Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:56:48.897Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - The Neuroscience of Imaginative Thought: An Integrative Framework

from Part III - Intentionality-Based Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

The repertoire of human imaginative thought is incredibly large. Whether reminiscing on memorable past experiences, or entertaining our hypothetical futures; whether transporting ourselves into a good book, or listening to an engaging conversation; whether reasoning about the thoughts of others, or pondering the meaning of life – the human mind is constantly imagining that which lies beyond our current sensory input. In this chapter, we aim to introduce a neuroscience-informed framework by which the diverse array of imaginative thoughts can be functionally organized. We first review some key psychological ingredients of imaginative thought: their content, their level of abstraction, their mode of representation, and the ways in which they arise and unfold over time. Next, we propose a neural basis for these different elements based on research highlighting the role of the brain’s default network in aspects of imagination. Third, we propose an integrative model whereby imaginative thoughts can be divided into two broad classes that often interact in everyday experience and in the brain: one encompassing contextually rich, perceptually vivid mental simulations, and another encompassing conceptually abstract, often verbal modes of imagination. We hope this neurocognitive framework will help organize the many varieties of imagination and facilitate cross-disciplinary collaboration and discovery.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, A. (2013). The World According to Me: Personal Relevance and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 341.Google Scholar
Amit, E., Algom, D., Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2009). “Thou Shalt not Make Unto Thee Any Graven Image”: The Distance-Dependence of Representation. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M., and Suhr, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 5368.Google Scholar
Amit, E., Hoeflin, C., Hamzah, N., and Fedorenko, E. (2017). An Asymmetrical Relationship between Verbal and Visual Thinking: Converging Evidence from Behavior and fMRI. NeuroImage, 152, 619627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The Brain’s Default Network and Its Adaptive Role in Internal Mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18(3), 251270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Christoff, K., and O’Connor, M. F. (in press). Dynamic Regulation of Internal Experience. In Lane, R, Ryan, L, and Nadel, L (eds.), The Neuroscience of Enduring Change: The Neural Basis of Talk Therapies. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Kaiser, R. H., Turner, A. E., et al. (2013). A Penny for Your Thoughts: Dimensions of Self-Generated Thought Content and Relationships with Individual Differences in Emotional Wellbeing. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 900.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain’s Default Network. Neuron, 65(4), 550562.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Saxe, R., and Yarkoni, T. (2014). Contributions of Episodic Retrieval and Mentalizing to Autobiographical Thought: Evidence from Functional Neuroimaging, Resting-State Connectivity, and fMRI Meta-Analyses. Neuroimage, 91, 324335.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., and Spreng, R. N. (2014). The Default Network and Self‐Generated Thought: Component Processes, Dynamic Control, and Clinical Relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 2952.Google Scholar
Baetens, K., Ma, N., Steen, J., and van Overwalle, F. (2014). Involvement of the Mentalizing Network in Social and Non-Social High Construal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 817824.Google Scholar
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the Future: Autobiographical Planning and the Functionality of Mind-Wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 16041611.Google Scholar
Bar., M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., and Fenske, M. (2007). The Units of Thought. Hippocampus, 17(6), 420428.Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., and Desai, R. H. (2011). The Neurobiology of Semantic Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(11), 527536.Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta-Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 27672796.Google Scholar
Braga, R. M., and Buckner, R. L. (2017). Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Neuron, 95(2), 457–471.e5.Google Scholar
Braga, R. M., van Dijk, K. R., Polimeni, J. R., Eldaief, M. C., and Buckner, R. L. (2018). Parallel Distributed Networks Resolved at High Resolution Reveal Close Juxtaposition of Distinct Regions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 121(4), 15131534.Google Scholar
Brown, T. I., Rissman, J., Chow, T. E., Uncapher, M. R., and Wagner, A. D. (2018). Differential Medial Temporal Lobe and Parietal Cortical Contributions to Real-world Autobiographical Episodic and Autobiographical Semantic Memory. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6190.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-Projection and the Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 4957.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network: Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 138.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., and St. Jacques, P. (2007). Functional Neuroimaging of Autobiographical Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219227.Google Scholar
Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE Meta-Analysis of Action Observation and Imitation in the Human Brain. Neuroimage, 50(3), 11481167.Google Scholar
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., and Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-Wandering as Spontaneous Thought: A Dynamic Framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The Construction of Autobiographical Memories in the Self-Memory System. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261.Google Scholar
Cui, X., Jeter, C. B., Yang, D., Montague, P. R., and Eagleman, D. M. (2007). Vividness of Mental Imagery: Individual Variability Can Be Measured Objectively. Vision Research, 47(4), 474478.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A. (2013). On the Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Self-Processing: The Valuation Hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 372.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Cassol, H., Phillips, C., et al. (2013). Brains Creating Stories of Selves: The Neural Basis of Autobiographical Reasoning. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(5), 646652.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Lardi, C., and van der Linden, M. (2012). Self-Defining Future Projections: Exploring the Identity Function of Thinking about the Future. Memory, 20(2), 110120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual Differences in the Phenomenology of Mental Time Travel: The Effect of Vivid Visual Imagery and Emotion Regulation Strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 342350.Google Scholar
Delamillieure, P., Doucet, G., Mazoyer, B., et al. (2010). The Resting State Questionnaire: An Introspective Questionnaire for Evaluation of Inner Experience during the Conscious Resting State. Brain Research Bulletin, 81(6), 565573.Google Scholar
Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). A Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Self- and Other Judgments Reveals a Spatial Gradient for Mentalizing in Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 17421752.Google Scholar
Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M., and Christoff, K. (2012). Evaluative and Generative Modes of Thought during the Creative Process. Neuroimage, 59(2), 17831794.Google Scholar
Ernst, A., and D’Argembeau, A. (2017). Make it Real: Belief in Occurrence within Episodic Future Thought. Memory and Cognition, 45(6), 10451061.Google Scholar
Ferstl, E. C., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? Neuroimage, 17(3), 15991612.Google Scholar
Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W., and Christoff, K. (2013). Dreaming as Mind Wandering: Evidence from Functional Neuroimaging and First-Person Content Reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 412.Google Scholar
Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Christoff, K. (2015). The Wandering Brain: Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Mind-Wandering and Related Spontaneous Thought Processes. Neuroimage, 111, 611621.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2006). The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531534.Google Scholar
Grandin, T. (2006). Thinking in Pictures: And Other Reports from my Life with Autism. London, UK: Vintage.Google Scholar
Grilli, M. D., and Ryan, L. (Forthcoming). Autobiographical Memory and the Self-Concept. In Lane, R, Ryan, L, and Nadel, L (eds.), The Neuroscience of Enduring Change: The Neural Basis of Talk Therapies. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the Friendliest: Homo Sapiens Evolved via Selection for Prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., and Maguire, E.A. (2007). Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 1436514374.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Patients with Hippocampal Amnesia Cannot Imagine New Experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(5), 17261731.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing Episodic Memory with Construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 299306.Google Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., and Layton, J. B. (2010). Social Relationships and Mortality Risk: A Meta-Analytic Review. PloS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316.Google Scholar
Huth, A. G., de Heer, W. A., Griffiths, T. L., Theunissen, F. E., and Gallant, J. L. (2016). Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps that Tile Human Cerebral Cortex. Nature, 532(7600), 453.Google Scholar
Irish, M., and Piolino, P. (2016). Impaired Capacity for Prospection in the Dementias – Theoretical and Clinical Implications. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 4968.Google Scholar
Keogh, R., and Pearson, J. (2018). The Blind Mind: No Sensory Visual Imagery in Aphantasia. Cortex, 105, 5360.Google Scholar
Kim, H. (2010). Dissociating the Roles of the Default-Mode, Dorsal and Ventral Networks in Episodic Memory Retrieval. Neuroimage, 50(4), 16481657.Google Scholar
Klinger, E., (2009). Daydreaming and Fantasizing: Thought Flow and Motivation. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M. P., and Suhr, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 225240.Google Scholar
Klinger, E., and Cox, W. M. (1987). Dimensions of Thought Flow in Everyday Life. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 7(2), 105128.Google Scholar
Konishi, M., McLaren, D. G., Engen, H., and Smallwood, J. (2015). Shaped by the Past: The Default Network Supports Cognition that is Independent of Immediate Perceptual Input. PloS one, 10(6), e0132209.Google Scholar
Krienen, F. M., Tu, P. C., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Clan Mentality: Evidence That the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Responds to Close Others. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(41), 1390613915.Google Scholar
Lardi, C., D’Argembeau, A., Chanal, J., Ghisletta, P., and van der Linden, M. (2010). Further Characterisation of Self-Defining Memories in Young Adults: A Study of a Swiss Sample. Memory, 18(3), 293309.Google Scholar
Lavenex, P., and Amaral, D. G. (2000). Hippocampal-Neocortical Interaction: A Hierarchy of Associativity. Hippocampus, 10(4), 420430.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., Tipsord, J. M., and Tate, E. B. (2008). Allo-Inclusive Identity: Incorporating the Social and Natural Worlds into One’s Sense of Self. In Wayment, H. A. and Bauer, J. J. (eds.), Decade of Behavior Transcending Self-interest: Psychological Explorations of the Quiet Ego. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 137147.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social Cognitive Neuroscience: A Review of Core Processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259289.Google Scholar
Mar, R. A. (2011). The Neural Bases of Social Cognition and Story Comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mar, R. A., Mason, M. F., and Litvack, A. (2012). How Daydreaming Relates to Life Satisfaction, Loneliness, and Social Support: The Importance of Gender and Daydream Content. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 401407.Google Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., and Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: Meta‐Analysis of the Relation between Language Ability and False‐Belief Understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622646.Google Scholar
Mills, C., Raffaelli, Q., Irving, Z. C., Stan, D., and Christoff, K. (2018). Is an Off-Task Mind a Freely-Moving Mind? Examining the Relationship between Different Dimensions of Thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 2033.Google Scholar
Mooneyham, B. W., and Schooler, J. W. (2013). The Costs and Benefits of Mind-Wandering: A Review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 67(1), 11.Google Scholar
Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., and Kelley, W. M. (2009). Modulation of Cortical Midline Structures by Implicit and Explicit Self-Relevance Evaluation. Social Neuroscience, 4(3), 197211.Google Scholar
Mullally, S. L., and Maguire, E. A. (2014). Memory, Imagination, and Predicting the Future: A Common Brain Mechanism? The Neuroscientist, 20(3), 220234.Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parikh, N., Ruzic, L., Stewart, G. W., Spreng, R. N., and De Brigard, F. (2018). What if? Neural Activity Underlying Semantic and Episodic Counterfactual Thinking. Neuroimage, 178, 332345.Google Scholar
Pfeiffer, B. E. (2020). The Content of Hippocampal “Replay”. Hippocampus, 30(1), 618.Google Scholar
Prebble, S. C., Addis, D. R., and Tippett, L. J. (2013). Autobiographical Memory and Sense of Self. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 815.Google Scholar
Raffaelli, Q., Mills, C., and Christoff, K. (2017). The Knowns and Unknowns of Boredom: A Review of the Literature. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 24512462.Google Scholar
Rim, S., Amit, E., Fujita, K., et al. (2015). How Words Transcend and Pictures Immerse: On the Association between Medium and Level of Construal. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(2), 123130.Google Scholar
Ross, B. (2016). Imagine a Dog. Got it? I Don’t. Here’s What it’s Like to be Unable to Visualize Anything. Vox. May 19. www.vox.com/2016/5/19/11683274/aphantasia.Google Scholar
Ruby, F. J. M., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., and Singer, T. (2013). How Self-Generated Thought Shapes Mood – The Relation between Mind-Wandering and Mood Depends on the Socio-Temporal Content of Thoughts. PloS ONE, 8, e77554.Google Scholar
Saxe, R. (2009). The Happiness of the Fish: Evidence for a Common Theory of One’s Own and Others’ Actions. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M., and Suhr, J. A., (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 257266.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2007). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Constructive Memory: Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773786.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 657.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677694.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., and Sripada, C. (2016). Homo Prospectus. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2014). The Evolution of Language from Social Cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 59.Google Scholar
Sheldon, S., Farb, N., Palombo, D. J., and Levine, B. (2016). Intrinsic Medial Temporal Lobe Connectivity Relates to Individual Differences in Episodic Autobiographical Remembering. Cortex, 74, 206216.Google Scholar
Smallwood, J., and Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not All Minds That Wander Are Lost: The Importance of a Balanced Perspective on the Mind-Wandering State. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 441.Google Scholar
Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2015). The Science of Mind Wandering: Empirically Navigating the Stream of Consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487518.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., and Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2015). The Default Network and Social Cognition. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, 1316, 165169.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., DuPre, E., Selarka, D., et al. (2014). Goal-Congruent Default Network Activity Facilitates Cognitive Control. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1410814114.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Gerlach, K. D., Turner, G. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Autobiographical Planning and the Brain: Activation and its Modulation by Qualitative Features. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(11), 21472157.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. (2009). The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489510.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., and Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default Network Activity, Coupled with the Frontoparietal Control Network, Supports Goal-Directed Cognition. NeuroImage, 53, 303–317.Google Scholar
Spunt, R. P., Kemmerer, D., and Adolphs, R. (2015). The Neural Basis of Conceptualizing the Same Action at Different Levels of Abstraction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(7), 11411151.Google Scholar
Spunt, R. P., Meyer, M. L., and Lieberman, M. D. (2015). The Default Mode of Human Brain Function Primes the Intentional Stance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(6), 11161124.Google Scholar
Sripada, C. S. (2018). An Exploration/Exploitation Trade-off Between Mind-Wandering and Goal-Directed Thinking. In Fox, K. C. and Christoff, K (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2334.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., and D’Argembeau, A. (2015). Neural Correlates of Personal Goal Processing during Episodic Future Thinking and Mind‐Wandering: An ALE Meta‐Analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 36(8), 29282947.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., Cassol, H., and D’Argembeau, A. (2013). Phenomenology of Future-Oriented Mind-Wandering Episodes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 425.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., van der Linden, M., and D’Argembeau, A. (2011). Mind-Wandering: Phenomenology and Function as Assessed with a Novel Experience Sampling Method. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 370–381.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is It Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299313.Google Scholar
Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D., and Mitchell, J. P. (2015). Reading Fiction and Reading Minds: The Role of Simulation in the Default Network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 215224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and Consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 26(1), 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Veluw, S. J., and Chance, S. A. (2014). Differentiating between Self and Others: An ALE Meta-Analysis of fMRI Studies of Self-Recognition and Theory of Mind. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(1), 2438.Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1925). Treatise on Physiological Optics. Translated from the 3rd German edition and edited by Southall, J. Volume 3: The Perceptions of Vision. Ithaca, NY: Secretary, Optical Society of America, Rockefeller Hall.Google Scholar
Wang, H. T., Poerio, G., Murphy, C., et al. (2018). Dimensions of Experience: Exploring the Heterogeneity of the Wandering Mind. Psychological Science, 29(1), 5671.Google Scholar
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., et al. (2011). The Organization of the Human Cerebral Cortex Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 11251165.Google Scholar
Zaki, J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The Neuroscience of Empathy: Progress, Pitfalls and Promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 675.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×