Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:26:11.545Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

35 - Working Memory and Childhood Deafness

from Part VI - Language Disorders, Interventions, and Instruction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Get access

Summary

This chapter focuses on how working memory develops in children who are born deaf. It includes studies of deaf users of spoken and signed languages from within the medical and social models of deafness. It also reviews how differences in working memory capacity have been explained between deaf and hearing children. It reviews the role of auditory function in the establishment of working memory, as well as consideration of language as a mediator. It concludes with a proposal that deafness leads to disrupted early exposure to language and reduced subvocal rehearsal abilities, which both impact on the operation of the working memory system.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 38(2), 403414. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3802.403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., & Willis, C. (2005). Working memory abilities in children with special educational needs. Education and Child Psychology, 22.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., Willis, C., Eaglen, R., & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and other cognitive skills as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417426.Google Scholar
Almomani, F., Al-Momani, M. O., Garadat, S., Alqudah, S., Kassab, M., Hamadneh, S., Rauterkus, G., & Gans, R. (2021). Cognitive functioning in deaf children using cochlear implants. BMC Pediatrics, 21(1), 71. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-021-02534-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
AuBuchon, A. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kronenberger, W. G. (2015). Verbal processing speed and executive functioning in long-term cochlear implant users. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(1), 151162. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-H-13-0259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105,158173.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 742775). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., Newman, A. J., Mukherjee, M., Hauser, P., Kemeny, S., Braun, A., & Boutla, M. (2008). Encoding, rehearsal, and recall in signers and speakers: Shared network but differential engagement. Cerebral Cortex, 18(10), 22632274. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bebko, J., & Metcalfe-Haggert, A. (1997). Deafness, language skills, and rehearsal: A model for the development of a memory strategy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(3), 131139. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.deafed.a014319Google Scholar
Brentari, D. 2019. Sign language phonology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan-Worster, E; MacSweeney, M; Pimperton, H; Kyle, F; Harris, M; Beedie, I; Ralph-Lewis, A., & Hulme, C. (2020) Speechreading ability is related to phonological awareness and single-word reading in both deaf and hearing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 37753785Google Scholar
Campbell, R., & Dodd, B. (1980). Hearing by eye. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 8599.Google Scholar
Campbell, R., MacSweeney, M., & Woll, B. (2014). Cochlear implantation (CI) for prelingual deafness: The relevance of studies of brain organization and the role of first language acquisition in considering outcome success. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleary, M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kirk, K. I. (2000). Working memory spans as predictors of spoken word recognition and receptive vocabulary in children with cochlear implants. The Volta Review, 102, 259280.Google Scholar
Conrad, R. (1970). Short-term memory processes in the deaf. British Journal of Psychology, 61(2), 179195.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 5157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N., Li, Y., Glass, B., & Saults, J. S. (2017). Development of the ability to combine visual and acoustic information in working memory. Developmental Science, 21, e12635, 114.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Rouder, J. N., Blume, C. L., & Saults, J. S. (2012). Models of verbal working memory capacity: What does it take to make them work? Psychological Review, 119, 480499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Souza, D., D’Souza, H., Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2017). Precursors to language development in typically and atypically developing infants and toddlers: The importance of embracing complexity. Journal of Child Language, 44, 591627.Google Scholar
Davidson, K., Lillo-Martin, D., & Chen Pichler, D. (2014). Spoken English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(2), 238250.Google Scholar
Desjardin, J. (2006). Family empowerment: Supporting language development in young children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Review, 106, 275298.Google Scholar
Dillon, C. M., Cleary, M., Pisoni, D. B., & Carter, A. K. (2004). Imitation of nonwords by hearing-impaired children with cochlear implants: Segmental analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18(1), 3955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B., Hobson, P., Brasher, J., & Campbell, R. (1983). Deaf children’s short-term memory for lip-read, graphic and signed stimuli. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(4), 353364.Google Scholar
Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of the Dimensional Change Card Sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children. Developmental Review, 38, 241268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 65, 4959.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Giezen, M. R., Petrich, J. A. F., Spurgeon, E., & O’Grady Farnady, L. (2017). The relation between working memory and language comprehension in signers and speakers. Acta Psychologica, 177, 6977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagan, M. K., Pisoni, D., Horn, D., & Dillon, C. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates of vocabulary, reading, and working memory in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 461471Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, E., Hamel, C., Stevens, A., Pratt, M., Moher, D., Doucet, S., … Na, E. (2016). Sign language and spoken language for children with hearing loss: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 137, e20151974Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 410418.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770778.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Hitch, G. J. (1993). Developmental changes in short-term memory: A revised working memory perspective. In Collins, A. F., Gathercole, S. E., Conway, M. A., & Morris, P. E. (Eds.), Theories of memory (p. 189209). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A. S. C., & the ALSPAC Team (2005). Developmental consequences of phonological loop deficits during early childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 598611.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., & Sedey, A. L. (2011). Language and verbal reasoning skills in adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear and Hearing, 32(1 Suppl), 39S48S.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J. G., Tobey, E., & Davidson, L. (2016). Persistent language delay versus late language emergence in children with early cochlear implantation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 59, 155170.Google Scholar
Gottardis, L., Nunes, T., & Lunt, I. (2011). A synthesis of research on deaf and hearing children’s mathematical achievement. Deafness & Education International, 13, 131150.Google Scholar
Hall, M. L., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Working memory, deafness, and sign language. In Marshark, M. & Spencer, P. E. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (Vol. 2, pp. 458472). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, M. L., Hall, W. C., & Caselli, N. K. (2019). Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Language, 39(4), 367395.Google Scholar
Hanson, V. L. (1986). Access to spoken language and the acquisition of orthographic structure: Evidence from deaf readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 193212.Google Scholar
Harlor, A. D. B., & Bower, C. (2009). Hearing assessment in infants and children: Recommendations beyond neonatal screening. Pediatrics, 124, 12521263.Google Scholar
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonological coding: evidence from reading, spelling, and working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education, 9(3), 253268.Google Scholar
Harris, M. S., Kronenberger, W. G., Gao, S., Hoen, H. M., Miyamoto, R. T., & Pisoni, D. B. (2013). Verbal short-term memory development and spoken language outcomes in deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 34(2), 179192.Google Scholar
Henry, L. A, Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 3745Google Scholar
Jones, A., Atkinson, J., Marshall, C. R., Botting, N., St Clair, M., & Morgan, G. (2020). Expressive vocabulary predicts non-verbal executive function: A 2-year longitudinal study of deaf and hearing children. Child Development, 91,e400–14.63.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122149.Google Scholar
Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233246.Google Scholar
Kapa, L. L., & Erikson, J. A. (2019). Variability of executive function performance in pre-schoolers with developmental language disorder. Seminars in Speech and Language, 40(4), 243255.Google Scholar
Keehner, M., & Atkinson, J. (2006). Working memory and deafness: Implications for cognitive development and functioning. In Pickering, Susan J. (Ed.), Working memory and education. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Koo, D., Crain, K., LaSasso, C., & Eden, G. (2008). Phonological awareness and short-term memory in hearing and deaf individuals of different communication backgrounds. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, pp. 8399.Google Scholar
Kronenberger, W. G., Pisoni, D. B., Henning, S. C., & Colson, B. G. (2013). Executive functioning skills in long-term users of cochlear implants: A case control study. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38, 902914.Google Scholar
Lauro, L. J. R., Crespi, M., Papagno, C., & Cecchetto, C. (2014). Making sense of an unexpected detrimental effect of sign language use in a visual task. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19 (3), 358365.Google Scholar
Lina-Granade, G., Comte-Gervais, I., Gippon, L., Nappez, G., Morin, E., & Truy, E. (2010). Correlation between cognitive abilities and language level in cochlear implanted children. Cochlear Implants International, 11(Suppl. 1), 328331.Google Scholar
López-Crespo, G., Daza, M. T., & Méndez-López, M. (2012). Visual working memory in deaf children with diverse communication modes: Improvement by differential outcomes. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 362368.Google Scholar
Lu, J., Jones, A., & Morgan, G. (2016). The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners. Journal of Child Language, 43, 537552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacSweeney, M., Campbell, R., & Donlan, C. (1996). Varieties of short-term memory coding in deaf teenagers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(4), 249262.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., Rhoten, C., & Fabich, M. (2007). Effects of cochlear implants on children’s reading and academic achievement. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 269282.Google Scholar
Marshall, C., Jones, A., Denmark, T., Mason, K., Atkinson, J., Botting, N., & Morgan, G. (2015). Deaf children’s non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 527.Google Scholar
Miller, P. (2001). Communication mode and the information processing capacity of Hebrew readers with prelingually acquired deafness. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 13(2001), 8396Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138163.Google Scholar
National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE). (2019). Cochlear implants for children and adults with severe to profound deafness. NICE technology appraisal guidance 566. [online]. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ta566Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Sansom, E., Low, K., Rice, C., & Caldwell-Tarr, A. (2014). Language structures used by kindergartners with cochlear implants: Relationship to phonological awareness, lexical knowledge and hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 35(5), 506518.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Caldwell, A., Lowenstein, J.H., Tarr, E., & Holloman, C. (2012). Emergent literacy in kindergartners with cochlear implants, Ear Hear, 33(6) 683697.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Caldwell-Tarr, A., & Lowenstein, J. H. (2013). Working memory in children with cochlear implants: Problems are in storage, not processing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 77(11), 18861898.Google Scholar
Nouwens, S., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). How working memory relates to children’s reading comprehension: The importance of domain-specificity in storage and processing. Reading and Writing, 30(1), 105120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, N., & Hermelin, B. (1972). Seeing and hearing and time and space. Perception and Psychophysics, 11, 4648.Google Scholar
Pierce, L., Genesee, F., Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 12651300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., & Cleary, M. (2003). Measures of working memory span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24(1 Suppl), 106S120S.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., Kronenberger, W. G., Roman, A. S., & Geers, A. E. ( 2011). Measures of digit span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after more than 10 years of cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 32, 60S74S.Google Scholar
Power, D. (2005). Models of deafness: Cochlear implants in the Australian daily press. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10, 451459.Google Scholar
Soleymani, Z., Amidfar, M., Dadgar, H., & Jalaei, S. (2014). Working memory in Farsi-speaking children with normal development and cochlear implant. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, D. J., McGregor, K. K., & Bentler, R. A. (2012). Vocabulary and working memory in children fit with hearing aids. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 154167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torkildsen, J. V., Arciuli, J., Haukedal, C. L., & Wie, O. B. (2018). Does a lack of auditory experience affect sequential learning? Cognition, 170, 123129.Google Scholar
Vugs, B., Knoors, H., Cuperus, J., Hendriks, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Executive function training in children with SLI: A pilot study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 33(1), 4766.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Studies in communication, thought and language. In Hanfmann, E. & Vakar, G. (Eds.), MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1997). A visuospatial “phonological loop” in working memory: Evidence from American Sign Language. Memory & Cognition, 25(3), 313320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wingfield, A., Tun, P. A., & McCoy, S. L. (2005). Hearing loss in older adulthood: What it is and how it interacts with cognitive performance. Current Directions in Psycholological Science, 14, 144148.Google Scholar
Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental Psychobiology, 44(1), 6883.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×