Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:02:00.816Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Lessons from Two Decades of Project Implicit

from Section I - What is Implicit Bias and (How) Can We Measure It?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2024

Jon A. Krosnick
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Tobias H. Stark
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Amanda L. Scott
Affiliation:
The Strategy Team, Columbus, Ohio
Get access

Summary

There are many reasons why the implicit bias construct took root in everyday conversation, but one of them is that millions of people have experienced the most commonly used measure of implicit bias – the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) at the Project Implicit websites. Project Implicit is a non-profit organization and international research collaboration between behavioral scientists interested in implicit social cognition. The organization’s primary public contribution is its education websites (https://implicit.harvard.edu) where more than twenty-eight million IATs have been completed. This chapter provides an overview of Project Implicit and the contributions and challenges of more than twenty years of internet-based data collection on implicit attitudes and stereotypes. The first section describes Project Implicit’s history and organizational structure; next, some of the key insights gleaned from the data collected at the Project Implicit websites are reviewed. These include assessment of the pervasiveness and correlates of implicit bias, comparisons across time and by geographic area, and reactions to learning about one’s own implicit bias. Finally, we reflect on some of the challenges of being uniquely situated between academic researchers and the general public, and describe how changing scientific knowledge has changed scientific communication about implicit bias.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axt, J. R., Conway, M. A., Westgate, E. C., et al. (2021). Implicit transgender attitudes independently predict beliefs about gender and transgender people. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(2), 257274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banaji, M. R. (2004). The opposite of a great truth is also true: Homage to koan #7. In Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (Eds.), Perspectivism in Social Psychology: The Yin and Yang of Scientific Progress. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 127140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). A comparative investigation of seven indirect attitude measures. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 668688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Anan, Y., & Vianello, M. (2018). A multi-method multi-trait test of the dual-attitude perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(8), 12641272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bianchi, E. C., Hall, E. V., & Lee, S. (2018). Reexamining the link between economic downturns and racial antipathy: Evidence that prejudice against Blacks rises in recessions. Psychological Science, 29(10), 15841597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(3), 242261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carleton, R. N., Norton, M. P. J., & Asmundson, G. J. (2007). Fearing the unknown: A short version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(1), 105117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, T. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2019). Patterns of implicit and explicit attitudes: I. Long term change and stability from 2007 to 2016. Psychological Science, 30, 174192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chin, M. J., Quinn, D. M., Dhaliwal, T. K., et al. (2020). Bias in the air: A nationwide exploration of teachers’ implicit racial attitudes, aggregate bias, and student outcomes. Educational Researcher, 49(8), 566578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, P., & Evers, E. R. (2020). The bias of individuals (in crowds): Why implicit bias is probably a noisily measured individual-level construct. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(6), 13291345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corneille, O., & Hütter, M. (2020). Implicit? What do you mean? A comprehensive review of the delusive implicitness construct in attitude research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 24(3), 212232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J., Lindgren, K. P., & De Houwer, J. (2021). On the role of (implicit) drinking self identity in alcohol use and problematic drinking: A comparison of five measures. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 35(4), 458471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 12(2), 163170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daumeyer, N. M., Onyeador, I. N., Brown, X., et al. (2019). Consequences of attributing discrimination to implicit vs. explicit bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 84, 103812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In Wiers, R. W. & Stacy, A. W. (Eds.), The Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, pp. 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2014). A propositional model of implicit evaluation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(7), 342353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2019). Implicit bias is behavior: A functional-cognitive perspective on implicit bias. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(5), 835840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dentale, F., Vecchione, M., Ghezzi, V., et al. (2020). Beyond an associative conception of automatic self-evaluation: Applying the Relational Responding Task to measure self-esteem. The Psychological Record, 70(2), 227242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual differences, 47(6), 541546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object–evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25(5), 603637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., et al. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 10131027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1981). Direct experience and attitude–behavior consistency. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 161202). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). When and why do implicit measures predict behaviour? Empirical evidence for the moderating role of opportunity, motivation, and process reliance. European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 285338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friese, M., Smith, C. T., Plischke, T., et al. (2012). Do implicit attitudes predict actual voting behavior particularly for undecided voters?. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e44130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B. (2019). Six lessons for a cogent science of implicit bias and its criticism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(4), 574595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2011). The associative–propositional evaluation model: Theory, evidence, and open questions. In Gawronski, B. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 59127). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gawronski, B., Hofmann, W., & Wilbur, C. J. (2006). Are “implicit” attitudes unconscious? Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 485499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Morrison, M., Phills, C. E., et al. (2017). Temporal stability of implicit and explicit measures: A longitudinal analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(3), 300312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The Power of Thinking Without Thinking. New York, NY: Little Brown & Co.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 553561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Lai, C. K. (2020). Implicit social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 419445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., et al. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 1741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Smith, C. T., Sriram, N., et al. (2009). Implicit race attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 US presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 9(1), 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, A., & Gawronski, B. (2018). Implicit social cognition. Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 133.Google Scholar
Hahn, A., & Gawronski, B. (2019). Facing one’s implicit biases: From awareness to acknowledgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(5), 769794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, A., & Goedderz, A. (2020). Trait-unconsciousness, state-unconsciousness, preconsciousness, and social miscalibriation in the context of implicit evaluations. Social Cognition, 38(Supplement), s155s134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, A., Judd, C. M., Hirsh, H. K., et al. (2014). Awareness of implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 13691392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haines, N., Kvam, P. D., Irving, L. H., et al. (2020). Learning from the reliability paradox: How theoretically informed generative models can advance the social, behavioral, and brain sciences. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Hehman, E., Calanchini, J., Flake, J. K., et al. (2019). Establishing construct validity evidence for regional measures of explicit and implicit racial bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 1022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2018). Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(4), 393401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., et al. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 13691385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B. A., et al. (2005). What moderates implicit–explicit consistency? European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 335390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoover, J., & Dehghani, M. (2020). The big, the bad, and the ugly: Geographic estimation with flawed psychological data. Psychological Methods, 25, 412429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, J. L., Gaither, S. E., & Ratliff, K. A. (2015). Caught in the middle: Defensive responses to IAT feedback among whites, blacks, and biracial black/whites. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 373381. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614561127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, J. L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2017). Not your average bigot: The better‐than‐average effect and defensive responding to Implicit Association Test feedback. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56, 125145. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, J. L., Redford, L., Pogge, G., et al. (2017). Defensive responding to IAT feedback. Social Cognition, 35, 520562. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2017.35.5.520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inbar, Y., Westgate, E. C., Pizarro, D. A., et al. (2016). Can a naturally occurring pathogen threat change social attitudes? Evaluations of gay men and lesbians during the 2014 Ebola epidemic. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 420427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irving, L. H., & Smith, C. T. (2020). Measure what you are trying to predict: Applying the correspondence principle to the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 86, 103898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, C., Vitiello, C., Axt, J., et al. (2021). An examination of ingroup preferences among people with multiply socially stigmatized identities. Self and Identity, 20(4), 569586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T. (2019). The IAT is dead, long live the IAT: Context-sensitive measures of implicit attitudes are indispensable to social and political psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpinski, A., Steinman, R. B., & Hilton, J. L. (2005). Attitude importance as a moderator of the relationship between implicit and explicit attitude measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 949962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraus, S. J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: A meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(1), 5875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurdi, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Repeated evaluative pairings and evaluative statements: How effectively do they shift implicit attitudes?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(2), 194213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdi, B., Ratliff, K. A., & Cunningham, W. A. (2021). Can the Implicit Association Test serve as a valid measure of automatic cognition? A response to Schimmack (2021). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 422434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E., et al. (2016). Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 10011016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, C., Beach, M. C., & Roter, D. L. (2020). Individuation and implicit racial bias in genetic counseling communication. Patient Education and Counseling, 103(4), 804810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacInnis, C. C., Page-Gould, E., & Hodson, G. (2017). Multilevel intergroup contact and antigay prejudice (explicit and implicit) evidence of contextual contact benefits in a less visible group domain. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 243251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maximiano-Barreto, M. A., Fabrício, D. D. M., Luchesi, B. M., et al. (2020). Factors associated with levels of empathy among students and professionals in the health field: A systematic review. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 42(2), 207215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moors, A., & Houwer, J. D. (2007). What is Automaticity? An analysis of its component features and their interrelations. In Bargh, J. A. (Ed.), Frontiers of Social Psychology. Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 1150.Google Scholar
Neville, H. A., Gallardo, M. E., & Sue, D. W. E. (2016). The Myth of Racial Color Blindness: Manifestations, Dynamics, and Impact. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002a). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002b). E‐research: Ethics, security, design, and control in psychological research on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues, 58(1), 161176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18(1), 3688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ofosu, E. K., Chambers, M. K., Chen, J. M., et al. (2019). Same-sex marriage legalization associated with reduced implicit and explicit antigay bias. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(18), 88468851CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orchard, J., & Price, J. (2017). County-level racial prejudice and the black–white gap in infant health outcomes. Social Science & Medicine, 181, 191198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: a meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(2), 171192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., & Gawronski, B. (2010). A history of implicit social cognition: Where is it coming from? Where is it now? Where is it going? In Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L. (2019). Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(24), 1169311698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017). The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 233248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prairie, T. M., Wrye, B., Bowman, A. S., et al. (2019). Does location of practice or religiosity predict negative physician attitudes or beliefs toward LGB+ individuals? Journal of Religion and Health, 58(6), 22082218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rae, J. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2017). Persons or situations? Individual differences explain variance in aggregated implicit race attitudes. Psychological Inquiry, 28(4), 297300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rae, J. R., Newheiser, A. K., & Olson, K. R. (2015). Exposure to racial out-groups and implicit race bias in the United States. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 535543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratliff, K. A., Lofaro, N., Howell, J. L., et al. (2020). Documenting Bias from 2007-2015: Pervasiveness and Correlates of Implicit Attitudes and Stereotypes II. Unpublished Manuscript.Google Scholar
Ratliff[Ranganath], K. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Implicit attitude generalization occurs immediately; explicit attitude generalization takes time. Psychological Science, 19(3), 249254.Google Scholar
Ratliff[Ranganath], K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement methods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 386396.Google Scholar
Ratliff, K. A., Redford, L., Conway, J., et al. (2019). Engendering support: Hostile sexism predicts voting for Donald Trump over Hillary Clinton in the 2016 US presidential election. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(4), 578593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravary, A., Baldwin, M. W., & Bartz, J. A. (2019). Shaping the body politic: Mass media fat shaming affects implicit anti-fat attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(11), 15801589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2016). Perceived moral responsibility for attitude‐based discrimination. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(2), 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riddle, T., & Sinclair, S. (2019). Racial disparities in school-based disciplinary actions are associated with county-level rates of racial bias. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(17), 82558260. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1808307116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: a systems of reasoning analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 9951008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Mackie, D. M., et al. (2006). Of two minds: Forming and changing valence-inconsistent implicit and explicit attitudes. Psychological Science, 17(11), 954958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabin, J. A., Riskind, R. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Health care providers’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward lesbian women and gay men. American Journal of Public Health, 105, 18311841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U. (2021). The Implicit Association Test: A method in search of a construct. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 396414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, K., & Axt, J. R. (2016). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward African Americans and Barack Obama did not substantively change during Obama’s presidency. Social Cognition, 34, 559588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2005). A latent state-trait analysis of implicit and explicit personality measures. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21(2), 100107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. T., Calanchini, J., Hughes, S., et al. (2020). The impact of instruction- and experience-based evaluative learning on IAT performance: A quad model perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 34(1), 2141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. T., & De Houwer, J. (2014). The impact of persuasive messages on IAT performance is moderated by source attractiveness and likeability. Social Psychology, 45(6), 437448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. T., De Houwer, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2013). Consider the source: Persuasion of implicit evaluations is moderated by source credibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(2), 193205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2011). Affective focus increases the concordance between implicit and explicit attitudes. Social Psychology, 42, 300313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. T., Ratliff, K. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Rapid assimilation: Automatically integrating new information with existing beliefs. Social Cognition, 30(2), 199219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starck, J. G., Riddle, T., Sinclair, S., et al. (2020). Teachers are people too: Examining the racial bias of teachers compared to other American adults. Educational Researcher. 49(4), 273284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vianello, M., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2021). Can the Implicit Association Test measure automatic judgment? The validation continues. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 415421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuletich, H. A. & Payne, B. K. (2019). Stability and change in implicit bias. Psychological Science, 30(6), 854862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westgate, E. C., Riskind, R. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Implicit preferences for straight people over Lesbian Women and Gay Men weakened from 2006 to 2013. Collabra, 1, (Art.1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107(1), 101126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zanon, R., De Houwer, J., Gast, A., et al. (2014). When does relational information influence evaluative conditioning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(11), 21052212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×