Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:07:48.383Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Implicit Bias and Discrimination: Evidence on Causality

from Section II - Do Measures of Implicit Bias Predict Cognition and Behavior?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2024

Jon A. Krosnick
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Tobias H. Stark
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Amanda L. Scott
Affiliation:
The Strategy Team, Columbus, Ohio
Get access

Summary

There are widespread assumptions that implicit group bias leads to biased behavior. This chapter summarizes existing evidence on the link between implicit group bias and biased behavior, with an analysis of the strength of that evidence for causality. Our review leads to the conclusion that although there is substantial evidence that implicit group bias is related to biased behavior, claims about causality are not currently supported. With plausible alternative explanations for observed associations, as well as the possibility of reverse causation, scientists and policy makers need to be careful about claims made and actions taken to address discrimination, based on the assumption that implicit bias is the problem.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agerström, J., & Rooth, D. O. (2011). The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 790805. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0021594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Association of Medical Colleges. Unconscious bias resources for health professionals. (2019). Retrieved from: www.aamc.org/initiatives/diversity/322996/lablearningonunconsciousbias.htmlGoogle Scholar
Amodio, D. M., & Devine, P. G. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 652661. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, R M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). Moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 11731182. http://doi.org/10.1037.0022–3514.41.5.1173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., et al. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323370. http://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Brendan more employable than Latoya and Tyrone? Evidence on racial discrimination in the labor market from a large randomized experiment. American Economic Review, 94(4), 9921013. http://doi.org/10.1257/0002828042002561CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bessenoff, G. R., & Sherman, J. W. (2000). Automatic and controlled components of prejudice toward fat people: Evaluation versus stereotype activation. Social Cognition, 18(4), 329353. http://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2000.18.4.329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, I. V., Havranek, E. P., Price, D. W., et al. (2013). Assessment of biases against Latinos and African Americans among primary care providers and community membersAmerican Journal of Public Health103(1), 9298. http:///doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2012.300812CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, F. A., Crandall, C. S., Brigham, J. C., et al. (1994). Condemning and condoning racism: A social context approach to interracial settings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(6), 993997. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.79.6.993CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, F. A., Lilly, T., & Vaughn, L. A. (1991). Reducing the expression of racial prejudice. Psychological Science, 2(2), 101105. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00108.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, M. D., Monteith, M. J., & Parker, L. R. (2017). Training away bias: The differential effects of counterstereotype training and self-regulation on stereotype activation and applicationJournal of Experimental Social Psychology73, 97110. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.06.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition a meta-analysis of associations with behavior and explicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(4), 330350. http://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312440047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlsson, R., & Agerström, J. (2016). A closer look at the discrimination outcomes in the IAT literatureScandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57(4), 278287. http://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castelli, L., Carraro, L., Pavan, G., et al. (2012). The power of the unsaid: The influence of nonverbal cues on implicit attitudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(6), 13761393. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00903.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltrane, S., & Messineo, M. (2000). The perpetuation of subtle prejudice: Race and gender imagery in 1990s television advertising. Sex Roles, 42(5–6), 363389. http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007046204478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crandall, C. S., Eshleman, A., & O’Brien, L. (2002). Social norms and the expression and suppression of prejudice: The struggle for internalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(3), 359378. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 800814. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.800CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasgupta, N., & Rivera, L. M. (2008). When social context matters: The influence of long-term contact and short-term exposure to admired outgroup members on implicit attitudes and behavioral intentionsSocial Cognition26(1), 112123. http://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.1.112Google Scholar
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S., Quinn, D. M., et al. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 16151628. http://doi.org/10.1177/014616702237644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 518. https://doi.org/libraries.colorado.edu/10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. S., et al. (2012). Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 12671278. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.%202012.06.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2010) Intergroup bias. In S. T. Fiske, , D. T. Gilbert, , & G. Lindzey, (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (5th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons, Inc., pp. 10841121.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. E., Kawakami, K., et al. (2002). Why can’t we just get along? Interpersonal biases and interracial distrustCultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology8(2), 88102. http://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.8.2.88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes, 82(1), 6268. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62Google ScholarPubMed
Entman, R. M. (1994). Representation and reality in the portrayal of Blacks on network television news. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 71(3), 509520. http://doi.org/10.1177/107769909407100303Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 297327. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. In Chaiken, S. & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual Process Theories in Social Pscyhology. New York, NY: The Guilford Press, pp. 97116.Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., & Molm, L. D. (2010). Bridging inequality from both sides nowSocial Psychology Quarterly73(4), 341346. http://doi.org/10.1177/0190272510389007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Florack, A., Scarabis, M., & Bless, H. (2001). When do associations matter? The use of automatic associations toward ethnic groups in person judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37(6), 518524. http://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1477CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forscher, P. S., Lai, C. K., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measuresJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(3), 522559. http://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabriel, U., Banse, R., & Hug, F. (2007). Predicting private and public helping behaviour by implicit attitudes and the motivation to control prejudiced reactions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46(2), 365382. http://doi.org/10.1348/014466606X120400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692731. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonsalkorale, K., Hippel, W. von Sherman, J. W., et al. (2009). Bias and regulation of bias in intergroup interactions: Implicit attitudes toward Muslims and interaction quality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 161166. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, A. R., Carney, D. R., Pallin, D. J., et al. (2007). Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombosis decisions for Black and White patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22(9), 12311238. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11606–007-0258-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J., & Kosloff, S. (2008). Terror management theory: Implications for understanding prejudice, stereotyping, intergroup conflict, and political attitudesSocial and Personality Psychology Compass2(5), 18811894. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00144.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 427. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, A. T., Uhlmann, E. L., et al. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 1741. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Tucker Smith, C., Sriram, N., et al. (2009). Implicit race attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 U.S. presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 9(1), 341253. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-2415.2009.01195.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Castelli, L., et al. (2008). Implicit and explicit attitudes and interracial interaction: The moderating role of situationally available control resources. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11(1), 6987. http://doi.org/10.1177/1368430207084847CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, P. W. (1986). Statistics and causal inference. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 81(396), 945960. http://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1986.10478354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollands, G., Prestwich, A., & Marteau, M. (2011). Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices and implicit attitudes: An experimental test of evaluative conditioning. Health Psychology, 30, 195203. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0022261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houben, K., Havermans, , R. Nederkoom, C., C., et al. (2012). Beer a no-go: Learning to stop responding to alcohol cues reduces alcohol intake via reduced affective associations rather than increased response inhibition. Addiction, 107, 12801287. http://doi.org/10.llllIj.J360–0443.201203827.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby-Senghor, D. S., Sinclair, S., & Shelton, J. N. (2016). A lesson in bias: The relationship between implicit racial bias and performance in pedagogical contextsJournal of Experimental Social Psychology63, 5055. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 127. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25(6), 881919. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., & Carvallo, M. R. (2002). Non-conscious forms of system justification: Implicit and behavioral preferences for higher status groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 586602. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022–1031(02)00505-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., et al. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 871888. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., & van Kamp, S. (2005). Kicking the habit: Effects of nonstereotypic association training and correction processes on hiring decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 6875. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawakami, K., Phills, C. E., Steele, J. R., et al. (2007). (Close) distance makes the heart grow fonder: Improving implicit racial attitudes and interracial interactions through approach behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 957971. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysisAmerican Psychologist74(5), 569. http://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000364CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K., Hoffman, K. M., & Nosek, B. A. (2013). Reducing implicit prejudice. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(5), 315330. http://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, A. J., Payne, B. K., Ramsey, S., et al. (2005). On the predictive validity of implicit attitude measures: The moderating effect of perceived group variability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 114128. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, S., Federico, C. M., Sidanius, J., et al. (2002). Social dominance orientation and intergroup bias: The legitimation of favoritism for high-status groupsPersonality and Social Psychology Bulletin28(2), 144157. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202282002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loersch, C. & Payne, B. K. (2011). The situated inference model: An integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 234252. http://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406921CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudiceJournal of Personality and Social Psychology81(5), 842855. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.842CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, N. H., & Kawakami, K. (2012). The long, steep path to equality: Progressing on egalitarian goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 187197. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0025602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsden, P. V. (2012). Social Trends in American Life: Findings from the General Social Survey since 1972. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Monteith, M. J., Deneen, N. E., & Tooman, G. D. (1996). The effect of social norm activation on the expression of opinions concerning gay men and Blacks. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18(3), 267288. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1803_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, R., Hülsenbeck, K., & Seibt, B. (2004). Attitudes towards people with AIDS and avoidance behavior: Automatic and reflective bases of behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(4), 543550. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web siteGroup Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice6(1), 101115. http://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Hawkins, C. B., & Frazier, R. S. (2011). Implicit social cognition: From measures to mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 152159. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, K. S., Hunter, J. A., & Banks, M. (2007). Implicit anti-fat bias in physical educators: Physical attributes, ideology and socializationInternational Journal of Obesity31(2), 308314. http://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803398CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Trait Inferences as a function of automatically activated racial attitudes and motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26(1), 111. http://doi.org/10.1206/s15324834basp2601_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2007). Discordant evaluations of Blacks affect nonverbal behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (online). http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207303023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2015). Using the IAT to predict ethnic and racial discrimination: Small effect sizes of unknown societal significance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 562571. http://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pager, D., & Shepherd, H. (2008). The sociology of discrimination: Racial discrimination in employment, housing, credit, and consumer markets. Annual Review of Sociology, 34, 181209. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.33.040406.131740CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Krosnick, J. A., Pasek, J., et al. (2010). Implicit and explicit prejudice in the 2008 American presidential election. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 367374. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, A. R., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2009). The nature of contemporary prejudice: Insights from aversive racism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(3), 314338. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00183.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, E. R., Rubie-Davies, C., Osborne, D., et al. (2016). Teachers’ explicit expectations and implicit prejudiced attitudes to educational achievement: Relations with student achievement and the ethnic achievement gapLearning and Instruction42, 123140. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.01.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup Contact Theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1), 6585. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theoryJournal of Personality and Social Psychology90(5), 751783. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F., Tropp, L. R., Wagner, U., et al. (2011). Recent advances in intergroup contact theoryInternational Journal of Intercultural Relations35(3), 271280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratto, F., & Shih, M. (2000). Social dominance orientation and group context in implicit group prejudicePsychological Science11(6), 515518. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., Wesselmann, E. D., et al. (2013). The influence of social norms upon behavioral expressions of implicit and explicit weight-related stigma in an interactive game. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 86, 189201.Google Scholar
Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test: Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 139165. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowatt, W. C., Franklin, L. M., & Cotton, M. (2005). Patterns and personality correlates of implicit and explicit attitudes toward Christians and MuslimsJournal for the Scientific Study of Religion44(1), 2943. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5906.2005.00263.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 7982. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00279.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., & Ashmore, R. D. (2007). Discrimination and the Implicit Association Test. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(3), 359372. http://doi.org/10.1177/1368430207078696CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., Feinberg, J., & Fairchild, K. (2002). Minority members’ implicit attitudes: Automatic ingroup bias as a function of group status. Social Cognition, 20(4), 294320. http://doi.org/10.1521/soco.20.4.294.19908CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., & Lee, M. R. (2002). Implicit and explicit consequences of exposure to violent and misogynous rap musicGroup Processes & Intergroup Relations5(2), 133150. http://doi.org/10.1177/1368430202005002541CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of priming gender roles on women’s implicit gender beliefs and career aspirationsSocial Psychology, 41(3), 192202. http://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuman, H., Steeh, C., Bobo, L., et al. (1997). Racial Attitudes in America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M. B., Chambliss, H. O. N., Brownell, K. D., et al. (2003). Weight bias among health professionals specializing in obesityObesity Research11(9), 10331039. http://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2003.142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sechrist, G. B., & Stangor, C. (2001). Perceived consensus influences intergroup behavior and stereotype accessibilityJournal of Personality and Social Psychology80(4), 645654. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.645CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., & Mitchell, M. (1994). In-group identification, social dominance orientation, and differential intergroup social allocationJournal of Social Psychology134(2), 151167. http://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1994.9711378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, R. (2018). Starbucks’s bias training finally happened. Here’s what it looked like. The Washington Post. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/news/business/wp/2018/05/29/starbucks-training-finally-happened-heres-what-it-looked-like/Google Scholar
Smith, R. J., & Levinson, J. D. (2011). The impact of implicit racial bias on the exercise of prosecutorial discretionSeattle UL Rev.35, 795826.Google Scholar
Somashekhar, S., Lowery, W., & Alexander, K. L. (2015, August 8). Black and Unarmed. The Washington Post. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/sf/national/2015/08/08/black-and-unarmed/Google Scholar
Spohn, C., & Holleran, D. (2000). Imprisonment penalty paid by young, unemployed Black and Hispanic male offenders, Criminology, 38, 281. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2000.tb00891.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Banaji, M. R., et al. (2011). Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(19), 77107715. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014345108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepanikova, I., Triplett, J., & Simpson, B. (2011). Implicit racial bias and prosocial behavior. Social Science Research, 40, 11861195. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2011.02.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strick, M., van Baaren, R B., Holland, R.W., et al. (2009). Humor in advertisements enhances product liking by mere association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 3545. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014812Google ScholarPubMed
TeachmanB. A.Marker, C. D.Smith-JanikS. B. (2008). Automatic associations and panic disorder: Trajectories of change over the course of treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology76(6), 9881002. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0013113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Towles-Schwen, T., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Automatically activated racial attitudes as predictors of the success of interracial roommate relationships. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(5), 698705. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. A., Ross, S., Galster, G. C., et al. (2002). Discrimination in Metropolitan Housing Markets: National Results from Phase 1 of the Housing Discrimination Study (HDS) (Working paper No. 2002–16). University of Connecticut, Department of Economics. Retrieved from: https://ideas.repec.org/p/uct/uconnp/2002-16.htmlGoogle Scholar
Umphress, E. E., Simmons, A. L., Boswell, W. R., et al. (2008). Managing discrimination in selection: The influence of directives from an authority and social dominance orientationJournal of Applied Psychology93(5), 982993. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.5.982CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United States Department of Justice. Department of Justice announces new department-wide implicit bias training for personnel. (2016). Retrieved from: www.justice.gov/opa/pr/department-justice-announces-new-department-wide-implicit-bias-training-personnelGoogle Scholar
van den Bergh, L., Denessen, E., Hornstra, L., et al. (2010). The implicit prejudiced attitudes of teachers: Relations to teacher expectations and the ethnic achievement gap. American Educational Research Journal, 47(2), 497527. http://doi.org/10.3102/0002831209353594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, N. (2009). Unspoken cultural influence: Exposure to and influence of nonverbal bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6), 11041119. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0015642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisbuch, M., Pauker, K., & Ambady, N. (2009). The subtle transmission of race bias via televised nonverbal behavior. Science, 326(5960), 17111714. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1178358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, F. D., Tienda, M., & Wu, L. (1995). race and unemployment: Labor market experiences of Black and White men, 1968–1988. Work and Occupations, 22(3), 245270. http://doi.org/10.1177/0730888495022003002/CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107(1), 101126. http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.107.1.101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshida, E., Peach, J. M., Zanna, M. P., et al. (2012). Not all automatic associations are created equal: How implicit normative evaluations are distinct from implicit attitudes and uniquely predict meaningful behaviorJournal of Experimental Social Psychology48(3), 694706. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegert, J. C., & Hanges, P. J. (2005). Employment discrimination: The role of implicit attitudes, motivation and a climate for Racial Bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 553562. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.553CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×