Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T21:08:54.506Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Musical Imagery

from Part II - Imagery-Based Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

Musical imagery – the mental experience of music – is a common occurrence for both musical experts and nonexperts alike. A diverse range of experiences can be classified as musical imagery, from the replaying of a tune in a listener’s head after hearing it on the radio to a composer conjuring up the next notes of a new symphony. Overall, research in this domain has indicated that incidences of musical imagery often replicate a perceptual experience with a high degree of fidelity, and similar neural resources are recruited in both music perception and imagery. The diversity and fidelity of musical imagery experiences can be affected by expertise, suggesting imaginative processes related to music may be developed through dedicated, long-term practice. Both everyday and expert experiences of musical imagery can vary widely in terms of both intentionality and modality of recall, with some episodes comprising aspects of auditory, motor, and visual imagery. Implications for our understanding of creativity and suggestions for cross-disciplinary approaches to integrating psychological research findings with creative practice are discussed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agnew, M. (1922). The Auditory Imagery of Great Composers. Psychological Monographs, 31(1), 279.Google Scholar
Aranosian, C. M. (1981). Musical Creativity: The Stream of Consciousness in Composition, Improvisation, and Education. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 1(1), 6788.Google Scholar
Arora, S., Aggarwal, R., Sirimanna, P., et al. (2011). Mental Practice Enhances Surgical Technical Skills: A Randomized Controlled Study. Annals of Surgery, 253(2), 265270.Google Scholar
Bailes, F. A. (2002). Musical Imagery: Hearing and Imagining Music. Sheffield, UK: University of Sheffield, PhD thesis.Google Scholar
Bailes, F. A. (2006). The Use of Experience-Sampling Methods to Monitor Musical Imagery in everyday life. Musicae Scientiae, 10, 173190.Google Scholar
Bailes, F. A. (2007a). Timbre as an Elusive Component of Imagery for Music. Empirical Musicology Review, 2, 2134.Google Scholar
Bailes, F. A. (2007b). The Prevalence and Nature of Imagined Music in the Everyday Lives of Music Students. Psychology of Music, 35, 555570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailes, F. A. (2015). Music in Mind? An Experience Sampling Study of What and When, Towards an Understanding of Why. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 5868.Google Scholar
Bailes, F., Bishop, L., Stevens, C. J., and Dean, R. T. (2012). Mental Imagery for Musical Changes in Loudness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (Dec).Google Scholar
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., et al. (2012). Inspired by Distraction: Mind-Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 11171122.Google Scholar
Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., et al. (2006). Shared Networks for Auditory and Motor Processing in Professional Pianists: Evidence from fMRI Conjunction. NeuroImage, 30(3), 917926.Google Scholar
Baruss, I., and Wammes, M. (2009). Characteristics of Spontaneous Musical Imagery. Journal Of Consciousness Studies, 16(1), 3761.Google Scholar
Beaman, C. P., and Williams, T. I. (2010). Earworms (Stuck Song Syndrome): Towards a Natural History of Intrusive Thoughts. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 637653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaman, C. P., and Williams, T. I. (2013). Individual Differences in Mental Control Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery. Musicae Scientiae, 17, 398409.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Nusbaum, E. C., et al. (2013). Music to the Inner Ears: Exploring Individual Differences in Musical Imagery. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 11631173.Google Scholar
Bishop, L., Bailes, F., and Dean, R. T. (2013a). Musical Expertise and the Ability to Imagine Loudness. PLoS ONE, 8(2).Google Scholar
Bishop, L., Bailes, F., and Dean, R. T. (2013b). Musical Imagery and the Planning of Dynamics and Articulation During Performance. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(2), 97117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely Pleasurable Responses to Music Correlate with Activity in Brain Regions Implicated in Reward and Emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(20), 1181811823.Google Scholar
Brown, S. (2006). The Perpetual Music Track: The Phenomenon of Constant Musical Imagery. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 13(6), 4362.Google Scholar
Byron, T. P., and Fowles, L. C. (2015). Repetition and Recency Increases Involuntary Musical Imagery of Previously Unfamiliar Songs. Psychology of Music, 43(3), 375389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and Music: Can Traits Explain How People Use Music in Everyday Life? British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 175185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on Time: Brain Network for Auditory-Motor Synchronization Is Modulated by Rhythm Complexity and Musical Training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 226239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clynes, M., and Walker, J. (1982). Neurobiologic Functions of Rhythm, Time, and Pulse in Music. In Clynes, M (ed.), Music, Mind, and Brain: The Neuropsychology of Music. New York: Plenum, 171216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coffman, D. D. (1990). Effects of Mental Practice, Physical Practice, and Knowledge of Results on Piano Performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 38(3), 187.Google Scholar
Connolly, C., and Williamon, A. (2004). Mental Skills Training. In Williamon, A (ed.), Music Excellence: Strategies and Techniques to Enhance Performance. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 221242.Google Scholar
Cotter, K. N., Christensen, A. P., and Silvia, P. J. (2018). Understanding Inner Music: A Dimensional Approach to Musical Imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000195.Google Scholar
Cotter, K. N., and Silvia, P. J. (2017). Measuring Mental Music: Comparing Retrospective and Experience Sampling Methods for Assessing Musical Imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(3), 335343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowder, R. G. (1989). Imagery for Musical Timbre. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 472.Google Scholar
Davidson-Kelly, K., Schaefer, R. S., Moran, N., and Overy, K. (2015). “Total Inner Memory”: Deliberate Uses of Multimodal Musical Imagery During Performance Preparation. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 83.Google Scholar
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., and Moran, A. (1994). Does Mental Practice Enhance Performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), 481492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. (2012). On the Evolutionary Function of Song and Dance. In Bannan, N (ed.), Music, Language and Human Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 201214.Google Scholar
Eerola, T., Friberg, A., and Bresin, R. (2013). Emotional Expression in Music: Contribution, Linearity, and Additivity of Primary Musical Cues. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., and Smith, A. F. (1983). Perceptual Interference and Facilitation with Auditory Imagery. Perception & Psychophysics, 33(5), 475478.Google Scholar
Farrugia, N., Jakubowski, K., Cusack, R., and Stewart, L. (2015). Tunes Stuck in Your Brain: The Frequency and Affective Evaluation of Involuntary Musical Imagery Correlate with Cortical Structure. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 6677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fine, P. A., Wise, K. J., Goldemberg, R., and Bravo, A. (2015). Performing Musicians’ Understanding of the Terms “Mental Practice” and “Score Analysis”. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 6982.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four Principles of Bio-Musicology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140091.Google Scholar
Floridou, G. A. (2016). Investigating the Relationship between Involuntary Musical Imagery and Other Forms of Spontaneous Cognition. London, UK: Goldsmiths, University of London, PhD thesis.Google Scholar
Floridou, G. A., and Müllensiefen, D. (2015). Environmental and Mental Conditions Predicting the Experience of Involuntary Musical Imagery: An Experience Sampling Method Study. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 472486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floridou, G., Williamson, V. J., and Müllensiefen, D. (2012). Contracting Earworms: The Roles of Personality and Musicality. In Cambouropoulos, E, Tsougras, C, Mavromatis, K, and Pastiadis, K, (eds.), Proceedings of ICMPC-ESCOM 12, Thessaloniki, Greece, 302310.Google Scholar
Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J., and Stewart, L. (2017). A Novel Indirect Method for Capturing Involuntary Musical Imagery under Varying Cognitive Load. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(11), 21892199.Google Scholar
Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J., Stewart, L., and Müllensiefen, D. (2015). The Involuntary Musical Imagery Scale (IMIS). Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 2836.Google Scholar
Forster, S., and Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the Wandering Mind: The Role of Perceptual Load. Cognition, 111(3), 345355.Google Scholar
Gabrielsson, A., and Lindström, E. (2010). The Role of Structure in the Musical Expression of Emotions. In Juslin, P. N. and Sloboda, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, and Applications. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 367400.Google Scholar
Gelding, R. W., Thompson, W. F., and Johnson, B. W. (2015). The Pitch Imagery Arrow Task: Effects of Musical Training, Vividness, and Mental Control. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0121809.Google Scholar
Gordon, E. E. (1999). All About Audiation and Music Aptitudes. Music Educators Journal, 86(2), 4144.Google Scholar
Gordon, E. E. (2003). Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content, and Patterns. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications.Google Scholar
Gould, D., Voelker, D. F., Damarjian, N., and Greenleaf, C. (2014). Imagery Training for Peak Performance. In van Raalte, J. L. and Brewer, B. W. (eds.), Exploring Sport and Exercise Psychology. 3rd edition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 5582.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. D. (2000). Musical Hallucinosis in Acquired Deafness: Phenomenology and Brain Substrate. Brain, 123(10), 20652076.Google Scholar
Hallam, S. (2010). The Power of Music: Its Impact on the Intellectual, Social and Personal Development of Children and Young People. International Journal of Music Education, 28(3), 269289.Google Scholar
Halpern, A. R. (1988). Perceived and Imagined Tempos of Familiar Songs. Music Perception, 6(2), 193202.Google Scholar
Halpern, A. R. (1989). Memory for the Absolute Pitch of Familiar Songs. Memory & Cognition, 17(5), 572581.Google Scholar
Halpern, A. R. (1992). Musical Aspects of Auditory Imagery. In Reisberg, D (ed.), Auditory Imagery. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 127.Google Scholar
Halpern, A. R. (2015). Differences in Auditory Imagery Self-Report Predict Neural and Behavioral Outcomes. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 3747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, A. R., and Bartlett, J. C. (2011). The Persistence of Musical Memories: A Descriptive Study of Earworms. Music Perception, 28(4), 425432.Google Scholar
Halpern, A. R., Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., and Johnson, J. A. (2004). Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Perceived and Imagined Musical Timbre. Neuropsychologia, 42(9), 12811292.Google Scholar
Herholz, S. C., Halpern, A. R., and Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Neuronal Correlates of Perception, Imagery, and Memory for Familiar Tunes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(6), 13821397.Google Scholar
Herholz, S. C., Lappe, C., Knief, A., and Pantev, C. (2008). Neural Basis of Music Imagery and the Effect of Musical Expertise. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 23522360.Google Scholar
Highben, Z., and Palmer, C. (2004). Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 159, 5865.Google Scholar
Holmes, P. (2005). Imagination in Practice: A Study of the Integrated Roles of Interpretation, Imagery and Technique in the Learning and Memorisation Processes of Two Experienced Solo Performers. British Journal of Music Education, 22(3), 217235.Google Scholar
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., et al. (2009). The Effects of Musical Training on Structural Brain Development: A Longitudinal Study. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169(1), 182186.Google Scholar
Hyman, I. E., Burland, N. K., Duskin, H. M., et al. (2013). Going Gaga: Investigating, Creating, and Manipulating the Song Stuck in My Head. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 204215.Google Scholar
Hyman, I. E., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., et al. (2015). Involuntary to Intrusive: Using Involuntary Musical Imagery to Explore Individual Differences and the Nature of Intrusive Thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 1427.Google Scholar
Intons-Peterson, M. J. (1980). The Role of Loudness in Auditory Imagery. Memory & Cognition, 8(5), 385393. dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03211134.Google Scholar
Intons-Peterson, M. J. (1992). Components of Auditory Imagery. In Reisberg, D (ed.), Auditory Imagery. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 4572.Google Scholar
Jakubowski, K., Bashir, Z., Farrugia, N., and Stewart, L. (2018). Involuntary and Voluntary Recall of Musical Memories: A Comparison of Temporal Accuracy and Emotional Responses. Memory and Cognition, 46(5), 741756.Google Scholar
Jakubowski, K., Farrugia, N., Halpern, A. R., Sankarpandi, S. K., and Stewart, L. (2015). The Speed of Our Mental Soundtracks: Tracking the Tempo of Involuntary Musical Imagery in Everyday Life. Memory and Cognition, 43(8), 12291242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakubowski, K., Farrugia, N., and Stewart, L. (2016). Probing Imagined Tempo for Music: Effects of Motor Engagement and Musical Experience. Psychology of Music, 44(6), 12741288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowski, K., Finkel, S., Stewart, L., and Müllensiefen, D. (2017). Dissecting an Earworm: Melodic Features and Song Popularity Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(2), 122135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janata, P., and Paroo, K. (2006). Acuity of Auditory Images in Pitch and Time. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(5), 829844.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E. (2012). Mental Imagery in Music Performance: Underlying Mechanisms and Potential Benefits. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 206213.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E., and Appel, M. (2010). Individual Differences, Auditory Imagery, and the Coordination of Body Movements and Sounds in Musical Ensembles. Music Perception, 28(1), 2746.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E., Dalla Bella, S., and Koch, I. (2010). Auditory Imagery Shapes Movement Timing and Kinematics: Evidence from a Musical Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 508513.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E., and Koch, I. (2006). The Planning and Execution of Short Auditory Sequences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 711716.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E., and Koch, I. (2008). Action Planning in Sequential Skills: Relations to Music Performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 275291.Google Scholar
Kleber, B., Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Trevorrow, T., and Lotze, M. (2007). Overt and Imagined Singing of an Italian Aria. NeuroImage, 36(3), 889900.Google Scholar
Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain Correlates of Music-Evoked Emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 170180.Google Scholar
Kraemer, D. J. M., Macrae, C. N., Green, A. E., and Kelley, W. M. (2005). Musical Imagery: Sound of Silence Activates Auditory Cortex. Nature, 434(7030), 158.Google Scholar
Lancashire, R. (2017). An Experience-Sampling Study to Investigate the Role of Familiarity in Involuntary Musical Imagery Induction. In Harrison, P. M. C. (ed.), Proceedings of the 10th International Conference of Students of Systematic Musicology (SysMus17), London, UK.Google Scholar
Liikkanen, L. A. (2012a). Musical Activities Predispose to Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music, 40, 236256.Google Scholar
Liikkanen, L. A. (2012b). Inducing Involuntary Musical Imagery: An Experimental Study. Musicae Scientiae, 16(2), 217234.Google Scholar
Liikkanen, L. A., Jakubowski, K., and Toivanen, J. M. (2015). Catching Earworms on Twitter: Using Big Data to Study Involuntary Musical Imagery. Music Perception, 33(2), 199216.Google Scholar
Limb, C. J., and Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural Substrates of Spontaneous Musical Performance: An fMRI Study of Jazz Improvisation. PLoS one, 3(2), e1679.Google Scholar
Lucas, B. J., Schubert, E., and Halpern, A. R. (2010). Perception of Emotion in Sounded and Imagined Music. Music Perception, 27(5), 399412.Google Scholar
McAdams, S., Winsberg, S., Donnadieu, S., de Soete, G., and Krimphoff, J. (1995). Perceptual Scaling of Synthesized Musical Timbres: Common Dimensions, Specificities, and Latent Subject Classes. Psychological Research, 58(3), 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough Campbell, S., and Margulis, E. H. (2015). Catching an Earworm Through Movement. Journal of New Music Research, 44(4), 347358.Google Scholar
McDermott, J., and Hauser, M. (2005). The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution. Music Perception, 23(1), 2959.Google Scholar
McNally-Gagnon, A. (2016). Imagerie musicale involontaire: caractéristiques phénoménologiques et mnésiques. Montreal, Canada: University of Montreal, PhD thesis.Google Scholar
Mountain, R. (2001). Composers and Imagery: Myths and Realities. In Godøy, R. I. and Jörgensen, H (eds.), Musical Imagery. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis, 271288.Google Scholar
Müllensiefen, D., Jones, R., Jilka, S., Stewart, L., and Williamson, V. J. (2014). Individual Differences Predict Patterns in Spontaneous Involuntary Musical Imagery. Music Perception, 31(4), 323338.Google Scholar
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and Hargreaves, J. J. (2004). Uses of Music in Everyday Life. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22(1), 4177.Google Scholar
Okada, H., and Matsuoka, K. (1992). Effects of Auditory Imagery on the Detection of a Pure Tone in White Noise: Experimental Evidence of the Auditory Perky Effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74(2), 443448.Google Scholar
Pecenka, N., and Keller, P. E. (2009). Auditory Pitch Imagery and Its Relationship to Musical Synchronization. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 282286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitt, M. A., and Crowder, R. G. (1992). The Role of Spectral and Dynamic Cues in Imagery for Musical Timbre. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 728738.Google Scholar
Reybrouck, M. (2001). Musical Imagery between Sensory Processing and Ideomotor Simulation. In Godøy, R. I. and Jörgensen, H (eds.), Musical imagery. New York: Taylor & Francis, 117136.Google Scholar
Ross, S. L. (1985). The Effectiveness of Mental Practice in Improving the Performance of College Trombonists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 33(4), 221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaefer, R. S., Desain, P., and Farquhar, J. (2013). Shared Processing of Perception and Imagery of Music in Decomposed EEG. NeuroImage, 70, 317326. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.064.Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. A., O’Neill, S. A., and Ivaldi, A. (2001). Functions of Music in Everyday Life: An Exploratory Study Using the Experience Sampling Method. Musicae Scientiae, 5(1), 932.Google Scholar
Stewart, L., von Kriegstein, K., Warren, J. D., and Griffiths, T. D. (2006). Music and the Brain: Disorders of Musical Listening. Brain, 129(10), 25332553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S., McKay, D., Miguel, E. C., et al. (2014). Musical Obsessions: A Comprehensive Review of Neglected Clinical Phenomena. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(6), 580589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., et al. (1995). Stimulus-Independent Thought Depends on Central Executive Resources. Memory & Cognition, 23(5), 551559.Google Scholar
Trusheim, W. H. (1991). Audiation and Mental Imagery: Implications for Artistic Performance. Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 2, 138147.Google Scholar
Weber, R. J., and Brown, S. (1986). Musical Imagery. Music Perception, 3(4), 411426.Google Scholar
Weir, G., Williamson, V. J., and Müllensiefen, D. (2015). Increased Involuntary Musical Mental Activity Is Not Associated with More Accurate Voluntary Musical Imagery. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 4857.Google Scholar
Williams, T. I. (2015). The Classification of Involuntary Musical Imagery: The Case for Earworms. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 513.Google Scholar
Williamson, V. J., and Jilka, S. R. (2013). Experiencing Earworms: An Interview Study of Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music, 42 (5), 653670.Google Scholar
Williamson, V. J., Jilka, S. R., Fry, J., et al. (2012). How Do Earworms Start? Classifying the Everyday Circumstances of Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music, 40(3), 259284.Google Scholar
Williamson, V. J., and Müllensiefen, D. (2012). Earworms from Three Angles. In Cambouropoulos, E, Tsougras, C, Mavromatis, K, and Pastiadis, K, (eds.), Proceedings of ICMPC-ESCOM 12, Thessaloniki, Greece, 11241133.Google Scholar
Zatorre, R. J., and Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental Concerts: Musical Imagery and Auditory Cortex. Neuron 47(1), 912.Google Scholar
Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., and Bouffard, M. (2010). Mental Reversal of Imagined Melodies: A Role for the Posterior Parietal Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 775789.Google Scholar
Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E., and Evans, A. C. (1996). Hearing in the Mind’s Ear: A PET Investigation of Musical Imagery and Perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(1), 2946.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×