Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T21:58:58.776Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

38 - Dance and the Imagination: Be a Butterfly!

from Part V - Phenomenology-Based Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

In dance, there are three components that stir our imagination: the movement of our body or the movement of others (kinesthetic imagery), the music that accompanies the movement (auditory imagery), and the emotions that arise from the physical sensation in our body while we dance (interoceptive imagery). All three types of imagery tap into different yet interrelated neural systems that are engaged both during dancing, as much as during the imagination of a dance. In this chapter, we will describe each of these imagery types in relation to dance. We also show evidence about how the combination of kinesthetic, auditory, and interoceptive imagery helps both amateur and professional dancers to rehearse their dance movements and find inspiration for new moves without actually moving their body at all.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brampton, S. (2018). Cognitive Imagery Training in a Dancer’s Deliberate Practice: Skills Development, Confidence and Creativity. Perth, Australia: Edith Cowan University, BA of Arts (Dance) thesis.Google Scholar
Carlson, N. R. (2004). Physiology of Behavior. 8th edition. New York, NY: Pearson.Google Scholar
Cesari, P., Camponogara, I., Papetti, S., Rocchesso, D., and Fontana, F. (2014). Might as Well Jump: Sound Affects Muscle Activation in Skateboarding. PLoS One, 9(3), e90156.Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Gaigg, S. B., and Calvo-Merino, B. (2018). I Can Feel My Heartbeat: Dancers Have Increased Interoceptive Awareness. Psychophysiology, 55:e13008.Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Gaigg, S. B., Gomila, A., Oke, P., and Calvo-Merino, B. (2014). Enhancing Emotional Experiences to Dance through Music: The Role of Valence and Arousal in the Cross-Modal Bias. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00757.Google Scholar
Coker, E., McIsaac, T. L., and Nilsen, D. (2015). Motor Imagery Modality in Expert Dancers: An Investigation of Hip and Pelvis Kinematics in Demi-Plie and Saute. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, 19(2), 6369.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2008). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY: Harper Perennial Modern Classics.Google Scholar
Fish, L., Hall, C., and Cumming, J. (2004). Investigating the Use of Imagery by Elite Ballet Dancers. AVANTE, 10(3), 2639.Google Scholar
Freedberg, D., and Gallese, V. (2007). Motion, Emotion and Empathy in Esthetic Experience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 197203.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S. N., and Critchley, H. D. (2013). Interoception, Emotion and Brain: New Insights Link Internal Physiology to Social Behaviour. Commentary on: “Anterior Insular Cortex Mediates Bodily Sensibility and Social Anxiety” by Terasawa et al. (2012). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 231234.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzujum, J., and Critchley, H. (2015). Knowing Your Own Heart: Distinguishing Interoceptive Accuracy from Interoceptive Awareness. Biological Psychology, 104, 6574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginot, I., Barlow, A., and Franko, M. (2010). Shusterman’s Somaesthetics to a Radical Epistemology of Somatics. Dance Research Journal, 42(1), 1229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giron, E. C., McIsaac, T., and Nilsen, D. (2012). Effects of Kinesthetic versus Visual Imagery Practice on Two Technical Dance Movements: A Pilot Study. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, 16(1), 3638.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, H. (2002). Dancing with Your Head On: Mental Imagery Techniques for Dancers. Journal of Dance Education, 2(1), 1522.Google Scholar
Golomer, E., Bouillette, A., Mertz, C., and Keller, J. (2008). Effects of Mental Imagery Styles on Shoulder and Hip Rotations during Preparation of Pirouettes. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40(4), 281290.Google Scholar
Hanna, J. L. (1995a). The Power of Dance: Health and Healing. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 1(4), 323331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanna, T. (1995b). What is Somatics? In Johnson, D. H. (ed.), Bone, Breath, and Gesture. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books, 339359.Google Scholar
Herholz, S. C., Lappe, C., Knief, A., and Pantev, C. (2008). Neural Basis of Music Imagery and the Effect of Musical Expertise. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 23522360.Google Scholar
Honing, H., and Merchant, H. (2014). Differences in Auditory Timing between Human and Nonhuman Primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(6), 557558; discussion 577604.Google Scholar
Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory Imagery: Empirical Findings. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 302329.Google Scholar
Jeong, E.-H. (2012). The Application of Imagery to Enhance “Flow State” in Dancers. Melbourne, Australia: Victoria University, PhD thesis.Google Scholar
Jeong, Y. J., Hong, S. C., Lee, M. S., et al. (2005). Dance Movement Therapy Improves Emotional Responses and Modulates Neurohormones in Adolescents with Mild Depression. International Journal of Neuroscience, 115(12), 17111720.Google Scholar
Juslin, P. N., and Vastfjall, D. (2008). Emotional Responses to Music: The Need to Consider the Underlying Mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(6), 559575.Google Scholar
Karin, J. (2016). Recontextualizing Dance Skills: Overcoming Impediments to Motor Learning and Expressivity in Ballet Dancers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 431.Google Scholar
Karin, J., Haggard, P., and Christensen, J. F. (2016). Mental Training. In Wilmerding, V and Krasnow, D (eds.), Dancer Wellness. Champaign, Canada: Human Kinetics, 5770.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E. (2012). Mental Imagery in Music Performance: Underlying Mechanisms and Potential Benefits. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 206213.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E., Dalla Bella, S., and Koch, I. (2010). Auditory Imagery Shapes Movement Timing and Kinematics: Evidence from a Musical Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 508513.Google Scholar
Koch, S., Kunz, T., Lykou, S., and Cruz, R. (2014). Effects of Dance Movement Therapy and Dance on Health-Related Psychological Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 41(1), 4664.Google Scholar
Kornysheva, K., von Cramon, D. Y., Jacobsen, T., and Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Tuning-in to the Beat: Aesthetic Appreciation of Musical Rhythms Correlates with a Premotor Activity Boost. Human Brain Mapping, 31(1), 4864.Google Scholar
Mahoney, M. J., and Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the Elite Athlete: An Exploratory Study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1(2), 135141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mainka, S. (2015). Music Stimulates Muscles, Mind, and Feelings in One Go. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1547–1547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meister, I. G., Krings, T., Foltys, H., et al. (2004). Playing Piano in the Mind – An fMRI Study on Music Imagery and Performance in Pianists. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(3), 219228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merchant, H., Grahn, J., Trainor, L., Rohrmeier, M., and Fitch, W. T. (2015). Finding the Beat: A Neural Perspective across Humans and Non-Human Primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140093.Google Scholar
Merchant, H., and Honing, H. (2013). Are Non-Human Primates Capable of Rhythmic Entrainment? Evidence for the Gradual Audiomotor Evolution Hypothesis. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 274.Google Scholar
Molinari, M., Leggio, M. G., de Martin, M., Cerasa, A., and Thaut, M. (2003). Neurobiology of Rhythmic Motor Entrainment. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 313321.Google Scholar
Murphy, S. M., Nordin, S. M., and Cumming, J. I. E. (2008). Imagery in Sport, Exercise and Dance. In Horn, T (ed.), Advances in Sport Psychology. 3rd edition. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 297324.Google Scholar
Nordin, S., and Cumming, J. (2006). Measuring the Content of Dancers’ Images: Development of the Dance Imagery Questionnaire (DIQ). Journal of Dance Medicine and Science, 10(3).Google Scholar
Nordin, S., and Cumming, J.(2008a). Types and Functions of Athletes’ Imagery: Testing Predictions from the Applied Model of Imagery Use by Examining Effectiveness. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 6(2), 189206.Google Scholar
Nordin, S., and Cumming, J.(2008b). Exploring Common Ground: Comparing the Imagery of Dancers and Aesthetic Sport Performers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20(4), 375391.Google Scholar
Noy, L., Dekel, E., and Alon, U. (2011). The Mirror Game as a Paradigm for Studying the Dynamics of Two People Improvising Motion Together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(52), 2094720952.Google Scholar
Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R., and Hietanen, J. K. (2014). Bodily Maps of Emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 646651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olshansky, M. P., Bar, R. J., Fogarty, M., and DeSouza, J. F. (2015). Supplementary Motor Area and Primary Auditory Cortex Activation in an Expert Break-Dancer during the Kinesthetic Motor Imagery of Dance to Music. Neurocase, 21(5), 607617.Google Scholar
Omigie, D., Pearce, M. T., Williamson, V. J., and Stewart, L. (2013). Electrophysiological Correlates of Melodic Processing in Congenital Amusia. Neuropsychologia, 51(9), 17491762.Google Scholar
Overby, L. Y., and Dunn, J. (2011). The History and Research of Dance Imagery: Implications for Teachers. IADMS Bulletin for Teachers, 2, 911.Google Scholar
Paltsev, Y. I., and Elner, A. M. (1967). Change in Functional State of the Segmental Apparatus of the Spinal Cord under the Influence of Sound Stimuli and its Role in Voluntary Movement. Biophysics, 12, 12191226.Google Scholar
Pavlik, K., and Nordin-Bates, S. (2016). Imagery in Dance: A Literature Review. Journal of Dance Medicine and Science, 20(2), 2153.Google Scholar
Peasley-Miklus, C. E., Panayiotou, G., and Vrana, S. R. (2016). Alexithymia Predicts Arousal-Based Processing Deficits and Discordance between Emotion Response Systems during Emotional Imagery. Emotion, 16(2), 164.Google Scholar
Perica, R. (2010). The Dancing Imagination: How Does Imaginative Imagery Facilitate Movement Qualities in Dance Training and Performance? Perth, Australia, Edith Cowan University, BA (Hons) thesis. ro.ecu.edu.au/theses_hons/1407.Google Scholar
Poikonen, H., Toiviainen, P., and Tervaniemi, M. (2016). Early Auditory Processing in Musicians and Dancers during a Contemporary Dance Piece. Scientific Reports, 6, 33056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schirmer-Mokwa, K. L., Fard, P. R., Zamorano, A. M., et al. (2015). Evidence for Enhanced Interoceptive Accuracy in Professional Musicians. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 349.Google Scholar
Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (2009). Fiber Pathways of the Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shafir, T. (2016). Using Movement to Regulate Emotion: Neurophysiological Findings and Their Application in Psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafir, T., Taylor, S. F., Atkinson, A. P., Langenecker, S. A., and Zubieta, J. K. (2013). Emotion Regulation through Execution, Observation, and Imagery of Emotional Movements. Brain and Cognition, 82(2), 219227.Google Scholar
Shafir, T., Tsachor, R. P., and Welch, K. B. (2015). Emotion Regulation through Movement: Unique Sets of Movement Characteristics Are Associated with and Enhance Basic Emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2030.Google Scholar
Sumanapala, D. K., Fish, L. A., Jones, A. L., and Cross, E. S. (2017). Have I Grooved to This Before? Discriminating Practised and Observed Actions in a Novel Context. Acta Psychologica, 175, 4249.Google Scholar
Thaut, M. H. (2003). Neural Basis of Rhythmic Timing Networks in the Human Brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 364373.Google Scholar
Thaut, M. H., Kenyon, G. P., Schauer, M. L., and McIntosh, G. C. (1999). The Connection between Rhythmicity and Brain Function. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine, 18(2), 101108.Google Scholar
Williams, S. E., Cooley, S. J., and Cumming, J. (2013). Layered Stimulus Response Training Improves Motor Imagery Ability and Movement Execution. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 35(1), 6071.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., and Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational Principles of Movement Neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3 (supplement 1212–1217).Google Scholar
Xenos, S., Rossignol, S., and Jones, G. M. (1976). Audio-Spinal Influence in Man Studied by the H-Reflex and its Possible Role on Rhythmic Movements Synchronized to Sound. Electroencephalography of Clinical Neurophysiology, 41(1), 8392.Google Scholar
Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the Brain Plays Music: Auditory-Motor Interactions in Music Perception and Production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(7), 547558.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×