Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T08:42:16.631Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Dynamical systems and embedded cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2014

Randall D. Beer
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Keith Frankish
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
William M. Ramsey
Affiliation:
University of Nevada, Las Vegas
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The conceptual frameworks that we bring to our study of cognition can have a tremendous impact on the nature of that study. They provide a set of filters through which we view the world, influencing our choice of phenomena to study, the language in which we describe these phenomena, the questions we ask about them, and our interpretations of the answers we receive. For much of the last fifty years, thinking about thinking has been dominated by the computational framework, the idea that systems are intelligent to the extent that they can encode knowledge in symbolic representations which are then algorithmically manipulated so as to produce solutions to the problems that these systems encounter (see Chapter 4 of this volume). More recently, the connectionist framework forced an important refinement of the computational framework, in which representation and computation could be distributed across a large number of loosely neuron-like units (see Chapter 5).

Beginning around the mid 1980s, just as the popularity of connectionism was rising, another conceptual framework appeared (or, as in the case of connectionism, reappeared) on the scene. This framework, which, for want of a catchier label, I will call the situated, embodied, dynamical (SED) framework, focuses on concrete action and emphasizes the way in which an agent’s behavior arises from the dynamical interaction between its brain, its body, and its environment. In this chapter, I will attempt to trace some of the history of the individual intellectual threads of situated activity, embodiment, and dynamics that underlie the SED approach. I will particularly focus on the years 1985–1995. Although there were important precursors to the SED approach (some of which I will briefly mention), and work in this area has grown rapidly in recent years, many of the pivotal ideas were first given their modern form during this ten-year period.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. An early philosophical treatment of situated, embodied and dynamical approaches to cognition.Google Scholar
Nolfi, S. and Floreano, D. (2000). Evolutionary Robotics: The Biology, Intelligence, and Technology of Self-Organizing Machines. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. A comprehensive overview of the use of evolutionary algorithms to produce control systems for model agents and robots.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, R. and Bongard, J. (2006). How the Body Shapes the Way We Think: A New View of Intelligence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. This book provides a gentle introduction to the crucial role that embodiment plays in cognition.Google Scholar
Port, R. F. and van Gelder, T. (eds.) (1995). Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. An early collection of papers on the dynamical approach to cognition, with contributions from most of the major players.Google Scholar
Spivey, M. (2007). The Continuity of Mind. New York: Oxford University Press. This book assembles an impressive array of behavioral and neurophysiological evidence that demonstrates the many ways in which continuous processes play an essential role in cognition.Google Scholar
Abraham, R. H. and Shaw, C. D. (1992). Dynamics: The Geometry of Behavior (2nd edn.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and Chapman, D. (1987). Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity, in Proceedings of the 6th National Conference on AI (AAAI’87) (pp. 268–72). Seattle, WA: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. L. (2003). Embodied cognition: A field guide, Artificial Intelligence 149: 91–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (1987). Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10: 407–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arkin, R. C. (1998). Behavior-Based Robotics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ashby, W. R. (1960). Design for a Brain: The Origin of Adaptive Behaviour (2nd edn.). New York: John Wiley and Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1991). Animate vision, Artificial Intelligence 48: 57–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W. (1998). Representations and cognitive explanations: Assessing the dynamicist’s challenge in cognitive science, Cognitive Science 22: 295–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, R. D. (1990). Intelligence as Adaptive Behavior: An Experiment in Computational Neuroethology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Beer, R. D. (1995a). A dynamical systems perspective on agent-environment interaction, Artificial Intelligence 72: 173–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, R. D. (1995b). Computational and dynamical languages for autonomous agents. In Port, R. F. and van Gelder, T. (eds.), Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition (pp. 121–47). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Beer, R. D. (1998). Framing the debate between computational and dynamical approaches to cognitive science, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, R. D. (2000). Dynamical approaches to cognitive science, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 91–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beer, R. D. (2003). The dynamics of active categorical perception in an evolved model agent (with commentary and response), Adaptive Behavior 11: 209–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, R. D. (2004). Autopoiesis and cognition in the game of life, Artificial Life 10: 309–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beer, R. D. and Gallagher, J. C. (1992). Evolving dynamical neural networks for adaptive behavior, Adaptive Behavior 1: 91–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braitenburg, V. (1984). Vehicles: Experiments in Synthetic Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A. (1986). A robust layered control system for a mobile robot, IEEE Journal of Robotics and Automation 2: 14–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. A. (1991a). Intelligence without representation, Artificial Intelligence 47: 139–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. A. (1991b). New approaches to robotics, Science 253: 1227–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiel, H. J. and Beer, R. D. (1997). The brain has a body: Adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment, Trends in Neurosciences 20: 553–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. S., Ramachandran, V. S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1994). A critique of pure vision, in Koch, C. and Davis, J. L. (eds.), Large-Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain (pp. 23–60). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clancey, W. J. (1997). Situated Cognition: On Human Knowledge and Computer Representations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. and Chalmers, D. J. (1998). The extended mind, Analysis 58: 7–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cliff, D. (1991). Computational neuroethology: A provisional manifesto, in Meyer, J. A. and Wilson, S. W. (eds.), From Animals to Animats: Proceedings of the First International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior (pp. 29–39). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cliff, D., Husbands, P., and Harvey, I. (1993). Explorations in evolutionary robotics, Adaptive Behavior 2: 73–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Paolo, E. A. (2000). Behavioral coordination, structural congruence and entrainment in a simulation of acoustically coupled agents, Adaptive Behavior 8: 27–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Paolo, E. A (2005). Autopoiesis, adaptivity, teleology, agency, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 4: 429–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Paolo, E. A. and Harvey, I. (2003). Decisions and noise: The scope of evolutionary synthesis and dynamical analysis, Adaptive Behavior 11: 284–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L. (1972/1992). What Computers Still Can’t Do: A Critique of Artificial Reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Revised edition of What Computers Can’t Do, 1972.)Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M., Reeke, G. N., Gall, W. E., Tononi, G., Williams, D., and Sporns, O. (1992). Synthetic neural modeling applied to a real-world artifact, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 89: 7267–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eliasmith, C. (1997). Computation and dynamical models of mind, Minds and Machines 7: 531–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1995). Language as a dynamical system, in Port, R. and van Gelder, T. (eds.), Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition (pp. 195–225). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Floreano, D. and Mondada, F. (1996). Evolution of plastic neurocontrollers for situated agents, in Maes, P., Mataric, M. J., Meyer, J.-A., Pollack, J., and Wilson, S. W. (eds.), From Animals to Animats 4: Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior (pp. 402–10). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1969). Embedding fields: A theory of learning with physiological implications, Journal of Mathematical Psychology 6: 209–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grush, R. (1997). Review of Port and van Gelder’s Mind as Motion, Philosophical Psychology 10: 233–42.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem, Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena 42: 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, I. (1992/1996). Untimed and misrepresented: Connectionism and the computer metaphor, AISB Quarterly 96: 20–7. Originally published 1992 as University of Sussex Cognitive Science Research Paper 245.Google Scholar
Hayes, P. J., Ford, K. M., and Agnew, N. (1994). On babies and bathwater: A cautionary tale. AI Magazine 15(4): 15–26.Google Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1927/1962). Being and Time. New York: Harper and Row. Originally published in 1927.Google Scholar
Husbands, P., Harvey, I., and Cliff, D. (1995). Circle in the round: State space attractors for evolved sighted robots, Robotics and Autonomous Systems 15: 83–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Izhikevich, E. M. (2007). Dynamical Systems in Neuroscience: The Geometry of Excitability and Bursting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Izquierdo-Torres, E. and Harvey, I. (2006). Learning on a continuum in evolved dynamical node networks. In Rocha, L. M., Yaeger, L. S., Bedau, M. A., et al. (eds.), Artificial Life X: Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on the Simulation and Synthesis of Living Systems (pp. 507–12). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kirsch, D. (1991). Today the earwig, tomorrow man? Artificial Intelligence 47: 161–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kodjabachian, J. and Meyer, J.-A. (1998). Evolution and development of neural controllers for locomotion, gradient-following, and obstacle avoidance in artificial insects, IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks 9: 796–812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kugler, P. N, Kelso, J. A. S., and Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures, in Stelmach, G. E. and Requin, J. (eds.), Tutorials in Motor Behavior (pp. 3–47). Amsterdam: North Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langton, C. G. (ed.) (1989). Artificial Life. Reading MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Maes, P., (ed.) (1990). Designing Autonomous Agents: Theory and Practice from Biology to Engineering and Back. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marocco, D., Cangelosi, A., and Nolfi, S. (2003). The emergence of communication in evolutionary robots, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London A 361: 2397–421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maturana, H. R. and Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of Perception. New York: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Noë, A. (2004). Action in Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nolfi, S. (2005). Emergence of communication in embodied agents: Co-adapting communicative and non-communicative behaviors, Connection Science 17: 231–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolfi, S. and Floreano, D. (2000). Evolutionary Robotics: The Biology, Intelligence, and Technology of Self-Organizing Machines. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, R. and Scheier, C. (1999). Understanding Intelligence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Port, R. F. and van Gelder, T. (eds.) (1995). Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rosenschein, S. J. and Kaelbling, L. P. (1986). The synthesis of digital machines with provable epistemic properties, in Halpern, J. Y. (ed.), Proceedings of the Conference on Theoretical Aspects of Reasoning about Knowledge (pp. 83–98). Los Altos, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schöner, G., Dose, M., and Engels, C. (1995). Dynamics of behavior: Theory and applications for autonomous robot architectures. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 16: 213–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skarda, C. A. and Freeman, W. J. (1987). How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10: 161–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smithers, T. (1995). Are autonomous agents information processing systems?, in Steels, L. and Brooks, R. (eds.), The Artificial Life Route to Artificial Intelligence (pp. 123–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Steels, L. (2003) Evolving grounded communication for robots, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 308–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strogatz, S. H. (1994). Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human–Machine Communication. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., and Smith, L. B. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant preservative reaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 1–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. and Smith, L. B. (1994). A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tuci, E., Quinn, M., and Harvey, I. (2002). An evolutionary ecological approach to the study of learning behavior using a robot-based model, Adaptive Behavior 10: 201–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. S. (2000). The Extended Organism: The Physiology of Animal-Built Structures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1990). Coordination, American Psychologist 45: 938–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Gelder, T. (1995). What might cognition be, if not computation?, Journal of Philosophy 91: 345–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Gelder, T. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 615–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Leeuwen, M. (2005). Questions for the dynamicist: The use of dynamical systems theory in the philosophy of cognition, Minds and Machines 15: 271–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vera, A. H. and Simon, H. A. (1993). Situated action: A symbolic interpretation, Cognitive Science 17: 7–48 (with commentary and responses).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vickerstaff, R. J. and Di Paolo, E. A. (2005). Evolving neural models of path integration, Journal of Experimental Biology 208: 3349–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walter, W. G. (1953). The Living Brain. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Ward, R. and Ward, R. (2006). Cognitive conflict without explicit conflict monitoring in a dynamical agent, Neural Networks 19: 1430–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warren, W. H. (2006). The dynamics of perception and action, Psychological Review 113: 358–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, M. (2005). Reconstructing the Cognitive World: The Next Step. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Winograd, T. and Flores, F. (1986). Understanding Computers and Cognition: A New Foundation for Design. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Yamauchi, B. and Beer, R. D. (1994). Sequential behavior and learning in evolved dynamical neural networks. Adaptive Behavior 2: 219–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×