Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T13:42:19.951Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Caretaker’s Influence on Vernacularity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2020

Mary Kohn
Affiliation:
Kansas State University
Walt Wolfram
Affiliation:
North Carolina State University
Charlie Farrington
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
Jennifer Renn
Affiliation:
Purdue University, Indiana
Janneke Van Hofwegen
Affiliation:
Google, Inc.
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we examine the relationship between the vernacularity of caretaker speech as represented by the participants’ mothers and the subjects in the FPG sample. In this chapter, we examine the relationship between the vernacularity of caretaker speech as represented by the participants’ mothers and the subjects in the FPG sample. The analysis indicates that a significant relationship between the relative vernacularity of AAL-speaking mothers and their children exists across most of the early lifespan. Further, the analysis of the impact of relevant social and family factors on the development of AAL vernacularity reveals gender as a significant determiner at only the earliest and the latest points in the early lifespan. Finally, the analysis uncovers the nuanced picture of the relationship between mothers’ and children’s vernacularity – that adolescent females from low-vernacular families are likely to become low-vernacular speakers (like their mothers) in older adolescence. Finally, we see the departure of the children from the parents in the early stages of schooling, following the roller coaster trajectory described in the overall trajectory of vernacularity described for morphosyntactic features.

Type
Chapter
Information
African American Language
Language development from Infancy to Adulthood
, pp. 111 - 134
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agresti, Alan. 2007. An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis, 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 2008. Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bailey, Guy. 2002. Real and apparent time. In Chambers, J.K., Trudgill, Peter, and Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 312331. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bellinger, David. 1980. Consistency in the pattern of change in mothers’ speech: Some discriminant analyses. Journal of Child Language 7(3): 469487.Google Scholar
Brugman, Henni and Wittenburg, Peter. 2005. Eudico Linguistic Annotator (ELAN) (Version 3.3.0) [Computer Program]. Retrieved February 24, 2008, from www.lat-mpi.eu/tools/elan/Google Scholar
Chevrot, Jean-Pierre, Nardy, Aurélie, and Barbu, Stéphanie. 2011. Developmental dynamics of SES-related differences in children’s production of obligatory and variable phonological alternations. Language Sciences 33(1): 180191.Google Scholar
Craig, Holly K. and Washington, Julie A.. 2006. Malik Goes to School: Examining the Language Skills of African American Students from Preschool-5th Grade. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, Manual. 2005. The emergence of adult-like command of sociolinguistic variables: A study of consonant weakening in Spanish-speaking children. In Eddington, David (ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages, 5665. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2000. Linguistic Variation as Social Practice: The Linguistic Construction of Social Meaning in Belten High. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope and Sally, McConnell-Ginet. 2003. Language and Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisikovits, Edina. 1997. Sex differences in inter- and intra-group interactions among adolescents. In Pawels, Anne (ed.), Women and Language in Australia and New Zealand Society, 4558. Sydney: Australian Professional Publications.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerard, and Watt, Dominic. 1999. Tracking the emergence of structured variation realizations of (t) by Newcastle children. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics 7: 123.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerard, and Watt, Dominic. 2005. Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language 81(1): 177205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garbarino, James, Sebes, Janet, and Schellenbach, Cynthia. 1984. Families at risk for destructive parent-child relations in adolescence. Child Development 55(1): 174183.Google Scholar
Giles, Howard and Powesland, Peter F.. 1975. Speech Style and Social Evaluation. London/New York: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Green, Lisa J. 2011. Language and the African American Child. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hazen, Kirk. 2002. The family. In Chambers, J.K., Trudgill, Peter and Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 500525. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Holmes, Thomas H. and Rahe, Richard H.. 1967. The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 11(2): 213218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hymes, Dell. 1974. Foundations in Sociolinguistics: An Ethnographic Approach. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Daniel Ezra. 2009. Getting off the Goldvarb standard: Introducing Rbrul for mixed-effects variable rule analysis. Language and Linguistics Compass 3(1): 359383.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul. 1996. Children, adolescents, and language change. Language Variation and Change 8(2): 177202.Google Scholar
Kovac [Lucas], Ceil. 1980. Children’s Acquisition of Variable Features. Ph.D. Dissertation. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Kovac, Ceil and Douglas Adamson, H.. 1981. Variation theory and first language acquisition. In Sankoff, David and Cedergren, Henrietta (eds.), Variation Omnibus: Current Inquiry into Language and Linguistics, 403410. Edmonton, AB: Linguistics Research, Inc.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1965. Stages in the acquisition of Standard English. In Shuy, Roger W. (ed.), Social Dialects and Language Learning, 77103. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1989. The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change 1(1): 8597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. 1990. The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Language Variation and Change 2(2): 205254.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 2001. Principles of Linguistic Change, Vol. 2: Social Factors. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Loban, Walter. 1975. Language Development: Kindergarten Through Grade Twelve. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers.Google Scholar
Meyerhoff, Miriam. 2006. Introducing Sociolinguistics. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Jon and Iglesias, Aquiles. 2008. Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) (Version 2008.0.1) [Computer Software]. SALT Software, LLC.Google Scholar
Nardy, Aurélie, Chevrot, Jean-Pierre, and Barbu, Stéphanie. 2014. Sociolinguistic convergence and social interactions within a group of preschoolers: A longitudinal study. Language Variation and Change 26(3): 273301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oetting, Janna B. and McDonald, Janet L.. 2002. Methods for characterizing participants’ nonmainstream dialect use in child language research. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45(3): 505518.Google Scholar
Pianta, Robert C. 1992. Child-parent Relationship Scale. Unpublished measure, University of Virginia. https://curry.virginia.edu/faculty-research/centers-labs-projects/castl/measures-developed-robert-c-pianta-phdGoogle Scholar
Pianta, Robert C., Nimetz, Sheri L., and Bennett, Elizabeth. 1997. Mother-child relationships, teacher-child relationships, and school outcomes in preschool and kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 12(3): 263280.Google Scholar
Renn, Jennifer E. 2007. Measuring Style Shift: A Quantitative Analysis of African American English. Master’s thesis. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.Google Scholar
Renn, Jennifer E. 2010. Acquiring Style: The Development of Dialect Shifting among African American Children. Ph.D. Dissertation. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.Google Scholar
Renn, Jennifer E. and Terry, J. Michael. 2009. Operationalizing style: Quantifying style shift in the speech of African American adolescents. American Speech 84(4): 367390.Google Scholar
Reveron, Wilhelmina Wright. 1978. The Acquisition of Four Black English Morphological Rules by Black Preschool Children. Ph.D. dissertation. Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Rickford, John R., Ball, Arnetha, Blake, Renée, Jackson, Raina, and Martin, Naomi. 1991. Rappin on the copula coffin: Theoretical and methodological issues in the analysis of copula variation in African American Vernacular English. Language Variation and Change 3(1): 103132.Google Scholar
Rickford, John R. and Théberge Rafal, Christine. 1995. Preterite had + v-ed in the narratives of African-American preadolescents. American Speech 71(3): 227254.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie. 1997. Hitting a moving target: Acquisition of sound change in progress by Philadelphia children. Language Variation and Change 9(2): 249265.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie. 2002. Child language variation. In Chambers, J.K., Trudgill, Peter, and Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 333348. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie and Labov, William. 1995. Learning to talk Philadelphian: Acquisition of short a by preschool children. Language Variation and Change 7(1): 101–12.Google Scholar
Scherre, Maria Marta Pereira and Naro, Anthony J.. 1992. The serial effect on internal and external variables. Language Variation and Change 4(1): 113.Google Scholar
Sankoff, David, Tagliamonte, Sali A., and Smith, Eric. 2005. GoldVarb (Version 3.0b3) [Computer Program]. Retrieved March 31, 2008, from http://individual.utoronto.ca/tagliamonte/goldvarb.html.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, Jack and Phillips, Deborah (eds.), 2000. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Singer, Judith and Willett, John. 2003. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Even Occurrence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirkin, R. Mark. 2005. Statistics for the Social Sciences, 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, Durham, Mercedes, and Fortune, Liane. 2007. “Mam, my trousers is fa’in doon!”: Community, caregiver, and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect. Language Variation and Change 19(1): 6399.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, Durham, Mercedes, and Richards, Hazel M. 2013. The social and linguistic in the acquisition of sociolinguistic norms: Caregivers, children, and variation. Linguistics 51(2): 285324.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine E. 1972. Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development 43(2): 549565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, Catherine E. 1985. Conversations with children. In Fletcher, Paul and Garman, Michael (eds.), Language Acquisition, 2nd ed., 6989. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine E. 1995. Issues in the study of input: Fine tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In Fletcher, Paul and MacWhinney, Brian (eds.), The Handbook of Child Language, 180193. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Steffenson, Margaret S. 1974. The Acquisition of Black English. Ph.D. Dissertation. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Stockman, Ida J. and Vaughn-Cooke, Fay Boyd. 1982. A re-examination of research on the language of Black children: The need for a new framework. Journal of Education 164(2): 157172.Google Scholar
Stockman, Ida. J. and Vaughn-Cooke, Anna Fay. 1989. Addressing new questions about black children’s language. In Schiffrin, Deborah and Fasold, Ralph W. (eds.), Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 52, 275300. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Van Hofwegen, Janneke. 2011. A Caregiver’s Influence on AAE Vernacularity across the Early Lifespan. Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America 2011 Annual Meeting. Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, January.Google Scholar
Van Hofwegen, Janneke. 2015. The development of African American English through childhood and adolescence. In Lanehart, Sonja L. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of African American Language, 454474. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van Hofwegen, Janneke and Wolfram, Walt. 2010. Coming of age in African American English: A longitudinal study. Journal of Sociolinguistics 14(4): 427455.Google Scholar
Washington, Julie A. and Craig, Holly K.. 2002. Morphosyntactic forms of African American English used by young children and their caretakers. Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics 23(2): 209231.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×