Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:42:07.714Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 10 - Cultural Evolutionary Psychology of Belief

from Evolutionary Psychology of Beliefs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2022

Julien Musolino
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Joseph Sommer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Pernille Hemmer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we consider a cultural evolutionary psychological framework for understanding the origin, maintenance and diffusion of beliefs, and illustrate the utility of such a framework with two case studies – religious belief systems and conspiracy theory beliefs. A cultural evolutionary psychology of belief considers four broad sets of interacting factors: the content of a belief, a belief’s fit with individual conditions, the social dynamics surrounding a belief, and the socioecological conditions that promote or suppress a belief. A cultural evolutionary psychology of belief overcomes the limitations to what we call standard evolutionary psychology, a school of thought that emphasizes the activation of innate cognitive modules for understanding the generation and spread of beliefs. With this chapter, then, we aim to show how social and cognitive science researchers can approach the study of beliefs from an evolutionary perspective without committing to the controversial assumptions of standard evolutionary psychology.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cognitive Science of Belief
A Multidisciplinary Approach
, pp. 209 - 230
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acerbi, A. (2019) Cultural evolution in the digital age. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Acerbi, A. & Tehrani, J. J. (2018) Did Einstein really say that? Testing content versus context in the cultural selection of quotations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 18(3–4), 293311.Google Scholar
Altay, S., de Araujo, E., & Mercier, H. (2020) “If this account is true, it is most enormously wonderful”: interestingness-if-true and the sharing of true and false news. Digital Journalism, 10(3), 373394. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tdfh5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, K., Gravelle, T. B., Osmundsen, M., Petersen, M. B., Reifler, J., & Scotto, T. J. (2021). Some people just want to watch the world burn: the prevalence, psychology and politics of the “Need for Chaos.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1822), 20200147. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0147Google Scholar
Atran, S. & Henrich, J. (2010) The evolution of religion: how cognitive byproducts, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual displays, and group competition generate deep commitments to prosocial religions. Biological Theory, 5(1), 1830.Google Scholar
Atran, S., Medin, D., Ross, N. et al. (2002) Folkecology, cultural epidemiology, and the spirit of the commons: a garden experiment in the Maya lowlands, 1991–2001. Current Anthropology, 43(3), 421450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2020) Deciding what to observe: thoughts for a post-WEIRD generation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 445453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (2000) Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 2934.Google Scholar
Barsbai, T., Lukas, D., & Pondorfer, A. (2021) Local convergence of behavior across species. Science, 371(6526), 292295.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. (2019) Sense or non-sense? A critical discussion of a recent evolutionary–cognitive approach to “folk-economic beliefs”. Evolution, Mind and Behaviour, 17(1), 2947.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. (2020) How cultural evolution can inform the science of science communication – and vice versa. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 7(1), 135. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-00634-4Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. & Purzycki, B. G. (2021a) Competing forces account for the stability and evolution of religious beliefs. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 31(4), 307312. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2020.1844969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendixen, T. & Purzycki, B. G. (2021b) Peering into the minds of gods: what cross-cultural variation in gods’ concerns can tell us about the evolution of religion. Journal for the Cognitive Science of Religion, 5(2), 142165.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T., Apicella, C. L., Atkinson, Q. et al. (forthcoming) Appealing to the minds of gods: Religious beliefs and appeals correspond to features of local social ecologies. Religion, Brain & Behavior. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tjn3eGoogle Scholar
Bentzen, J. S. (2019) Acts of God? Religiosity and natural disasters across subnational world districts. The Economic Journal, 129(622), 22952321.Google Scholar
Berl, R. E. W., Samarasinghe, A. N., Roberts, S. G., Jordan, F. M., & Gavin, M. C. (2021). Prestige and content biases together shape the cultural transmission of narratives. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 3, 122. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.37Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Brown, G. R., Richardson, R. C., & Laland, K. N. (2011) Darwin in mind: new opportunities for evolutionary psychology. PLOS Biology, 9(7), e1001109.Google Scholar
Botero, C. A., Gardner, B., Kirby, K. R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin, M. C., & Gray, R. D. (2014) The ecology of religious beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(47), 1678416789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudry, M. (2020) The truth is (still) out there: On the epistemology and cultural dynamics of conspiracy beliefs. http://philsci-archive.pitt.edu/17699/.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. (2017) A different kind of animal: how culture transformed our species. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1992) Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171195.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained: the evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. & Petersen, M. B. (2018) Folk-economic beliefs: an evolutionary cognitive model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e158.Google Scholar
Brotherton, R. (2015) Suspicious minds: why we believe conspiracy theories. Bloomsbury Sigma.Google Scholar
Buskell, A., Enquist, M., & Jansson, F. (2019) A systems approach to cultural evolution. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 115.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2019) Evolutionary psychology: the new science of the mind, 6th ed. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chudek, M., Zhao, W. & Henrich, J. (2013) Culture-gene coevolution, large-scale cooperation, and the shaping of human social psychology. In Sterelny, K., Richard, J., Brett, C., & Ben, F. (Eds.). Cooperation and its evolution, life and mind: philosophical issues in biology and psychology (pp. 425457). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S. et al. (2010) Evolutionary psychology: controversies, questions, prospects, and limitations. The American Psychologist, 65(2), 110126.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989) The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31(3), 187276.Google Scholar
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017) Cultural evolutionary theory: how culture evolves and why it matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 77827789.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016) Someone is pulling the strings: hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1), 5777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017) The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538542.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M. et al. (2019) Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40(S1), 335.Google Scholar
Fogarty, L. & Creanza, N. (2017) The niche construction of cultural complexity: interactions between innovations, population size and the environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1735), 20160428.Google Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E. & Ploeger, A. (2007) Evolutionary psychology versus Fodor: arguments for and against the massive modularity hypothesis. Philosophical Psychology, 20(6), 687710.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2006) Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry, 17(2), 7595.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M. & Henrich, J. (2010) The Zeus problem: why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10(3–4), 383389.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., Najle, M. B., & Caluori, N. (2019) The origins of religious disbelief: A dual inheritance approach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(7), 13691379. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/e29rt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfinch, A. (2015). Rethinking evolutionary psychology. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Gray, R. D., Heaney, M., & Fairhall, S. (2003) Evolutionary psychology and the challenge of adaptive expansion. In Sterelny, K., & Fitness, J., J. (Eds.). From mating to mentality: evaluating evolutionary psychology (pp. 247268). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Kalish, M. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008) Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 35033514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, S. E. (1995) Faces in the clouds: anew theory of religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hagen, E. H. (2015) Evolutionary psychology and its critics. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilov’a, J., & Purzycki, B. G. (2019) War increases religiosity. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 129135.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & McElreath, R. (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(3), 123135.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2019). Precis of cognitive gadgets: the cultural evolution of thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, e169.Google Scholar
Hill, K. & Boyd, R. (2021) Behavioral convergence in humans and animals. Science, 371(6526), 235236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansson, F., Aguilar, E., Acerbi, A., & Enquist, M. (2021) Modelling cultural systems and selective filters. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1828), 20200045. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, J. (2019) What is religion? 2nd ed. Routledge.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. D. P. (2016) God is watching you: how the fear of god makes us human. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. (2010) Fixing the communications failure. Nature, 463(7279), 296297.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. & Landrum, A. R. (2017) A tale of two vaccines – and their science communication environments. In Jamieson, K. H., Kahan, D., & Scheufele, D. (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. M. (2015) What is the “science of science communication”? SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 2562025, Social Science Research Network, Rochester, NY.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H. S., Hooper, P. L., & Gurven, M. (2009) The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1533), 32893299.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., Bain, P. G., & Perfors, A. (2019). The psychology of cultural dynamics: what is it, what do we know, and what is yet to be known? Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 499529. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103112Google Scholar
Kendal, R. L., Boogert, N. J., Rendell, L., Laland, K. N., Webster, M., & Jones, P. L. (2018) Social learning strategies: bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(7), 651665.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2017) Darwin’s unfinished symphony: how culture made the human mind. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Leman, P. J. & Cinnirella, M. (2013) Beliefs in conspiracy theories and the need for cognitive closure. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 378.Google Scholar
Lifshitz, M., van Elk, M., & Luhrmann, T. M. (2019) Absorption and spiritual experience: a review of evidence and potential mechanisms. Consciousness and Cognition, 73, 102760.Google Scholar
Luhrmann, T. M., Weisman, K., Aulino, F. et al. (2021) Sensing the presence of gods and spirits across cultures and faiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(5) e2016649118.Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J. & Wilson, E. O. (1981) Genes, mind, and culture: the coevolutionary process. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mauritsen, A. L., Bendixen, T., & Christensen, H. R. (2022). Does a Pandemic Increase Religiosity in a Secular Nation? A Longitudinal Examination. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qsgejGoogle Scholar
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001) Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 415444.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011) Cultural evolution: how Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2019) Cultural evolution and cultural psychology. In Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (Eds.). Handbook of Cultural Psychology, 2nd ed. (pp. 144162). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Dall, S. R. X., & Thornton, A. (2016) The evolution of individual and cultural variation in social learning. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31(3), 215225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. M. (2020) Do COVID-19 conspiracy theory beliefs form a monological belief system? Canadian Journal of Political Science/Revue Canadienne de Science Politique, 53(2), 319326.Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidi’ere, N., & Mercier, H. (2015) Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303312.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. (2004) Is religion what we want? Motivation and the cultural transmission of religious representations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(2), 347371.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M. et al. (2016) The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E1.Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, J. & Laland, K. N. (2011) Ecological inheritance and cultural inheritance: what are they and how do they differ? Biological Theory, 6(3), 220230.Google Scholar
Peoples, H. C., Duda, P., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016) Hunter-gatherers and the origins of religion. Human Nature, 27(3), 261282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietraszewski, D. & Wertz, A. E. (2022). Why evolutionary psychology should abandon modularity. Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 17(2), 465490. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621997113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prooijen, J.-W. (2018) The psychology of conspiracy theories. Routledge.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2011) Tyvan cher eezi and the socioecological constraints of supernatural agents’ minds. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 3145.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2013) The minds of gods: a comparative study of supernatural agency. Cognition, 129(1), 163179.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2016) The evolution of gods’ minds in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology, 57(S13), S88S104.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Finkel, D. N., Shaver, J., Wales, N., Cohen, A. B., & Sosis, R. (2012) What does god know? Supernatural agents’ access to socially strategic and non-strategic information. Cognitive Science, 36(5), 846869.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. & McKay, R. (2022). Moralistic Gods and Social Complexity: A Brief History of the Problem. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vqjk2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. & McNamara, R. A. (2016) An ecological theory of gods’ minds. In De Cruz, H., & Nichols, R., R. (Eds.). Cognitive science of religion and its philosophical implications (pp. 143167). Continuum.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, N., Bollen, J., & Ahn, Y.-Y. (2016) Collective dynamics of belief evolution under cognitive coherence and social conformity. PLOS One, 11(11), e0165910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roes, F. L. & Raymond, M. (2003) Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 126135.Google Scholar
Rossano, M. J. (2007) Supernaturalizing social life: religion and the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature, 18(3), 272294.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009) Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 199207.Google Scholar
Shapiro, L. & Epstein, W. (1998) Evolutionary theory meets cognitive psychology: a more selective perspective. Mind & Language, 13(2), 171194.Google Scholar
Sibley, C. G. & Bulbulia, J. (2012). Faith after an Earthquake: A Longitudinal Study of Religion and Perceived Health before and after the 2011 Christchurch New Zealand Earthquake. PLOS ONE, 7(12), e49648. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049648Google Scholar
Skoggard, I., Ember, C. R., Pitek, E., Jackson, J. C., & Carolus, C. (2020) Resource stress predicts changes in religious belief and increases in sharing behavior. Human Nature, 31(3), 249271.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. (2000) Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In Cronk, L., Chagnon, N., Irons, & W. (Eds.). Adaptation and human behavior: an anthropological perspective (pp. 2746). Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Smolla, M., Jansson, F., Lehmann, L. et al. (2021) Underappreciated features of cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1828), 20200259. https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/3xvdw/Google Scholar
Snarey, J. (1996) The natural environment’s impact upon religious ethics: a cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 35(2), 8596.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. & Bulbulia, J. (2011) The behavioral ecology of religion: the benefits and costs of one evolutionary approach. Religion, 41(3), 341362.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Cl’ement, F., Heintz, C. et al. (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359393.Google Scholar
Sørensen, J. (2004) Religion, evolution and an immunology of cultural systems. Evolution and Cognition, 10(1), 6173.Google Scholar
Stubbersfield, J. M. (2021). Conspiracy Theories: A Cultural Evolution Theory approach. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/hqkjuGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J. (2020) Evolutionary psychology as the crystalizing core of a unified modern social science. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 14(4),390403.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19136). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1920) Primitive culture: researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, language, art, and custom. Murray.Google Scholar
Uscinski, J. E. (2020) Conspiracy theories: aprimer. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
van Mulukom, V., Pummerer, L. J., Alper, S. et al. (2022). Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: a systematic review. Social Science & Medicine, 301, 114912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114912Google Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W. & Grossmann, I. (2017). Cultural change: the how and the why. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 956972. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617699971CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willard, A. K. & Cingl, L. (2017) Testing theories of secularization and religious belief in the Czech Republic and Slovakia. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 604615.Google Scholar
Willard, A. K., Henrich, J., & Norenzayan, A. (2016) Memory and belief in the transmission of counterintuitive content. Human Nature, 27(3), 221243.Google Scholar
Williams, D. (2021) Socially adaptive belief. Mind & Language, 36(3), 333354. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12294.Google Scholar
Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012) Dead and alive: beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(6), 767773.Google Scholar
Yeh, D. J., Fogarty, L., & Kandler, A. (2018) Cultural linkage: the influence of package transmission on cultural dynamics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1916), 20191951.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×