Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:56:18.083Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Social Cognition Overview

from Part III - Social Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2021

Allison B. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Josep Call
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

The social life of animals poses specific adaptive challenges that may be cognitively different to challenges from ecological adaptations to their physical environment.Social cognitive adaptations for dealing with other agents are evolutionarily remarkable in that they automatically become an adaptive challenge that may trigger counter- or co-adaptations. This chapter discusses three main problems in social cognition: first, the issue of mentalism or theory of mind, or whether social cognitive adaptations in animals are based on mentalistic attribution skills that may involve representing the intentions and knowledge of others; second, the cognitive underpinnings of animal communication, with a focus on referential and intentional communication; and third, the problem of how animals know and represent the social relations structuring their groups. There is widespread debate about how the social knowledge and reasoning demonstrated in animal social behavior are exactly implemented. The traditional debate in comparative psychology between reductionist behavioristic explanations and complex cognitive explanations has become especially pronounced in social cognition. A widespread proposal is that the type of knowledge demonstrated by animals is ‘implicit,’ distinct both from the verbally expressible knowledge evolved by humans, and from low-level, reflex-like associative behaviours and habits. However, the key notion of implicit knowledge remains elusive and ill-defined.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Apperly, I. & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953970.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. & Schino, G. (2019). Social complexity from within: How individuals experience the structure and organization of their groups. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(6), 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bachmann, C. & Kummer, H. (1980). Male assessment of female choice in hama-dryas baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 6, 315321.Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Henzi, S. P., & Rendall, D. (2007). Social brains, simple minds: Does social complexity really require cognitive complexity? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 362, 561575.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205226.Google Scholar
Boucherie, P., Loretto, M.-C., Massen, J., & Bugnyar, T. (2019). What constitutes social complexity and social intelligence in birds? Lessons from ravens. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(12), 114.Google Scholar
Bshary, R. & Brown, C. (2014). Fish cognition. Current Biology, 24(19), R947R950.Google Scholar
Bugnyar, T.Reber, S., & Buckner, C. (2016). Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitorsNatural Communities7(10506). doi: 10.1038/ncomms10506 (2016).Google Scholar
Bunge, M. (1980). The Mind-Body Problem: A Psychobiological Approach. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Butterfill, S. A. & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind & Language, 28, 606637.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. & Whiten, A. (Eds.) (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(2), 192206.Google Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7(4), 488498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2005). What Chimpanzees Know about Seeing Revisited: An Explanation of the Third Kind. In Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T., & Roessler, J. (Eds.), Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds (pp. 4564). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Canteloup, C. & Meunier, H. (2017). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Tonkean macaques’ understanding of human goal-directed actions. PeerJ, 5, e3227. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3227Google Scholar
Catala, A., Mang, B., MangWallis L., & Huber, L. (2017). Dogs demonstrate perspective taking based on geometrical gaze following in a Guesser–Knower task. Animal Cognition, 20, 581589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How Monkeys See the World. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., & Silk, J. B. (1995). The responses of female baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) to anomalous social interactions: Evidence for causal reasoning? Journal of Comparative Psychology 109, 134141.Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S. & Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jaysNature, 395(6699), 272274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, N. S., Dally, J. M., & Emery, N. J. (2007). Social cognition by food-caching corvids: The western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 362, 507522.Google Scholar
Crane, T. (1998). Intentionality as the Mark of the Mental. In O’Hear, A (Ed.), Current Issues in the Philosophy of Mind (pp. 229251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Crockford, Catherine, Wittig, Roman M., Mundry, Roger, and Zuberbuhler, Klaus. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142146.Google Scholar
Dally, J. M., Clayton, N. S., & Emery, N. J. (2006). The behaviour and evolution of cache protection and pilferage. Animal Behavior, 72, 1323Google Scholar
Davidson, G. L. et al. (2014). Gaze sensitivity: Function and mechanisms from sensory and cognitive perspectives. Animal Behavior, 87, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, G. L. & Clayton, N. S. (2016). New perspectives in gaze sensitivity research. Learning and Behavior, 44, 917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1983). Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The panglossian paradigm defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 343390.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. and Haugeland, J. C. (1987). Intentionality. In Gregory, R. L. (Ed.), The Oxford Companion to the Mind (pp. 383386). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ducheminsky, N., Henzi, S. P., & Barrett, L. (2014). Responses of vervet monkeys in large troops to terrestrial and aerial predator alarm calls. Behavioral Ecology, 25(6), 14741484.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2009). The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution. Annals of Human Biology, 36(5), 562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, N. & Clayton, N. S. (2009). Comparative social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 87113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454459.Google Scholar
Fabricius, W. V., Boyer, T. W., Weimer, A. A., & Carroll, K. (2010). True or false: Do 5-year-olds understand belief? Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 14021416.Google Scholar
Fischer, J. & Price, T. (2017). Meaning, intention, and inference in primate vocal communication. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 2231.Google Scholar
Flombaum, J. I. & Santos, L. R. (2005). Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Current Biology, 15, 447452.Google Scholar
Furrer, R. D. & Manser, M. B. (2009). The evolution of urgency-based and functionally referential alarm calls in ground-dwelling species. The American Naturalist, 173(3), 400410.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1990). The Emergence of Intentional Communication as a Problem-Solving Strategy in the Gorilla. In Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R. (Eds.), “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives (pp. 333355). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1991). Visual Behavior as a Window for Reading the Minds of Others in Primates. In Whiten, A. (Ed.), Natural Theories of Mind: Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mindreading (pp. 195207). Oxford: B. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1996a). Ostensive Behavior in the Great Apes: The Role of Eye Contact. In Russon, A., Parker, S., & Bard, K. (Eds.), Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes (pp. 131151). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1996b). Second-person intentional relations and the evolution of social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 129130.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (2004). Apes, Monkeys, Children and the Growth of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (2007). Pointing behaviors in apes and human infants: A balanced interpretation. Child Development, 78, 729734.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (2008). The evolution of pretence: From intentional availability to intentional non-existence. Mind and Language, 23(5), 586606.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (2009). Embodying meaning: Insights from primates, autism, and Brentano. Neural Networks, 22(2), 190196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, J. C. (2020). Intentionality. In Vonk, J. and Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior (pp. 19). Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Graham, K. E., Hobaiter, C., Ounsley, J., Furuichi, T., & Byrne, R. W. (2018). Bonobo and chimpanzee gestures overlap extensively in meaning. PLoS Biology, 16(2), e2004825.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66, 377388.Google Scholar
Happé, F., Cook, J., & Bird, G. (2017). The structure of social cognition: In(ter)dependence of sociocognitive processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 243267.Google Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. & Waal, F. d. (Eds.) (1992). Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hare, B, Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771785.Google Scholar
Hare, B, Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Hare, B. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61(2), 139151.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101, 495514.Google Scholar
Hayashi, T., Akikawa, R, Kawasaki, K., Egawa, J., Minamimoto, T., Kobayashi, K., Kato, S., Hori, Y., Nagai, Y., Iijima, A., Someya, T., & Hasegawa, I. (2020). Macaques exhibit implicit gaze bias anticipating others’ false-belief-driven actions via medial prefrontal cortex. Cell Reports, 30(13), 44334444. e5, doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2020.03.013.Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1867). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik 3. Leipzig: Voss. (English: Treatise on Physiological Optics. Optical Society of America, 1924–1925).Google Scholar
Henzi, S. P. & Barrett, L. (2007) Coexistence in female-bonded primate groups. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 4381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(1), 101148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2017). Apes submentalise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(1), 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. (1979). Towards Understanding Relationships. London: Academy Press.Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. (2014). The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology, 24, 15961600.Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1987) Gestalt Theory. In Gregory, R. L. (Ed.), The Oxford Companion to the Mind (pp. 288291). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hochsler, D. Santos, L., & Mclean, E. (2019). Do non-human primates really represent others’ ignorance? A test of the awareness relations hypothesis. Cognition, 190, 7280.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1976). The Social Function of Intellect. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (Eds.), Growing Points in Ethology (pp. 303317). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jensen, K., Silk, J. B., Andrews, K., Bshary, R., Cheney, D. L., & Emery, N., … Teufel, C. (2011). Social Knowledge. In Menzel, R. and Fischer, J. (Eds.), Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition (pp. 267291). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109(2), 224234.Google Scholar
Kano, F. & Call, J. (2014). Great apes generate goal-based action predictions: An eye-tracking study. Psychological Science, 25, 16911698.Google Scholar
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2019) Great apes use self-experience to anticipate an agent’s action in a false-belief test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(42), 2090420909.Google Scholar
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015b). The goggles experiment: Can chimpanzees use self-experience to infer what a competitor can see? Animal Behaviour, 105, 211221.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kersken, V., Gómez, J.-C., Liszkowski, U., Soldati, A., & Hobaiter, C. (2019). A gestural repertoire of 1- to 2-year-old human children: In search of the ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 22(4), 577595.Google Scholar
KirchhoferK. C., ZimmermannF., Kaminski, J., & TomaselloM. (2012). Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing. PLoS One, 7(2), e30913.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal Signals: Mind-Reading and Manipulation. In Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach (2nd ed.), (pp. 380402). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Krøjgaard, P. (2005). Infants’ search for hidden persons. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(1), 7071.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(63)08, 110114.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes’ performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(4), e1343771. https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771Google Scholar
Lazareva, O. F. (2012). Transitive Inference in Nonhuman Animals. In Wasserman, E. A. & Zentall, T. R. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Cognition (2nd ed.) (online publication: doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195392661. 9780195392013.9780195390036).Google Scholar
Leavens, D. & Hopkins, W. (1998). Intentional communication by chimpanzees. A cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Developmental Psychology, 34, 813822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leavens, D., Hopkins, W., & Thomas, R. K. (2004). Referential communication by chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 4857.Google Scholar
Macedonia, J. M. & Evans, C. S. (1993). Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Journal of Ethology, 93(3), 177197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marticorena, D., Ruiz, A. M., Mukerji, C., Goddu, A., & Santos, L. (2011). Monkeys represent others’ knowledge but not their beliefs. Developmental Science , 14(6), 14061416.Google Scholar
Martin, A. & Santos, L. (2014). The origins of belief representation: Monkeys fail to automatically represent others’ beliefs. Cognition, 130, 300308.Google Scholar
McMillan, N., Hahn, A., Spetch, M., & Sturdy, C. (2015). Avian cognition: Examples of sophisticated capabilities in space and song. WIREs Cognitive Science, 6, 285297.Google Scholar
Olkowicz, S., Kocoureka, M., Lucan, R., Porteša, M., Fitch, W. T., Herculano-Houzel, S., & Nemec, P. (2016). Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 72557260.Google Scholar
Ostojic, L., Shaw, R. C., Cheke, L. G., & Clayton, N. S. (2013). Evidence that desire-state attribution may govern food sharing in Eurasian jays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(10), 41234128.Google Scholar
Ostojic, L., Legg, E. W., Brecht, K. F., Lange, F., Deininger, C., Mendl, M., & Clayton, N. S. (2017). Current desires of conspecific observers affect cache-protection strategies in California scrub-jays and Eurasian jays. Current Biology, 27(2), R51R53.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C. & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a “theory of mind.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 362: 731744.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2010), Who Took the Cog out of Cognitive Science? Mentalism in an Era of Anti-Cognitivism. In Frensch, P. A. & Schwarzer, R. (Eds.), International Perspectives on Psychological Science: Cognition and Neuropsychology (pp. 241261). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. & Roessler, J. (2012). From infants’ to children’s appreciation of belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 519525.Google Scholar
Perrett, D. (1999). A Cellular Basis for Reading Minds from Faces and Actions. In Hauser, M. & Konishi, M. (Eds.), The Design of Animal Communication, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestlée.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(3), 1190.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Perilloux, H. K., Reaux, J. E., & Bierschwale, D. T. (1998). Young and juvenile chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) reactions to intentional versus accidental and inadvertent actions. Behavioural Processes, 42, 205218.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 157160.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Vonk, J. (2004). We don’t need a microscope to explore the chimpanzee’s mind. Mind and Language, 19, 128.Google Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978a). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.Google Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978b). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science, 202, 532535.Google Scholar
Price, T., Wadewitz, P., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Hammerschmidt, K., & Fischer, J. (2015). Vervets revisited: A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity. Scientific Reports, 5, 111.Google Scholar
Ristau, C. (1991). Before Mindreading: Attention, Purposes, and Deception in Birds? In Whiten, A. (Ed.), Natural Theories of Mind: Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mindreading (pp. 209222), Oxford: B. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ristau, C. (1993). The cognitive ethology of an “injury-feigning” plover: A beginning. Etología, 3, 5768.Google Scholar
Rivière, A. (1991). Objetos con mente [Objects with Minds]. Madrid: Alianza.Google Scholar
Robbins, M. M., & Robbins, A. M. (2018). Variation in the social organization of gorillas: Life history and socioecological perspectives. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 27(5), 218233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero, T. & Aureli, F. (2017). Conflict Resolution. In Call, J., Burghardt, G. M., Pepperberg, I. M., Snowdon, C. T., & Zentall, T. (Eds.),  APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Basic Concepts, Methods, Neural Substrate, and Behavior (pp. 877–897).Google Scholar
Santos, L. & Hauser, M. (1999). How monkeys see the eyes: Cotton-top tamarins’ reactions to changes in visual attention and action. Animal Cognition, 2, 131139.Google Scholar
Schino, G. (2000). Beyond the Primates: Expanding the Reconciliation Horizon. In Aureli, F. & Waal, F. B. M. (Eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution (pp. 225242). Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Scott, R. & Baillargeon, R. (2017) Early false-belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Science, 21, P237P249.Google Scholar
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., & Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical text. Psychological Science, 22(7), 878880.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 10701094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 153177.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. (2015). Social cognition. Animal Behaviour, 103, 191202.Google Scholar
Shapiro, L. A. (2011). Embodied Cognition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Silk, J. B. (2002). The form and function of reconciliation in primates. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 2144.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. & Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 104, 1722817233.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K., Kaller, T., Call, J., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Chimpanzees extract social information from agonistic screams. PLoS One, 5(7), e11473.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief in two-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587592.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Steele, M. A., Halkin, S. L., Smallwood, P. D., McKenna, T. J., & Beam, M. (2008). Cache protection strategies of a scatter-hoarding rodent: Do tree squirrels engage in behavioural deception? Animal Behaviour, 75, 705714.Google Scholar
Stevens, M., Hopkins, E., Hinde, W., Adcock, A., Connolly, Y., Troscianko, T., (2007). Field experiments on the effectiveness of ‘eyespots’ as predator deterrents. Animal Behaviour, 74, 12151227.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., George, B., Kruger, A., Farrar, J., & Evans, E. (1985). The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 14, 175186.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Agnetta, B. (1999). Chimpanzees follow gaze direction geometrical “y.” Animal Behaviour, 58, 769777.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states: The question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 153156.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705722.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Koski, S. E., Byrne, R. W., Slocombe, K. E., Bickel, B., Boeckle, M. et al. (2017). Exorcising Grice’s ghost: An empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological Reviews, 92(3), 14271433.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex among Apes. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1993). Reconciliation among Primates: A Review of Empirical Evidence and Theoretical Issues. In Mason, W. A. & Mendoza, S. P. (Eds.), Primate Social Conflict (pp. 111144). New York: State University New York Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (1994). Grades of Mindreading. In Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P. (Eds.), Children’s Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development (pp. 4770). Hillsdale, MI: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Byrne, R. W. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(2), 233273.Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Wikberg, E., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2007). Kin-mediated reconciliation substitutes for direct reconciliation in female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B, 274, 11091115.Google Scholar
Wood, E. K. & Higley, J. D. (2018) Attachment. In Vonk, J. & Shackelford, T. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior. Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (2005). The infant origins of intentional understanding. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 33, 229262.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, F., Zemke, F., Call, J., & Gómez, J. C. (2009). Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) point to inform a human about the location of a tool. Animal Cognition, 12, 347358.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000a). Referential vocalizations in wild diana monkeys. Animal Behavior, 59, 917927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000b). Interspecies semantic communication in two forest monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B, 267, 713718.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2008). Audience effects. Current Biology, 18(5), R189eR190.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. & Gómez, J. C. (2018). Communication, Primate Intentional. In Callan, H. (Ed.), The International Encyclopedia of Anthropology (pp. 110). Hoboken, NJ: Cheney.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×