Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:00:39.710Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Cognitive Abilities in Elephants

from Part I - The Comparative Approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Everybody knows elephants are intelligent – everybody, that is, except for evolutionary psychologists. The popular characterization of these large, long-lived, very social mammals is that they have fantastic memories and are considered somehow “special,” but the scientific evidence behind this reputation is somewhat lacking, with there having been relatively few attempts to study elephant cognition during the twentieth century. Fewer than 20 manuscripts detailing novel studies of elephant cognitive abilities had been published by the end of the first decade of this millennium (Byrne, Bates, & Moss, 2009), though a gradually increasing research effort is now resulting in progress.

So why, as psychologists, are we even interested in elephants, animals with which we have not shared a common ancestor for around 105 million years (Hedges, 2001; Murphy et al., 2001)?

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. (2011). Which primates recognize themselves in mirrors? PLoS Biology, 9(3), 24.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Gillies, A., & Lock, L. C. (2010). Pan thanatology. Current Biology, 20(8), 349351.Google Scholar
Archie, E. A., Moss, C. J., & Alberts, S. C. (2006). The ties that bind: Genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 513522.Google ScholarPubMed
Arvidsson, J., Amundin, M., & Laska, M. (2012). Successful acquisition of an olfactory discrimination test by Asian elephants, Elephas maximus. Physiology and Behavior, 105(3), 809814.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A. (2018). Elephants – Studying cognition in the African savannah. In Bueno-Guerra, N. & Amici, F., eds., Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition, A Comparative Guide. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 177198.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A., & Byrne, R. W. (2015). Primate social cognition: What we have learned from nonhuman primates and other animals. In Mikulincer, M. & Shaver, P. R., APA Handbook Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 1. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 4778.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A., Sayialel, K. N., Njiraini, N. W., et al. (2007). Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Current Biology, 17(22), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, L. A., Sayialel, K. N., Njiraini, N. W., et al. (2008a). African elephants have expectations about the locations of out-of-sight family members. Biology Letters, 4(1), 3436.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A., Lee, P. C., Njiraini, N., et al. (2008b). Do elephants show empathy? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(10–11), 204225.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A., Handford, R., Lee, P. C., et al. (2010). Why do African elephants (Loxodonta africana) simulate oestrus? An analysis of longitudinal data. PLoS ONE, 5(4), e10052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, S., Bouché, P., Rasmussen, H., Orlando, A., & Douglas-Hamilton, I. (2003). The Last Sahelian Elephants: Ranging Behavior, Population Status and Recent History of the Desert Elephants of Mali. Nairobi: Save the Elephants.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 54, 8699.Google Scholar
Brownell, C. A., Zerwas, S., & Ramani, G. B. (2007). So big: The development of body self-awareness in toddlers. Child Development, 78(5), 14261440.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. A. (2010). Primate social cognition: Uniquely primate, uniquely social, or just unique? Neuron, 65(6), 815830.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Bates, L. A., & Moss, C. J. (2009). Elephant cognition in primate perspective. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 4, 115.Google Scholar
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S., & Liska, J. (1993). Tool use by wild and captive elephants. Animal Behaviour, 46, 209219.Google Scholar
Chiyo, P. I., Archie, E. A., Hollister-Smith, J. A., et al. (2011). Association patterns of African elephants in all-male groups: The role of age and genetic relatedness. Animal Behaviour, 81(6), 10931099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cozzi, B., Spagnoli, S., & Bruno, L. (2001). An overview of the central nervous system of the elephant through a critical appraisal of the literature published in the XIX and XX centuries. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(2), 219227.Google Scholar
Dale, R., & Plotnik, J. M. (2017). Elephants know when their bodies are obstacles to success in a novel transfer task. Scientific Reports, 7, 46309.Google Scholar
de Silva, S., & Wittemyer, G. (2012). A comparison of social organization in Asian elephants and African savannah elephants. International Journal of Primatology, 33(5), 11251141.Google Scholar
Douglas-Hamilton, I., Bhalla, S., Wittemyer, G., & Vollrath, F. (2006). Behavioural reactions of elephants towards a dying and deceased matriarch. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 100(1–2), 87102.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178190.Google Scholar
Fishlock, V., Caldwell, C., & Lee, P. C. (2016). Elephant resource-use traditions. Animal Cognition, 19(2), 429433.Google Scholar
Foerder, P., Galloway, M., Barthel, T., Moore, D. E., & Reiss, D. (2011). Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant. PLoS ONE, 6(8), e23251.Google Scholar
Foley, C., Pettorelli, N., & Foley, L. (2008). Severe drought and calf survival in elephants. Biology Letters, 4(5), 541544.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 8687.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, S. Z., Douglas-Hamilton, I., & Wittemyer, G. (2016). Vertical transmission of social roles drives resilience to poaching in elephant networks. Current Biology, 26(1), 7579.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Greco, B. J., Brown, T. K., Andrews, J. R. M., Swaisgood, R. R., & Caine, N. G. (2013). Social learning in captive African elephants (Loxodonta africana africana). Animal Cognition, 16(3), 459469.Google Scholar
Hakeem, A. Y., Hof, P. R., Sherwood, C. C., et al. (2005). Brain of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana): Neuroanatomy from magnetic resonance images. The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary Biology, 287(1), 11171127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B. L., & Hart, L. A. (1994). Fly switching by Asian elephants: Tool use to control parasites. Animal Behaviour, 48, 3545.Google Scholar
Hart, B., Hart, L. A., McCoy, M., & Sarath, C. (2001). Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: Use and modification of branches for fly switching. Animal Behaviour, 62, 839847.Google Scholar
Hart, B. L., Hart, L. A., & Pinter-Wollman, N. (2008). Large brains and cognition: Where do elephants fit in? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(1), 8698.Google Scholar
Hedges, S. B. (2001). Afrotheria: Plate tectonics meets genomics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(1), 12.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Reflections on self-recognition in primates. Animal Behaviour, 47, 909919.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M., & Street, G. (1995). Self-recognition in primates: Further reflections create a hall of mirrors. Animal Behaviour, 50, 15331542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. (2000). Human-like, population-level specialization in the manufacture of Pandanus tools by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloids. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 267, 403413.Google Scholar
Irie-Sugimoto, N., Kobayashi, T., Sato, T., & Hasegawa, T. (2009). Relative quantity judgment by Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Animal Cognition, 12(1), 193199.Google Scholar
Jerison, H. (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ketchaisri, O., Siripunkaw, C., & Plotnik, J. M. (2019). The use of a human’s location and social cues by Asian elephants in an object-choice task. Animal Cognition. doi:10.1007/s10071-019-01283-0.Google Scholar
Lahdenperä, M., Mar, K. U., & Lummaa, V. (2016). Nearby grandmother enhances calf survival and reproduction in Asian elephants. Scientific Reports, 6: 27213.Google Scholar
Lee, P. (1987). Allomothering among African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 35(1), 278291.Google Scholar
Lee, P. C., & Moss, C. J. (1999). The social context for learning and behavioural development among wild African elephants. In Box, H. & Gibson, K., eds., Mammalian Social Learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 102125.Google Scholar
Lee, P. C., & Moss, C. J. (2014). African elephant play, competence and social complexity. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2(2), 144.Google Scholar
Lee, P. C., Poole, J. H., Njiraini, N., Sayialel, K. N., & Moss, C. J. (2011). Male social dynamics: Independence and beyond. In Moss, C. J., Croze, H., & Lee, P. C., eds., The Amboseli Elephants. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press, pp. 260271.Google Scholar
Leggett, K. E. A. (2006). Home range and seasonal movement of elephants in the Kunene Region, northwestern Namibia. African Zoology, 41(1), 1736.Google Scholar
McComb, K., Moss, C., Sayialel, S., & Baker, L. (2000). Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 59(6), 11031109.Google Scholar
McComb, K., Moss, C., Durant, S. M., Baker, L., & Sayialel, S. (2001). Matriarchs act as repositories of social knowledge in African elephants. Science, 292(5516), 491494.Google Scholar
McComb, K., Reby, D., Baker, L., Moss, C., & Sayialel, S. (2003). Long-distance communication of acoustic cues to social identity in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 65(2), 317329.Google Scholar
McComb, K., Baker, L., & Moss, C. (2006). African elephants show high levels of interest in the skulls and ivory of their own species. Biology Letters, 2(1), 2628.Google Scholar
McComb, K., Shannon, G., Durant, S. M., et al. (2011). Leadership in elephants: The adaptive value of age. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1722), 32703276.Google Scholar
McComb, K., Shannon, G., Sayialel, K. N., & Moss, C. (2014). Elephants can determine ethnicity, gender, and age from acoustic cues in human voices. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(14), 54335438.Google Scholar
Miller, A. K., Hensman, M. C., Hensman, S., et al. (2015). African elephants (Loxodonta africana) can detect TNT using olfaction: Implications for biosensor application. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 171, 177183.Google Scholar
Mizuno, K., Irie, N., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., & Kutsukake, N. (2016). Asian elephants acquire inaccessible food by blowing. Animal Cognition, 19(1), 215222.Google Scholar
Moore, C., Mealiea, J., Garon, N., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). The development of body self-awareness. Infancy, 11, 157174.Google Scholar
Moss, C. J., & Lee, P. C. (2011). Female social dynamics: Fidelity and flexibility. In Moss, C. J., Croze, H., & Lee, P. C., eds., The Amboseli Elephants. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press, pp. 205223.Google Scholar
Moss, C. J., & Poole, J. H. (1983). Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In Hinde, R., ed., Primate Social Relationships: An Integrated Approach. Oxford: Blackwells, pp. 314325.Google Scholar
Murphy, W. J., Eizirik, E., O’Brien, S., et al. (2001). Resolution of the early placental mammal radiation using Bayesian phylogenetics. Science, 294, 23482351.Google Scholar
Nissani, M. (2006). Do Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) apply causal reasoning to tool-use tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 32(1), 91–6.Google Scholar
O’Connell-Rodwell, C. E., Wood, J. D., Kinzley, C., et al. (2011). Male African elephants (Loxodonta africana) queue when the stakes are high. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 23, 388397.Google Scholar
Perdue, B. M., Talbot, C. F., Stone, A. M., & Beran, M. J. (2012). Putting the elephant back in the herd: Elephant relative quantity judgments match those of other species. Animal Cognition, 15(5), 955961.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2014). Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) reassure others in distress. PeerJ, 2, e278.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M., & Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(45), 1705317057.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M., Moore, D., & Reiss, D. (2010). Self-recognition in the Asian elephant and future directions for cognitive research with elephants in zoological settings. Zoo Biology, 29(2), 179191.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., Lair, R., Suphachoksahakun, W., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2011). Elephants know when they need a helping trunk in a cooperative task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(12), 51165121.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., Pokorny, J. J., Keratimanochaya, T., et al. (2013). Visual cues given by humans are not sufficient for Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) to find hidden food. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e61174.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., Shaw, R. C., Brubaker, D. L., Tiller, L. N., & Clayton, N. S. (2014). Thinking with their trunks: Elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude nonrewarding alternatives. Animal Behaviour, 88, 9198.Google Scholar
Polansky, L., Kilian, W., & Wittemyer, G. (2015). Elucidating the significance of spatial memory on movement decisions by African savannah elephants using state-space models. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1805), 20143042.Google Scholar
Poole, J. H. (1987). Rutting behaviour in African elephants: The phenomenon of musth. Behaviour, 102, 283316.Google Scholar
Poole, J. H. (1989a). Announcing intent: The aggressive state of musth in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 37, 140152.Google Scholar
Poole, J. H. (1989b). Mate guarding, reproductive success and female choice in African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 37, 842849.Google Scholar
Poole, J. H., & Moss, C. J. (1981). Musth in the African elephant Loxodonta africana. Nature, 292, 830831.Google Scholar
Poole, J. H., Tyack, P. L., Stoeger-Horwath, A. S., & Watwood, S. (2005). Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature, 434, 455456.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. (1989). Failure to find self-recognition in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in contrast to their use of mirror cues to discover hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103(2), 122131.Google Scholar
Prior, H., Schwarz, A., & Gunturkun, O. (2008). Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biology, 6(8), e202.Google Scholar
Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(10), 59375942.Google Scholar
Rensch, B. (1957). The intelligence of elephants. Scientific American, 196(2), 4449.Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Call, J., & Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters, 5(3), 293296.Google Scholar
Shoshani, J., Kupsky, W. J., & Marchant, G. H. (2006). Elephant brain. Part I: Gross morphology, functions, comparative anatomy, and evolution. Brain Research Bulletin, 70(2), 124157.Google Scholar
Smet, A. F., & Byrne, R. W. (2013). African elephants can use human pointing cues to find hidden food. Current Biology, 23(20), 20332037.Google Scholar
Smet, A. F., & Byrne, R. W. (2014a). African elephants (Loxodonta africana) recognize visual attention from face and body orientation. Biology Letters, 10, 20140428.Google Scholar
Smet, A. F., & Byrne, R. W. (2014b). Interpretation of human pointing by African elephants: Generalisation and rationality. Animal Cognition, 17(6), 13651374.Google Scholar
Soltis, J., King, L. E., Douglas-Hamilton, I., Vollrath, F., & Savage, A. (2014). African elephant alarm calls distinguish between threats from humans and bees. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89403.Google Scholar
Stoeger, A. S., & Baotic, A. (2017). Male African elephants discriminate and prefer vocalizations of unfamiliar females. Scientific Reports, 7, 110.Google Scholar
Stoeger, A. S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., et al. (2012). An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22(22), 21442148.Google Scholar
Thuppil, V., & Coss, R. G. (2013). Wild Asian elephants distinguish aggressive tiger and leopard growls according to perceived danger. Biology Letters, 9(5), 20130518.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 187194.Google Scholar
Wittemyer, G., Douglas-Hamilton, I., & Getz, W. M. (2005). The socioecology of elephants: Analysis of the processes creating multitiered social structures. Animal Behaviour, 69(6), 13571371.Google Scholar
Wittemyer, G., Okello, J. B. A., Rasmussen, H. B., et al. (2009). Where sociality and relatedness diverge: The genetic basis for hierarchical social organization in African elephants. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 276(1672), 35133521.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×