality.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Simmel, Georg. 1955[1964]. Conflict and the Web of Group Affiliations. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Simmel, Georg. 1971. On Individuality and Social Forms: Selected Writings. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Simmel, Georg. 1976[1903]. The Metropolis and Mental Life: The Sociology of Georg Simmel. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Simmel, Georg. 1997. Simmel on Culture: Selected Writings. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Small, Mario Luis. 2009a. “‘How Many Cases Do I Need?’: On Science and the Logic of Case Selection in Field-Based Research.Ethnography 10(1): 538.Google Scholar
Small, Mario Luis. 2009b. Unanticipated Gains: Origins of Network Inequality in Everyday Life. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Small, Mario Luis. 2013. “Weak Ties and the Core Discussion Network: Why People Regularly Discuss Important Matters with Unimportant Alters.Social Networks 35(3): 470–83.Google Scholar
Small, Mario Luis. 2017. Someone to Talk To. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Small, Mario Luis. 2021. “How Actors Mobilize Their Networks in Practice.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Small, Mario L., and Adler, Laura. 2019. “The Role of Space in the Formation of Social Ties.Annual Review of Sociology 45(1): 111–32.Google Scholar
Smith, Edward B., Brands, Raina A., Brashears, Matthew E., and Kleinbaum, Adam M.. 2020. “Social Networks and Cognition.Annual Review of Sociology 46(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Edward B., Menon, Tanya, and Thompson, Leigh. 2012. “Status Differences in the Cognitive Activation of Social Networks.Organization Science 23(1): 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Sandra Susan. 2007. Lone Pursuit: Distrust and Defensive Individualism Among the Black Poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Smith, Sandra Susan, and Sanders, Jasmine M.. 2021. “Self-Verification, Trust, and Social Capital Mobilization.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snijders, Tom A. B. 1996. “Stochastic Actor‐Oriented Models for Network Change.The Journal of Mathematical Sociology 21(1–2): 149–72.Google Scholar
Spillane, James P., Shirrell, Matthew, and Sweet, Tracy M.. 2017. “The Elephant in the Schoolhouse: The Role of Propinquity in School Staff Interactions about Teaching.Sociology of Education 90(2): 149–71.Google Scholar
Stack, Carol B. 1974. All Our Kin: Strategies for Survival in a Black Community. Frisco, TX: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Stretesky, Paul B., Defeyter, Margaret Anne, Long, Michael A., Sattar, Zeibeda, and Crilley, Eilish. 2020. “Holiday Clubs as Community Organizations.The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 689(1): 129–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, Hui, Brashears, Matthew E., and Smith, Edward B.. 2021. “Network Representation Capacity: How Social Relationships Are Represented in the Human Mind.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tindall, David, Stoddart, Mark, McLevey, John, Jasny, Lorien, Fisher, Dana, Earl, Jennifer, and Diani, Mario. 2021. “The Opportunities and Challenges of Studying Social Movement Ego-Networks: Online and Offline.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Toennies, Ferdinand. 1957[1887]. “Community and Society.” Translated by C. P. Loomis. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University Press.Google Scholar
Vaisey, Stephen. 2009. “Motivation and Justification: A Dual‐Process Model of Culture in Action.American Journal of Sociology 114(6): 1675–715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bulte, Christophe, and Lilien, Gary L.. 2001. “Medical Innovation Revisited: Social Contagion versus Marketing Effort.American Journal of Sociology 106(5): 1409–35.Google Scholar
Verbrugge, Lois M. 1979. “Multiplexity in Adult Friendships.Social Forces 57(4): 1286–309.Google Scholar
Volker, Beate. 2021. “Three Decades of Research into Social Capital: Achievements, Blind Spots, and Future Directions.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wasserman, Stanley, and Faust, Katherine. 1994. Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Watts, Duncan J. 1999. Small Worlds: The Dynamics of Networks between Order and Randomness. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Watts, Duncan J., and Strogatz, Steven H.. 1998. “Collective Dynamics of ‘Small-World’ Networks.Nature 393(6684): 440–2.Google Scholar
Wellman, Barry. 1979. “The Community Question: The Intimate Networks of East Yorkers.American Journal of Sociology 84(5): 1201–31.Google Scholar
Wellman, Barry. 2018. Networks in the Global Village: Life in Contemporary Communities. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wellman, Barry, and Wortley, Scot. 1990. “Different Strokes from Different Folks: Community Ties and Social Support.American Journal of Sociology 96(3): 558–88.Google Scholar
Wellman, Barry, Hampton, Keith N., Quan-Haase, Anabel, and Harper, Molly-Gloria. 2021. “A Network Pilgrim’s Progress: Twenty-Six Realizations in Fifty-Five Years.” In Personal Networks: Classic Readings and New Directions in Egocentric Analysis, edited by Small, Mario L., Perry, Brea, Pescosolido, Bernice, and Smith, Edward B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
White, Douglas R., and Harary, Frank. 2001. “The Cohesiveness of Blocks in Social Networks: Node Connectivity and Conditional Density.Sociological Methodology 31(1): 305–59.Google Scholar
White, Douglas, and Johansen, Ulla. 2005. Network Analysis and Ethnographic Problems: Process Models of a Turkish Nomad Clan. Washington, DC: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
White, Harrison. 1970. Chains of Opportunity. System Models of Mobility in Organizations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
White, Harrison. 1971. “Multipliers, Vacancy Chains, and Filtering in Housing.Journal of the American Institute of Planners 37(2): 8894.Google Scholar
White, Harrison. 1992[2008]. Identity and Control: How Social Formations Emerge. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
White, Harrison C., Boorman, Scott A., and Breiger, Ronald L.. 1976. “Social Structure from Multiple Networks. I. Block Models of Roles and Positions.American Journal of Sociology 81(4): 730–80.Google Scholar
Whyte, William Foote. 1943. “Social Organization in the Slums.American Sociological Review 8(1): 34–9.Google Scholar
Wirth, Louis. 1938. “Urbanism as a Way of Life.American Journal of Sociology 44(1): 124.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×