Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:11:01.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

33 - Innovation in Capuchin Monkeys

from Part VI - Innovation and Problem-Solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2021

Allison B. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Josep Call
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Capuchins are highly encephalized New World monkeys (family Cebidae, subfamily Cebinae) living in a variety of forest and savannah habitats, from Central to South America, and currently classified as “gracile” (the Cebus genus) or “robust” (the Sapajus genus). The literature on behavioural plasticity in this taxon highlights purported traditions in the social domain (as the dyadic “games” of Cebus capucinus) and in foraging techniques (notably, the use of tools by Sapajus spp.). Behavioural innovations (sensu “process”) are more easily detected in the social realm, while technological traditions seem to result from [inferred] innovations (sensu “product”) facilitated by innate predispositions and environmental affordances and perpetuated by means of socially biased learning. Constraints related to simpler forms of social learning (like “stimulus enhancement”) may limit the potential for cumulative cultural processes, resulting in conservative traditions, as may be the case of percussive stone tools’ use. On the other hand, the degrees of “niche construction” and “observability” associated to different forms of tool use may explain the difference between the widespread stone tool use traditions and the rarer cases of customary probe use (where individual innovations may occur, but seldom spread by socially mediated learning), in terms of different opportunities for socially mediated learning.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrett, B. J., Monteza-Moreno, C. M., Dogandzic, T., Zwyns, N., Ibáñez, A., & Crofoot, M. C. (2018). Habitual stone-tool-aided extractive foraging in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. Royal Society Open Science, 5, 181002. doi: 10.1098/rsos.181002.Google Scholar
Cardoso, R. M. & Ottoni, E.B. (2016). The effects of tradition on problem solving by two wild populations of bearded capuchin monkeys in a probing task. Biology Letters, 12, 20160604. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0604Google Scholar
Coelho, C. G., Falótico, T., Izar, P., Mannu, M., Resende, B. D., Siqueira, J. O., & Ottoni, E. B. (2015). Social learning strategies for nut-cracking by tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Animal Cognition, 18, 911919. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0861-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. (2013). Stone throwing as a sexual display in wild female bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus. PLoS ONE, 8, e79535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079535CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. (2014). Sexual bias in probe tool manufacture and use by wild bearded capuchin monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 108, 117122. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.036Google Scholar
Falótico, T., Proffitt, T., Ottoni, E. B., Staff, R. A., & Haslam, M. (2019) Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 3, 10341038. doi. org/10.1038/s41559–019-0904-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. M. (2003). Making space for traditions. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 6170. doi: 10.1002/evan.10104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M., Visalberghi, E., & Fedigan, L. M. (2004a). The Complete Capuchin: The Biology of the Genus Cebus. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M., Izar, P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E. B., & Oliveira, M. (2004b). Wild capuchin monkeys use anvils and stone pounding tools. American Journal of Primatology, 64, 359366. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20085CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. M., Biro, D., Eshchar, Y., Humle, T., Izar, P., Resende, B., & Visalberghi, E. (2013). The fourth dimension of tool use: Temporally enduring artefacts aid primates learning to use tools. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20120410. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0410Google Scholar
Galef, B. (2003). Social Learning: Promoter or Inhibitor of Innovation? In Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (Eds.), Animal Innovation, ch.6 (pp. 137152). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, M., Luncz, L. V., Staff, R. A., Bradshaw, F., Ottoni, E. B., & Falótico, T. (2016). Pre-Columbian Monkey Tools. Current Biology, 26, R515R522. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.046Google Scholar
Itani, J. & Nishimura, A. (1973). The Study of Infrahuman Culture in Japan: A Review. In Menzel, E. W. (Ed.), Precultural Primate Behavior (pp. 2650). Basel, Germany: Karger.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 308, 203214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1985.0020Google Scholar
Kendal, R., Coelho, C., Corat, C., & Ottoni, E. B. (2017). Experimental field investigations of cultural capacities in the tool-using bearded capuchin (Sapajus libidinosus). Abstracts of the 7th European Federation for Primatology Meeting. Folia Primatologica, 88, 128. doi: 10.1159/000479094Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Reader, S. M. (2010). Innovation in Animals. In Breed, M. D. & Moore, J. (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of Animal Behaviour (pp. 150154). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C. & Moura, A. C. A. (2015). Necessity, Unpredictability and Opportunity: An Exploration of Ecological and Social Drivers of Behavioral Innovation. In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Animal Creativity and Innovation (pp. 217233). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800648-1.00011-5Google Scholar
Lynch Alfaro, J. W., Boubli, J.P., Olson, L.E., Di Fiore, A., Wilson, B., Gutierrez-Espeleta, G. A. … & Alfaro, M. E. (2012). Explosive Pleistocene range expansion leads to widespread Amazonian sympatry between robust and gracile capuchin monkeys. Journal of Biogeography, 39, 272288. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2699.2011.02609.xGoogle Scholar
Mannu, M. & Ottoni, E. B. (2009). The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: Tool making, associative use, and secondary tools. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 242251. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20642Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., & Sugiyama, Y. (Eds.) (2011). The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba. Tokyo: Springer.Google Scholar
Ottoni, E. B., Resende, B. D., & Izar, P. (2005). Watching the best nutcrackers: What capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others´ tool using skills. Animal Cognition, 8, 215219. doi: 10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8Google Scholar
Ottoni, E. B. & Izar, P. (2008). Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17, 171178. doi: 10.1002/evan.20185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, S. & Manson, J. H. (2008). Manipulative Monkeys: The Capuchins of Lomas Barbudal. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ramsey, G., Bastian, M. L., & van Schaik, C. (2007). Animal innovation defined and operationalized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 393437. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07002373.Google Scholar
Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R., & Beck, B. B. (2011). Animal Tool Behavior. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Souto, A., Bione, C. B. C., Bastos, M., Bezerra, B. M., Fragaszy, D., & Schiel, N. (2011). Critically endangered blonde capuchins fish for termites and use new techniques to accomplish the task. Biology Letters, 7, 532535. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spagnoletti, N., Visalberghi, E., Verderane, M.P., Ottoni, E., Izar, P., & Fragaszy, D. (2012). Stone tool use in wild bearded capuchin monkeys, Cebus libidinosus. Is it a strategy to overcome food scarcity? Animal Behaviour, 83, 12851294. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tebbich, S., Griffin, A.S., Peschl, M.F., & Sterelny, K. (2016). From mechanisms to function: An integrated framework of animal innovation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150195. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0195.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. & Limongelli, L. (1994). Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 1522. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.15Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E., Fragaszy, D. M., Izar, P., & Ottoni, E. B. (2005). Terrestriality and tool use. Science, 308, 951. doi: 10.1126/science.308.5724.951cGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, G. C. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1987). The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101, 159168. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.101.2.159Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y. … & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685. doi: 10.1038/21415Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×