Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T19:19:54.351Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 7 - Parental Coregulation of Child Emotions

from Part III - Influence of Parenting on Child Emotion Regulation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2024

Isabelle Roskam
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
James J. Gross
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Moïra Mikolajczak
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium

Summary

Emotional development can be described as the emergence of a child’s self-regulation out of coregulation by their caregivers, especially parents. This chapter highlights this transition, focusing on two interwoven facets of emotional development: the regulation of actions by emotions and the regulation of emotions by volitionally applied actions, called reflective emotion regulation. The significance of parental coregulation strategies is considered, specifically how they contribute to the differentiation of emotion qualities throughout ontogenesis as well as to a conceptual awareness of a child’s own elicited feelings and the inferred feelings of others, which is required for reflective emotion regulation. The chapter addresses core parental strategies such as context selection and modification, affect mirroring, modeling, reflective functioning and talking about emotions as well as levels of coregulation, and also presents a brief overview of the field. The chapter closes with a look at noteworthy issues for further research.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

Emotions can be conceived as systems that regulate actions in ways that serve a person’s concerns (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986). They establish, maintain, or disrupt individually significant relations between a person and the internal or external environment (Campos et al., Reference Campos, Camras, Lee, He and Campos2018). Understanding how emotions regulate a person’s actions in social contexts opens up a sophisticated approach to how emotions develop over the course of ontogenesis from biologically given precursor emotions to a repertoire of differentiated emotions that the developing child becomes increasingly aware of. This awareness of one’s own and others’ feelings can be seen as a prerequisite for a volitionally driven regulation of one’s own emotions. Parents play a pivotal role in this development, which can be conceptualized as a transition from comprehensive coregulation of an infant’s emotions and behaviors to increasing self-regulation of emotions and actions by the growing child. This chapter highlights these interwoven developmental trajectories and gives an overview of how parents contribute to them. The first section focuses on the development of emotions, and the second section on the development of a reflective form of emotion regulation. The third section provides an outlook on further research questions that follows from the described state-of-the-art.

7.1 Development of Emotions

7.1.1 Defining Emotions

Emotions are defined as a functional psychological system involving the interplay of several components that serve to initiate and regulate a person’s actions. These components appraise and regulate the relation between the person and their environment in the service of that person’s concerns (needs, motives, values, aims of life) (Campos et al., Reference Campos, Camras, Lee, He and Campos2018; Frijda, Reference Frijda1986). Emotions occur as episodes with a limited time span. They disappear (or change into another emotion) when an adaptive action changes the person’s relation to the environment in a way that affects the signaled concern (Shuman & Scherer, Reference Shuman, Scherer, Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia2014).

The set of components that an emotion must have in order to fulfill these functions is controversial (Shuman & Scherer, Reference Shuman, Scherer, Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia2014). However, most emotion theories, and especially so-called multicomponent theories of emotions (Campos et al., Reference Campos, Camras, Lee, He and Campos2018; Camras, Reference Camras2022; Frijda, Reference Frijda1986; Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski and Friedlmeier2006), agree on the following components: appraisal, action readiness and expression, body reactions, and feeling.

  1. 1. Through the appraisal component, the actual or imagined situation is appraised in terms of its significance for the person’s concerns (Moors et al., Reference Moors, Ellsworth, Scherer and Frijda2013). Initial appraisal processes are involuntary and not conscious. They need to be distinguished from any (subsequent) conscious reflection on and voluntary evaluation of possible elicitors of an emotion (Engelen et al., Reference Engelen, Markowitsch, Scheve, Röttger-Rössler, Stephan, Holodynski, Vandekerckhove, Röttger-Rössler and Markowitsch2009).

  2. 2. The component of action readiness and expression comprises the motor activities that initiate the appraised urge to establish, maintain, or change the person–environment relation during an emotion episode (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986); this indicates that the emotions are regulating the actions. Action readiness relates to an instrumental use of movements to change the person–environment relation directly, for example, the readiness to flee when a situation is appraised as threatening, whereas the expression part relates to a semiotic use of movements to change the person-environment relation indirectly by appealing to the interaction partner to bring about the desired change, for example, a passerby shows an expression of fear on their face as a dog approaches, such that the dog’s owner, upon noticing the expression, feels compelled to leash their dog, which, in turn, causes the passerby’s fear to subside.

  3. 3. Body reactions comprise an interplay of (peripheral) physiological processes (e.g. blood pressure, heartbeat, breathing) as well as psycho-endocrinological reactions (e.g. cortisol secretions) that prepare the body to deal with the elicited action readiness and expressions (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Berntson and Cacioppo2014). There is, nonetheless, some controversy over whether emotions always have to be accompanied by body reactions (Lewis, Reference Lewis2011).

  4. 4. Regardless, the feeling component can be defined as the internally experienced sensations of the ongoing body reactions, action readiness, and expressions from a first-person, present-tense perspective (Damasio, Reference Damasio1994; Price & Harmon-Jones, Reference Price and Harmon-Jones2015). A feeling works as an internal monitoring device by signaling that one should either act or stop acting (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986; Scherer, Reference Scherer, Manstead, Frijda and Fischer2004). Longstanding controversy exists over the significance of this component and, in particular, over how it relates to the other components (Russell, Reference Russell2014).

7.1.2 Trajectories of Emotional Development

The development of emotions has been conceptualized in very different ways ranging from evolutionary (e.g. Izard, Reference Izard2009) to functionalist (e.g. Campos et al., Reference Campos, Camras, Lee, He and Campos2018), and from dynamic-system theories (e.g. Camras, Reference Camras2022) to sociocultural theories (Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski, Friedlmeier and Valsiner2012). A closer look at the course of emotional development reveals at least three overarching developmental trajectories: the differentiation of emotion qualities, a shift from coregulation to self-regulation, and an increasing awareness of one’s own emotions. For all trajectories, parents play a pivotal role in how children manage these tasks.

7.1.2.1 Differentiation of Emotion Qualities

Development starts from a restricted set of so-called precursor emotions (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1996) and differentiates into a set of culturally reshaped emotions in children, adolescents, and adults. Beyond the ongoing discussions between functionalistic and discrete emotion theorists, who debate the emotions with which newborns are equipped, five emotional reaction patterns have been identified in newborns according to the coincidence of elicitors and expressions. These are distress, disgust, interest, endogenous pleasure, and fright. At the age of 2 to 3 years, about 15 emotion qualities are differentiated, according to the elicitors, inferred appraisal patterns, and perceivable expressions (Camras, Reference Camras2022; Holodynski & Seeger, Reference Holodynski and Seeger2019; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1996). These emotion qualities are joy, amusement, affection, distress, frustration and anger, fear, disgust, interest, surprise, jealousy, and embarrassment as well as the moral emotions of pride, shame, guilt, and compassion that enable children to consider the concerns of others in their own actions long before they are capable of metacognitive perspective taking.

7.1.2.2 From Coregulation to Self-Regulation by Emotions

This developmental trajectory deals with the question of whose actions are regulated by an elicited emotion. Emotional development can be described as a shift in the way emotions regulate subsequent actions during an emotion episode, namely from comprehensive coregulation by parents to self-regulation of actions by emotions in older children (Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski and Friedlmeier2006). As Sroufe (Reference Sroufe1996, p. 151) puts it, “In fact, the general course of emotional development may be described as movement from dyadic regulation to self-regulation of emotion. Moreover, dyadic regulation represents a prototype for self-regulation; the roots of individual differences in the self-regulation of emotion lie within the distinctive patterns of dyadic regulation.”

In infants, the main function of emotions is to regulate the actions of their parents by the expression of emotions. A cry expressing distress does not lead an infant to engage in any actions. Instead, it is directed toward influencing their parent’s mind and leads them to perform the necessary action to satisfy the infant’s need, which will stop the infant’s crying.

Alongside the aforementioned differentiation of emotions, children learn a variety of actions in their first years of life that enable them to autonomously satisfy some of their concerns and needs signaled via their emotions, such as fetching things for themselves, handling everyday objects and extending their range of mobility. As a consequence, by the age of 3, they can already carry out a number of need-serving actions by themselves, making them less dependent on vicarious regulation by their caregivers.

Children need to learn to address their emotions as signaled by expressions not only so that their caregivers respond, for example by comforting them when they are sad, but also so they can use their emotional feelings and expressions acquired during coregulation for their own self-regulation. They should start using their feelings as an appeal to the self to carry out the necessary actions alone, for example by comforting themselves when they are sad. This transition to self-regulation of actions by emotions is a very lengthy process extending across the entire preschool age and into the elementary school age (Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski and Friedlmeier2006).

7.1.2.3 From Unreflective to Reflective Emotions

Feeling an ongoing emotion from a first-person, present-tense perspective is not the same as being aware that one is currently feeling an emotion. For example, a person in a life-threatening situation may feel an overwhelming urge to run away. However, only during a short rest in a hiding place may the person become aware that what they are feeling is fear. Fogel (Reference Fogel2009) labels the former state of feelings as embodied self-awareness and the latter state of mediated feelings as conceptual self-awareness, because, in the latter scenario, the protagonist can label their feelings and can describe them with gestures and words. Lambie (Reference Lambie2009) also differentiates in a similar fashion between so-called unreflective and reflective emotions.

Conceptual self-awareness is not innate. It develops during early childhood and is a necessary prerequisite for becoming able to regulate one’s own emotions volitionally (Holodynski, Reference Holodynski, Gebauer, Holodynski, Koelsch and von Scheve2017; Holodynski & Seeger, Reference Holodynski and Seeger2019). One of the pivotal characteristics of emotions is that they are felt as involuntarily happening to the person and are accompanied by a strong urge to act (or to withdraw from acting) (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986). Controlling this (emotional) urge to act requires the ability to psychologically distance, such that one can acknowledge “I am currently in an emotional state and can thereby reflect on possible alternatives to the strong impulse to take action provided by the emotion I feel.” In their Affective Social Competence Model, Halberstadt and colleagues (Reference Halberstadt, Denham and Dunsmore2001) describe four levels of such an ability: (1) the protagonist becomes aware that they are feeling something like an emotion; (2) they can label the felt emotion and its elicitor; (3) they fully understand its significance in the given context and the possibility of regulating their feelings and/or expressions; and (4) they can choose emotion regulation strategies for regulating their emotion appropriately. An even more highly developed ability is the ability to anticipate which emotion may be elicited in a forthcoming situation in order to decide how to regulate the emotion in advance, which is much easier than regulating an already elicited emotion. Gross and Thompson (Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007) label this type of regulation as situation selection and modification.

Becoming aware of one’s feelings and labeling them is a challenging task (Gebauer, Reference Gebauer2012; Lambie, Reference Lambie2009). At first glance, it seems similar to perceiving and labeling a property of an object. For example, one can say, “this dog is black” because of its evident color or “this person is happy” because of the smile on their face. However, this kind of labeling does not apply to the subjective state of a feeling, which is, by definition, a sensation that is accessible only to the person experiencing it and nobody else (Gebauer, Reference Gebauer2012). Therefore, a first challenge for parents and a child is to refer an emotion term to a mental state, the child’s feeling, that cannot be observed from the outside. A second challenge is that children can observe their parents’ emotional expressions and corresponding antecedents and consequences but cannot observe their parents’ feelings. During their own emotional episodes, children may experience many subjective sensations but are not yet able to link these sensations to an image of how these sensations may look from the outside in the form of perceivable expressions, which is especially the case for facial expressions. Therefore, for a child to assign a word or an expression to a subjective feeling and not just to an expression, this requires that they already have the ability in question: being aware of one’s feelings.

How can children acquire the ability of becoming conceptually aware of their embodied feelings? Emotional expressions can serve as an interface between embodied and conceptual feelings, as postulated in so-called feedback theories of emotions. These theories have a long but also controversial tradition in psychology (Russell, Reference Russell2014). The core idea is that a perceivable expression is the connecting link that is represented as a subjective sensation for the sender (the feeling component) but also as a perceivable expressive behavior for the receiver (Holodynski, Reference Holodynski, Gebauer, Holodynski, Koelsch and von Scheve2017; Holodynski & Seeger, Reference Holodynski and Seeger2019). An important task of parents during early infant–caregiver interactions is to help their child grasp this relationship between feeling and expression and establish a mode of mutual reassurance by referring to feelings via expression signs. Gergely (Reference Gergely, Mayes, Fonagy and Target2007) has formulated a social biofeedback theory of parental affect mirroring that shows how this task can be solved; Stern (Reference Stern1985) called it affect attunement.

By observing infant–caregiver interactions in Western samples, both Gergely (Reference Gergely, Mayes, Fonagy and Target2007) and Stern (Reference Stern1985) found that caregivers intuitively mirror their infants’ emotion-specific expressions in their own expressions. They do this using conventionalized, succinct expression signs that are marked clearly through exaggeration and repetition, which makes it possible to distinguish between when a caregiver is mirroring the child’s emotion and when the caregiver is showing a genuine emotion. This mirroring process provides children with a perceivable expression in others of the sensations they are currently feeling through their emotion in themselves. Because young children are capable of grasping contingencies in their flow of experiences, they learn that their parents are referring to their feeling when mirroring their expression, given sensitive parents who mirror their child’s expression appropriately. Fonagy et al. (Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002) refer to this competence, called mentalization, as the capacity to ascribe and understand the mental states of others and of oneself (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Ensink, Lindqvist, Malberg, Muller, Midgley, Ensink, Lindqvist, Malberg and Muller2017).

At least for mothers in Western cultures, affect attunement is often observed during everyday mother-infant interactions with 2- to 12-month-olds (Jonsson & Clinton, Reference Jonsson and Clinton2006). Infants from 10 months onward begin to understand the mirrored expressions as symbols that refer to their own corresponding feeling state. However, this understanding initially works only on the level of exchanging expressions. In a subsequent developmental task, children begin to assign words to feelings via expressions and begin to understand elicitors, consequences, and the social dynamic of an ongoing emotion episode; in turn, they acquire all three ability levels of full-blown reflective emotions (Álvarez et al., Reference Álvarez, Lázaro, Gordo, Elejalde and Pampliega2022; Holodynski, Reference Holodynski, Gebauer, Holodynski, Koelsch and von Scheve2017, Itakura et al., Reference Itakura, Moriguchi, Morita, Watanabe and Kuczaj2013). In recent studies, reflective functioning has also been conceptualized to involve parents’ references to their children’s mental states in their sensitive affective and behavioral responses to their children’s needs and demands (Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017; Slade, Reference Slade2005).

7.1.3 Differential Impact of Parents on the Development of Emotions

For all three aforementioned developmental trajectories, parents play a pivotal role in supporting their children in mastering these developmental tasks (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007). This is because children’s emotions develop in a social context that is mainly preselected and shaped by their caregivers, especially their parents, in their first years of life. Parents interpret their children’s emotional reactions against the background of both their cultural and their individual child-rearing attitudes. Different child-rearing attitudes are related to different child-rearing practices that result in different impacts on children’s development of emotions (Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski, Friedlmeier and Valsiner2012; Röttger-Rössler et al., Reference Röttger-Rössler, Scheidecker, Jung and Holodynski2013).

Three interrelated parental strategies can be identified that shape the actions and expressions of children in a given context. Whether this shaping contributes to a more functional or dysfunctional development seems to depend mainly on parental sensitivity, at least in Western cultures (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cissé, Salali and Migliano2018), with parental sensitivity being defined as a caregiver’s ability to perceive and infer the meaning behind the infant’s behavioral and expressive signals and to respond to these promptly and appropriately. In the long run, differential parental child-rearing practices lead to both cultural and individual pathways of emotion differentiation (e.g. Kärtner et al., Reference Kärtner, Holodynski and Wörmann2013; Keller et al., Reference Keller, Bard, Morelli, Chaudhary, Vicedo, Rosabal‐Coto, Scheidecker, Murray and Gottlieb2018).

7.1.3.1 Preselection of Contexts

Parents preselect the contexts in which their infants and children grow up, which enables and restricts their modes of experience. For example, when parents strongly encourage their infants to have face-to-face interactions and provide opportunities for object manipulation and exploration, as many parents in Western cultures do, they enable their infants’ early experiences of self-efficacy, resulting in joy, but such interactions may also result in infants experiencing a loss of self-efficacy, resulting in frustration. In contrast, when parents favor a rather calm child, as they do in rural Nso in Cameroon or rural Bara in Madagascar, for example, they restrict infants from exciting exploration but enable close body contact and often feeding; this satisfies their infants’ needs, making it unnecessary for them to show distress through distress expressions (Kärtner et al., Reference Kärtner, Holodynski and Wörmann2013).

7.1.3.2 Mirroring Children’s Expressions and Reflective Functioning

Sensitive parents often model and mirror their children’s expressions from an early age. This provides the child with an image of what their inferred feeling looks like. This affect mirroring is not a mere copying of the child’s expression but a succinct, conventionalized pattern of emotion expression that is being assigned to the child. One result of this affect mirroring is that the child’s expressions are shaped toward more synchronized and conventionalized expression signs for the inferred emotion. A second result is that the child can match their actual feeling of an emotion to the corresponding expression of what their feeling looks like (Álvarez et al., Reference Álvarez, Lázaro, Gordo, Elejalde and Pampliega2022). In her review of empirical studies, Camoirano (Reference Camoirano2017) summarized that maternal reflective functioning correlates with quality of caregiving (even in clinical samples), correlates with children’s own reflective functioning and promotes children’s understanding of emotions and their emotional self-regulation. Low maternal reflective functioning was correlated with impairments in emotion regulation and children’s externalizing behaviors.

7.1.3.3 Modeling

Children learn the elicitors of emotions not only through their own experiences but also through observing and imitating their parents’ emotional expressions, especially when confronted with new or ambiguous objects, people or situations. From the age of 10 months onward, children start using the signaled conventionalized expressions of their parents as social referencing for how to feel toward ambiguous elicitors (Vandivier & Hertenstein, Reference Vandivier, Hertenstein, Mohiyeddini, Eysenck and Bauer2013); this is especially the case for elicitors of fear (e.g. de Rosnay et al., Reference de Rosnay, Cooper, Tsigaras and Murray2006).

Taken together, infants’ precursor emotions develop into fully functioning emotions. Parents support this development by appropriately interpreting their infant’s expressions and body reactions, mirroring them in their own expressions in the form of succinct expression displays, and responding promptly with actions that meet their child’s signaled needs. Hence, emotional development and processing in infants are initially shared between the child and caregiver. Starting with coregulation initiated by the caregiver, the infant develops an increasingly autonomous regulation of their actions by their own elicited emotions.

7.2 Development of a Reflective Form of Emotion Regulation

7.2.1 Relation between Emotion and Emotion Regulation

The aforementioned shift from coregulation to self-regulation is also related to the emergence of a reflective form of regulating one’s own emotions by volitionally applying emotion regulation strategies. This regulation of emotions is called emotion regulation and can be defined in line with Thompson (Reference Thompson1994, pp. 27–28) as “extrinsic and intrinsic processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish one’s goal.”

The concept of emotion regulation has been defined in multiple ways. In a broader sense, every action associated with the modification of emotions could be seen as emotion regulation (Thompson, Reference Thompson1994). However, from a developmental perspective, we find it critically important to distinguish regulation by emotion from regulation of emotion (Gross & Thompson Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007; Holodynski et al., Reference Holodynski, Seeger, Kortas-Hartmann, Wörmann, Barrett, Fox, Morgan, Fidler and Daunhauer2013). Both constitute developmental achievements during children’s development, but they occur at different ages. Regulation by an emotion refers to an action readiness that is inherent to the emotion itself (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986), for example, the avoidance of gaze in situations of (social) overstimulation or fleeing in situations of fear. This process develops relatively early in ontogenesis (Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski and Friedlmeier2006). By contrast, regulation of an emotion refers to situations that arise when the individual first experiences an emotion and then tries to regulate it, for example, they avoid looking at an attractive gift in a situation that requires delaying gratification. Thus, the emotion and its associated action readiness are no longer the motivating force of behavior but are the target process that becomes regulated. In this case, emotion regulation is based on the volitional inhibition or modification of an elicited emotion so that the dominant action readiness of an emotion is not enacted but replaced by a subdominant behavioral alternative (Campos et al., Reference Campos, Frankel and Camras2004). In the following discussion, this process is referred to as reflective emotion regulation (Holodynski et al., Reference Holodynski, Seeger, Kortas-Hartmann, Wörmann, Barrett, Fox, Morgan, Fidler and Daunhauer2013).

7.2.2 Emotional Awareness and Acquisition of Emotion Regulation Strategies

The acquisition of a reflective mode of emotion regulation is an endeavor that takes one’s whole childhood and consists of at least two intertwined abilities: (1) transitioning unreflective emotions into reflective emotions, that is, becoming aware of one’s own and others’ emotions and (2) acquiring emotion regulation strategies and using them appropriately. Most recent parental intervention programs, such as Tuning in to Kids, Parent–Child Interaction Therapy-Emotion Development and Emotion Enhanced Triple P, address both abilities (but in slightly different terms) with empirically confirmed success, especially concerning improved parenting behavior (England-Mason & Gonzalez, Reference England-Mason and Gonzalez2020).

7.2.2.1 Transitioning Unreflective Emotions into Reflective Emotions

The ability to perform this transition has been already described in Section 7.1.2, Trajectories of Emotional Development. Becoming aware of a currently felt emotion and understanding its relational meaning in the specific context enables children to psychologically distance themselves from the spontaneous action readiness of the felt emotion; this distancing facilitates the selection and application of an appropriate regulation strategy. However, only a few intervention programs explicitly mention the strategy of affect mirroring as a first step in triggering emotional awareness (Silkenbeumer et al., Reference Silkenbeumer, Schiller, Holodynski and Kärtner2016). Most of them start with the second step, labeling, exploring, and validating the inferred feeling of the child, which is suitable only when the child is already aware of their feelings (England-Mason & Gonzalez, Reference England-Mason and Gonzalez2020; Gottman et al., Reference Gottman, Katz and Hooven1997).

7.2.2.2 Acquiring Emotion Regulation Strategies

Gross and Thompson (Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007) distinguish four classes of regulation strategies: distraction, reappraisal, soothing, and response modulation. These strategies have their primary impact at different points in the emotion-generative process, but any of these strategies can lead to the desired behavioral outcome, that is, effective emotion regulation. For instance, whereas distraction strategies shift the focus to a new event that establishes a new line of consecutive emotion-generative processes (e.g. appraisal, action readiness, feeling) resulting in a different emotion, response modulation strategies directly operate on the level of the behavioral inclination and either inhibit the impulse or transform it to an appropriate response.

During the preschool years, children establish an increasing repertoire of effective emotion regulation strategies. Early in the second year, a significant developmental achievement can be seen in social referencing and the self-initiation of interpersonal regulation. Hence, an intentional search for social support is considered a rudimentary form of self-regulation. During the second and third years, there is a transition from using passive to more active strategies of emotion regulation, and, as a consequence, the two first truly self-initiated strategies emerge, namely distracting oneself from emotion-eliciting events and self-soothing (Bridges & Grolnick, Reference Bridges, Grolnick and Eisenberg1995; Calkins & Hill, Reference Calkins, Hill and Gross2007; Spinrad et al., Reference Spinrad, Stifter, Donelan-McCall and Turner2004).

During preschool years, children learn advanced forms of mentalizing (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Biney and Abbot-Smith2018) and self-distancing (Grenell et al., Reference Grenell, Prager, Schaefer, Kross, Duckworth and Carlson2019) that enables them to become aware of their emotional and motivational action readiness and to inhibit and modify them by applying a regulation strategy on them. The latter ability is also called “hot executive functions” because they are directed to emotional and motivational action readiness and their volitional inhibition and modification in contrast to “cool executive functions” that are directed to thoughts and actions and their volitional inhibition and modification (Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Qu, Müller, Schneider, Schumann-Hengsteler and Sodian2005). Together with further sociocognitive achievements, such as normative development that enables thinking based on rules and social norms (Rakoczy & Schmidt, Reference Rakoczy and Schmidt2013), children refine their skills to modulate emotionally triggered behavioral inclinations, and they acquire increasingly complex cognitive regulation strategies, such as reappraisal strategies, that allow them to satisfy their motives in socially coordinated and accepted ways. Regulation of behavior and emotions that are disapproved of can also be achieved by acquiring and eliciting moral emotions such as compassion, shame, and guilt, which trigger a strong readiness to override the ostracized behavior (Hofmann & Doan, Reference Hofmann, Doan, Hofmann and Doan2018).

The acquisition of emotion regulation strategies can also be described as a shift from coregulation to self-regulation, as was already stated for the action regulating function of emotions. The internalization model of reflective emotion regulation addresses the gradual transition that occurs as a child acquires increasingly self-regulated levels of emotion regulation (Silkenbeumer et al., Reference Silkenbeumer, Schiller, Holodynski and Kärtner2016, Reference Silkenbeumer, Schiller and Kärtner2018). During infancy and early childhood, parents’ and other caregivers’ focus is primarily on coregulation by their child’s emotions, that is, they interpret their child’s emotion expressions as appeals to carry out suitable actions on behalf of the child. However, as children grow older, parents increasingly start to apply coregulation of their child’s emotions, as defined previously, when they use distraction or soothing strategies when the intended action would be infeasible or inappropriate, for example, when a young child starts crying because their father prevents them from picking up a sharp knife, as it would be inappropriate to let the child have the knife. In social interaction, parents introduce culturally generated emotion regulation strategies that, in the course of development, their child can adopt and internalize as mental functions.

The internalization model of reflective emotion regulation (Silkenbeumer et al., Reference Silkenbeumer, Schiller, Holodynski and Kärtner2016) postulates three different levels that specify the strategies used by parents and the implications that these levels have for child development (see also Figure 7.1).

Figure 7.1 The internalization model of reflective emotion regulation

At Level 1 (substitutional emotion regulation), parents adopt all components of reflective emotion regulation. Thus, it is the parent who becomes aware of their child’s emotion by mirroring it, understands its significance and decides whether and how an emotion needs to be regulated, and does so where necessary, for example, by soothing or distracting the child.

At Level 2 (specific prompts), parents start prompting the child to carry out simple actions that enable a modification of the emotion in question. More specifically, coregulation is characterized by providing specific prompts that the child should follow. This might, for example, be an instruction to follow a specific rule (e.g. to take turns), to take a deep breath, or to play with the building blocks instead of the ball. This level of coregulation helps the child to establish a basic repertoire of effective behavioral routines to regulate emotions. Children can adopt the used strategy, the verbal instructions, into their repertoire by instructing themselves how to behave in order to regulate an elicited emotion.

At Level 3 (metacognitive prompts), caregivers use metacognitive prompts that leave the generation, selection, and application of strategies to the child: more specifically, caregivers prompt the child either to choose from a set of alternative appraisals or responses and execute the self-chosen alternative or to generate alternative appraisals or behavioral responses. This level of coregulation supports the child in flexibly exploring and evaluating alternative regulation strategies and in choosing and executing a specific regulation strategy from a set of possible responses.

Through these different levels of coregulation, children regulate emotions in increasingly self-regulated ways: they become aware of their inner feeling states (starting at Level 1), they establish a repertoire of effective strategies to regulate emotions (starting at Level 2), and they start to generate alternative strategies and select and enact one of these alternatives (starting at Level 3). Unlike other approaches (e.g. Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1994; Lemerise & Arsenio, Reference Lemerise and Arsenio2000), this model postulates a formative phase in which self-regulation is critically constituted by the way in which caregivers coregulate emotionally challenging episodes.

7.2.3 Parents’ Influences on the Development of Coregulation

Both correlational and intervention studies support the claims of the internalization model of reflective emotion regulation. For instance, a gradual transition occurs from parents to their child regarding who initiates emotion regulation, and parents’ coregulation decreases as children acquire the necessary skills for self-regulation (Grolnick et al., Reference Grolnick, Kurowski, McMenamy, Rivkin and Bridges1998; Holodynski & Friedlmeier, Reference Holodynski and Friedlmeier2006). Although parents initially adopt distraction and soothing strategies, children increasingly apply these strategies themselves (Bridges & Grolnick, Reference Bridges, Grolnick and Eisenberg1995; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1996). Furthermore, evidence suggests that a shift occurs from more substitutional strategies that are typically provided at Level 1 or 2 (e.g. distraction, soothing) to more instructive and reflective ways of coregulation that are provided at Level 2 or 3 (e.g. prompts for reappraisal or response modulation). Importantly, these age-dependent changes support children’s emotion regulation competence concurrently and prospectively (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Morris, Steinberg, Aucoin and Keyes2011; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Spritz and Stifter2002).

Following a transactional approach, Silkenbeumer et al. (Reference Silkenbeumer, Schiller and Kärtner2018, Reference Silkenbeumer, Lüken, Holodynski and Kärtner2022) showed that, first, caregivers’ (i.e. preschool teachers’) coregulation was sensitive to children’s developmental status. More specifically, the likelihood of coregulation decreased across the preschool years, and caregivers more often coregulated using metacognitive prompts when children had higher levels of socioemotional competence. Furthermore, caregivers were more likely to use distraction, reappraisal, and soothing strategies when children were sad, whereas they were more likely to apply response modulation strategies when children were angry (Silkenbeumer et al., Reference Silkenbeumer, Lüken, Holodynski and Kärtner2022). Second, the findings suggest that children were more likely to carry out a specific strategy successfully when this strategy was prompted by their caregiver and, for older children, when caregivers used metacognitive prompts (Silkenbeumer et al., Reference Silkenbeumer, Schiller and Kärtner2018, Reference Silkenbeumer, Lüken, Holodynski and Kärtner2022).

For parents, converging evidence from correlational studies (for a meta-analysis, see Zinsser et al., Reference Zinsser, Gordon and Jiang2021), intervention studies (for a review, see England-Mason & Gonzalez, Reference England-Mason and Gonzalez2020) and experimental studies (e.g. Loop & Roskam, Reference Loop and Roskam2016) indicates that parents’ coregulation can be improved and has immediate and long-term effects on children’s effective emotion regulation.

7.3 Outlook

The aim of this chapter was to embed the development of emotion and emotion regulation in a broader sociocultural context by emphasizing the constitutive role of culturally saturated social interaction between caregivers and their children. At the same time, the proposed models are normative in that they clearly focus on children’s explicit emotional awareness and reflective, rational forms of emotion regulation, which is a specific approach to emotion regulation that characterizes Western cultures (Kärtner et al., Reference Kärtner, Holodynski and Wörmann2013; Keller & Kärtner, Reference Keller, Kärtner, Gelfand, Chiu and Hong2013). As outlined in this chapter, becoming aware of feelings is fostered through specific parenting strategies in the coregulation of children’s emotions, especially affect mirroring, modeling, labeling, and validating children’s emotions. Later in development, caregivers help children to build an increasingly complex repertoire of regulation strategies by supporting emotion regulation at different levels through the provision of specific and, when appropriate, metacognitive prompts. Most of the recent parental intervention programs mentioned here consider one or more of these prerequisites. Based on this framework, we highlight three important directions for future research.

7.3.1 Developing Measures of Emerging Emotional Awareness

Although the theories on the early emergence of emotional awareness are well established, direct measures of emotional awareness mainly rely on self-report, that is, the labeling by the subject of their own emotions, or emotional awareness is inferred indirectly from the labeling of others’ emotions (which similarly depends on advanced language development) or from other downstream developmental attainments such as successful emotion regulation. In order to better document the developmental emergence of emotional awareness and to test the relation between emotional awareness and emotion regulation more directly, the construction of methods beyond verbal self-reports is needed to assess emotional awareness in young children. One promising approach is measures that are based on the action readiness of the elicited emotion. For instance, in the study by Kortas-Hartmann (Reference Kortas-Hartmann2013), children’s feelings of pride that prototypically lead to a stretching of the body with raised arms were translated into an upward movement of a marker on a vertical scale (in the study, a 1 m scale was used). The more intense the child’s sensation of stretching and raising arms, the higher up the scale the marker moved. The opposite movement was used for signaling the intensity of disappointment, corresponding to a collapsing of the body and a downward movement of the marker on the vertical one-meter scale. These nonverbal measures correlated with the valence and intensity of a pride elicitor resulting in an upward movement of the marker and of a disappointment elicitor resulting in a downward movement of the marker, as well as with the intensity of displayed expressions (Kortas-Hartmann, Reference Kortas-Hartmann2013).

7.3.2 Probing the Function of Affect Mirroring across Childhood

Although affect mirroring is a key concept of emotion development during infancy, its role during childhood needs to be further analyzed. Although most concepts and programs (e.g. the emotion coaching of Gottman et al., Reference Gottman, Katz and Hooven1997 or Havighurst et al., Reference Havighurst, Wilson, Harley and Prior2009) mainly rely on labeling, exploring, and validating children’s emotions, we propose that mirroring children’s affect retains an important function beyond infancy, and maybe even beyond childhood: it creates a closer connection with others, signals acceptance, and catalyzes awareness of others’ emotions. First evidence for such a function is provided by Silkenbeumer et al. (Reference Silkenbeumer, Lüken, Holodynski and Kärtner2022), showing that preschool teachers’ initial mirroring of their children’s emotion helps them to show self-regulation in emotionally challenging episodes that occur during natural observation. Beyond replication, it would be promising to provide experimental support for the function of affect mirroring for children’s emotion regulation.

7.3.3 Complementing Reflective Emotion Regulation with Regulation by Moral Emotions

The first two perspectives for future research are situated within the framework elaborated here; the third perspective aims at complementing the ideal of reflective emotion regulation with other mechanisms supporting socially appropriate experience and behavior. Although reflective forms of emotion regulation are often emphasized in parental training programs as the “royal road” to regulating inappropriate emotional reactions, inappropriate reactions can also be regulated by moral emotions such as shame or guilt. These emotions can be elicited through reappraisal strategies. In fact, these coregulation strategies occur across cultures, but are evaluated very differently: although they are typically associated with an emotion-dismissive meta-emotion philosophy in Western culture, they are evaluated very positively in other cultures (Quinn, Reference Quinn2005; Röttger-Rössler et al., Reference Röttger-Rössler, Scheidecker, Jung and Holodynski2013, Reference Röttger-Rössler, Funk, Scheidecker and Holodynski2015). The framework elaborated here clearly leans toward a normative perspective on the self (i.e. mirroring and validating disruptive emotions such as anger as well as self-enhancing emotions such as pride, but minimizing emotions that may harm children’s self-esteem such as shame; see, for instance, Miller et al., Reference Miller, Wiley, Fung and Liang1997). However, an interesting perspective lies in synthesizing these approaches and elaborating the healthy function of regulating moral emotions (e.g. guilt and shame) within this framework.

References

Álvarez, N., Lázaro, M. H., Gordo, L., Elejalde, L. I., & Pampliega, A. M. (2022). Maternal mentalization and child emotion regulation: A comparison of different phases of early childhood. Infant Behavior & Development, 66, 101681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.10168Google Scholar
Bridges, L. J., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The development of emotional self-regulation in infancy and early childhood. In Eisenberg, N. (Ed.), Social development (pp. 185211). Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation: Biological and environmental transactions in early development. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 229248). Guilford.Google Scholar
Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: A review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377394.Google Scholar
Campos, J., Camras, L., Lee, R., He, M., & Campos, R. G. (2018). A relational recasting of the principles of emotional competence. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 711727. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2018.1502921Google Scholar
Camras, L. (2022). Emotional development across the lifespan. Guilford.Google Scholar
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. Emotion, reason, and the human brain. Avon.Google Scholar
de Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(8), 11651175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.09.003Google Scholar
Engelen, E.-M., Markowitsch, H., Scheve, C. V., Röttger-Rössler, B., Stephan, A., Holodynski, M., & Vandekerckhove, M. (2009). Emotions as bio-cultural processes: Disciplinary debates and an interdisciplinary outlook. In Röttger-Rössler, B. & Markowitsch, H. (Eds.), Emotions as bio-cultural processes (pp. 2353). Springer.Google Scholar
England-Mason, G., & Gonzalez, A. (2020). Intervening to shape children’s emotion regulation: A review of emotion socialization parenting programs for young children. Emotion, 20(1), 98104. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000638Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (2009). The psychophysiology of self-awareness: Rediscovering the lost art of body sense. Norton.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. Other Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gebauer, G. (2012). How can we talk about emotions? Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Philosophie, 37(2), 131164Google Scholar
Gergely, G. (2007). The social construction of the subjective self: The role of affect-mirroring, markedness, and ostensive communication in self-development. In Mayes, L., Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (Eds.), Developmental science and psychoanalysis: Integration and innovation (pp. 4588). Karnac Books.Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Grenell, A., Prager, E. O., Schaefer, C., Kross, E., Duckworth, A. L., & Carlson, S. M. (2019). Individual differences in the effectiveness of self‐distancing for young children’s emotion regulation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 37, 84100. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12259Google Scholar
Grolnick, W. S., Kurowski, C. O., McMenamy, J. M., Rivkin, I., & Bridges, L. J. (1998). Mothers’ strategies for regulating their toddlers’ distress. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(3), 437450. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90018-2Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). Guilford.Google Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2001). Affective social competence. Social Development, 10(1), 79119. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00150Google Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., & Prior, M. R. (2009). Tuning in to kids: An emotion-focused parenting program—Initial findings from a community trial. Journal of Community Psychology, 37, 10081023. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, S. G., & Doan, S. N. (2018). Moral and collective emotions. In Hofmann, S. G. & Doan, S. N. (Eds.), The social foundations of emotion: Developmental, cultural, and clinical dimensions (pp. 103124). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Holodynski, M. (2017). Wie Kinder lernen, über ihre Emotionen zu sprechen [How children learn to talk about their emotions]. In Gebauer, G., Holodynski, M., Koelsch, S., & von Scheve, C. (Eds.), Von der Emotion zur Sprache. Wie wir lernen, über Gefühle zu sprechen (pp. 85189). Velbrück.Google Scholar
Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and emotion regulation. Springer.Google Scholar
Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2012). Affect and culture. In Valsiner, J. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 957986). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holodynski, M., & Seeger, D. (2019). Expressions as signs and their significance for emotional development. Developmental Psychology, 55, 18121829. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000698CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holodynski, M., Seeger, D., Kortas-Hartmann, P., & Wörmann, V. (2013). Placing emotion regulation in a developmental framework of self-regulation. In Barrett, K. C., Fox, N. A., Morgan, G. A., Fidler, D., & Daunhauer, L. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development: New directions and international perspectives (pp. 2759). Routledge.Google Scholar
Itakura, S., Moriguchi, Y., & Morita, T. (2013). The development of mentalizing and emotion in human children. In Watanabe, S. & Kuczaj, S. (Eds.), Emotions of animals and humans: Comparative perspectives (pp. 207222). Springer Science + Business Media.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 125. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonsson, C. O., & Clinton, D. (2006). What do mothers attune to during interactions with their infants? Infant and Child Development, 15(4), 387402. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.466Google Scholar
Kärtner, J., Holodynski, M., & Wörmann, V. (2013). Parental ethnotheories, social practice and the culture-specific development of social smiling in infants. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 20(1), 7995. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749039.2012.742112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Bard, K., Morelli, G., Chaudhary, N., Vicedo, M., Rosabal‐Coto, M., Scheidecker, G., Murray, M., & Gottlieb, A. (2018). The myth of universal sensitive responsiveness: Comment on Mesman et al. (2017). Child Development, 89, 19211928. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13031Google Scholar
Keller, H. & Kärtner, J. (2013). Development – The cultural solution of universal developmental tasks. In Gelfand, M., Chiu, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. (Eds.), Advances in culture and psychology: Vol. 3 (pp. 63116). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kortas-Hartmann, P. (2013). Methodische Artefakte im Internalisierungsmodell der Emotionsentwicklung [Methodological artefacts of the internalization model of emotional development]. Monsenstein und Vannerdat.Google Scholar
Lambie, J. A. (2009). Emotion experience, rational action, and self-knowledge. Emotion Review, 1(3), 272280. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073909103596Google Scholar
Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development, 71, 107118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M. (2011). Inside and outside: The relation between emotional states and expressions. Emotion Review, 3, 189196. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910387947Google Scholar
Loop, L., & Roskam, I. (2016). Do children behave better when parents’ emotion coaching practices are stimulated? A micro-trial study. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 22232235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0382-0Google Scholar
Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2018). Individual differences in children’s pragmatic ability: A review of associations with formal language, social cognition, and executive functions. Language Learning and Development, 14, 186223. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584Google Scholar
Mesman, J., Minter, T., Angnged, A., Cissé, I. A. H., Salali, G. D., & Migliano, A. B. (2018). Universality without uniformity: A culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89, 837850. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12795Google Scholar
Midgley, N., Ensink, K., Lindqvist, K., Malberg, N., & Muller, N. (2017). The development of mentalizing. In Midgley, N., Ensink, K., Lindqvist, K., Malberg, N., & Muller, N, Mentalization-based treatment for children: A time-limited approach (pp. 1537). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000028-002Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., & Liang, C.-H. (1997). Personal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American families. Child Development, 68(3), 557568. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131678Google Scholar
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5, 119124. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912468165Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Morris, M. S., Steinberg, L., Aucoin, K. J., & Keyes, A. W. (2011). The influence of mother–child emotion regulation strategies on children’s expression of anger and sadness. Developmental Psychology, 47, 213225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021021Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.xGoogle Scholar
Norman, G. J., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Emotion, somatovisceral afference, and autonomic regulation. Emotion Review, 6(2), 113123. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913512006Google Scholar
Price, T. F., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2015). Embodied emotion: The influence of manipulated facial and bodily states on emotive responses. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(6), 461473. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1370Google Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Spritz, B. L., & Stifter, C. A. (2002). Mother–child coregulation during delay of gratification at 30 months. Infancy, 3, 209225. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_6Google Scholar
Quinn, N. (2005). Universals of child rearing. Anthropological Theory, 5, 477516. https://doi.org/10.1177/1463499605059233Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., & Schmidt, M. F. H. (2013). The early ontogeny of social norms. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 1721. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12010Google Scholar
Röttger-Rössler, B., Scheidecker, G., Jung, S., & Holodynski, M. (2013). Socializing emotions in childhood: A cross-cultural comparison between the Bara in Madagascar and the Minangkabau in Indonesia. Mind, Culture, and Activity: An International Journal, 20, 260287. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749039.2013.806551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Röttger-Rössler, B., Funk, L., Scheidecker, G., & Holodynski, M. (2015). Learning (by) feeling: A cross-cultural comparison of the socialization and development of emotions. Ethos, 43, 187220. https://doi.org/10.1111/etho.12080Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2014). Introduction: William James and his legacy. Emotion Review, 6, 3. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913503610Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2004). Feelings integrate the central representation of appraisal-driven response organization in emotion. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 136157). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shuman, V., & Scherer, K. R. (2014). Concepts and structures of emotions. In Pekrun, R. & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education. (pp. 1335). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Silkenbeumer, J., Lüken, L., Holodynski, M., & Kärtner, J. (2022). Emotion socialization in early childhood education and care – How preschool teachers support children’s emotion regulation [Manuscript submitted for publication]. University of Münster.Google Scholar
Silkenbeumer, J., Schiller, E. M., & Kärtner, J. (2018). Co- and self-regulation of emotions in the preschool setting. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 44, 7281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.014Google Scholar
Silkenbeumer, J., Schiller, E., Holodynski, M., & Kärtner, J. (2016). The role of co-regulation for the development of social-emotional competence. Journal of Self-Regulation and Regulation, 2, 1126.Google Scholar
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 269281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245906Google Scholar
Spinrad, T. L., Stifter, C. A., Donelan-McCall, N., & Turner, L. (2004). Mothers’ regulation strategies in response to toddlers’ affect: Links to later emotion self-regulation. Social Development, 13, 4055. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00256.xGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 2552.Google Scholar
Vandivier, L. E., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2013). Social referencing in infancy: Important findings and future directions. In Mohiyeddini, C., Eysenck, M., & Bauer, S. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology of emotions: Vol, 1. Recent theoretical perspectives and novel empirical findings (pp. 8185). Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In Schneider, W., Schumann-Hengsteler, R., & Sodian, B. (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 7193). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zinsser, K. M., Gordon, R. A., & Jiang, X. (2021). Parents’ socialization of preschool-aged children’s emotion skills: A meta-analysis using an emotion-focused parenting practices framework. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 55, 377390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.02.001Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 7.1 The internalization model of reflective emotion regulation

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×