Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T10:22:11.150Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part IV - Views of the Interdisciplinary Dialogue: From Developmental Science and Sociology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2019

Ross D. Parke
Affiliation:
University of California, Riverside
Glen H. Elder, Jr.
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Get access

Summary

Philosophers through the ages have stressed time as a critical variable in life, and developmentalists today pretend to a lifespan framework. Paradoxically, many construals of time are still neglected in developmental science. This chapter focuses on time and fleshes out three developmental perspectives on time: the chronosystem from the bioecological systems framework, transaction, and specificity.

Bioecological systems theory characterizes development as a joint function of process, person, context, and time. The principle of transaction in development asserts that characteristics of individuals shape their experiences, and reciprocally experiences shape the characteristics of individuals, through time. Finally, the specificity principle contends that understanding lifespan development depends critically on what is studied in whom, how, and when. Time fits integrally into each prevailing developmental perspective.

Type
Chapter
Information
Children in Changing Worlds
Sociocultural and Temporal Perspectives
, pp. 275 - 298
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (1995) Family and child factors in stability and change in children’s aggressiveness in elementary school. In McCord, J. (Ed.), Coercion and Punishment in Long-Term Perspectives (pp. 124138). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 179197.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1995). Form and function: Implications for studies of culture and human development. Culture & Psychology, 1, 123137.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In Bornstein, M. H. & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Ecological Settings and Processes in Developmental Systems, Vol. 4: Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science (7th ed., pp. 55132). Editor-in-Chief: R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2017). The specificity principle in acculturation science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 345.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., & Esposito, G. (2017). Continuity and stability in development. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 113119.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., Rahn, C., Galperín, C. Z., Pêcheux, M.-G., Lamour, M., Azuma, H., Toda, S., Ogino, M., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1992). Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology, 28, 593603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Lerner, R. M. (Ed.), Damon, W. (Series Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development (6th ed., pp. 793828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Elder, G. H., Shanahan, M. J., & Jennings, J. A. (2015). Human development in time and place. In Bornstein, M. H. & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Ecological Settings and Processes in Developmental Systems (Vol. 4, pp. 654). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar

References

Kariya, T. & Rosenbaum, J. E. (2003). Stratified incentives and life course behaviors. In Mortimer, J. T. & Shanahan, M. J. (eds.), Handbook of the Life Course (Vol. 1, pp. 5178). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Mortimer, J. T. & Moen, P. (2016). The changing social construction of age and the life course: Precarious identity and enactment of “early” and “encore” stages of adulthood. In Shanahan, M. J., Mortimer, J. T., & Johnson, M. K. (eds.), Handbook of the Life Course (Vol. 2, pp. 111129). New York: Springer.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×