Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T12:29:08.696Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 9 - Do Cultural Misbeliefs Cause Costly Behavior?

from Evolutionary Psychology of Beliefs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2022

Julien Musolino
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Joseph Sommer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Pernille Hemmer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Cultural misbeliefs are false beliefs that are widely spread through social transmission, such as rumors or conspiracy theories. These misbeliefs are often feared to cause much damage, as people engage in costly actions on their basis. However, it is possible for people to hold a belief in such a way that the belief only has limited impact on their thoughts or behavior – if they hold a belief reflectively, by contrast with intuitively. Here, we argue that (i) most cultural misbeliefs are held reflectively and that (ii) they rarely directly cause costly behavior. We note that different cultures use different misbeliefs to justify similar behaviors, suggesting that the beliefs are secondary to the behavior. Moreover, misbeliefs usually do not have the consequences they would have if they were held intuitively, causing other types of (usually less costly) behaviors instead.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cognitive Science of Belief
A Multidisciplinary Approach
, pp. 193 - 208
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrett, J. L. (1999) Theological correctness: cognitive constraint and the study of religion. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 11(4), 325339.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L. & Keil, F. C. (1996) Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219247.Google Scholar
Baumard, N. & Boyer, P. (2013) Religious beliefs as reflective elaborations on intuitions: a modified dual-process model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 295300.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. & Liénard, P. (2006) Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595613.Google Scholar
Coury, C. (1967) The basic principles of medicine in the primitive mind. Medical History, 11(2), 111127.Google Scholar
Darley, J. M. & Latané, B. (1968) Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4p1), 377383.Google Scholar
de Figueiredo, A., Simas, C., Karafillakis, E., Paterson, P., & Larson, H. J. (2020) Mapping global trends in vaccine confidence and investigating barriers to vaccine uptake: a large-scale retrospective temporal modelling study. The Lancet, 396(10255), 898908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiFonzo, N. & Bordia, P. (2007) Rumor psychology: social and organizational approaches. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diggory, J. C. (1956) Some consequences of proximity to a disease threat. Sociometry, 19(1), 4753.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M. (2021) Are conspiracy theories harmless? The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 24, E13.Google Scholar
Durbach, N. (2000) “They might as well brand us”: working-class resistance to compulsory vaccination in Victorian England. Social History of Medicine, 13(1), 4563.Google Scholar
Horowitz, D. L. (2001) The deadly ethnic riot. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hortensius, R. & de Gelder, B. (2018) From empathy to apathy: the bystander effect revisited. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 249256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, J. (2011) Among the Truthers: a journey through America’s growing conspiracist underground. Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Kim, J. W. & Kim, E. (2019) Identifying the effect of political rumor diffusion using variations in survey timing. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 14(3), 293311.Google Scholar
Leeson, P. T. (2012) Ordeals. The Journal of Law and Economics, 55(3), 691714.Google Scholar
McKay, R. T. & Dennett, D. C. (2009) The evolution of misbelief. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(06), 493510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercier, H. (2020a) Not born yesterday: the science of who we trust and what we believe. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. (2020b) The cultural evolution of oaths, ordeals, and lie detectors. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 20(3–4), 159187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, H. & Boyer, P. (2020) Truth-making institutions: from divination, ordeals and oaths to judicial torture and rules of evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 2(3), 259267.Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidière, N., & Mercier, H. (2015) Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303312.Google Scholar
Miton, H. & Mercier, H. (2015) Cognitive obstacles to pro-vaccination beliefs. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 633636.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. (2021) Research note: examining false beliefs about voter fraud in the wake of the 2020 Presidential Election. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-51Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2020) The evolutionary psychology of mass mobilization: how disinformation and demagogues coordinate rather than manipulate. Current Opinion in Psychology, 35, 7175.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. & Rappaport, R. A. R. (1999) Ritual and religion in the making of humanity (Vol. 110). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Regehr, C., Goldberg, G., & Hughes, J. (2002) Exposure to human tragedy, empathy, and trauma in ambulance paramedics. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72(4), 505513.Google Scholar
Santoro, D. & Kumar, M. (2018) Speaking truth to power – a theory of whistleblowing (Vol. 6). Springer.Google Scholar
Shibutani, T. (1966) Improvised news. A sociological study of rumor. Bobbs-Merrill Company.Google Scholar
Siegler, A. J., Mbwambo, J. K., McCarty, F. A., & DiClemente, R. J. (2012) Condoms “contain worms” and “cause HIV” in Tanzania: Negative Condom Beliefs Scale development and implications for HIV prevention. Social Science & Medicine, 75(9), 16851691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, D., Kaltenthaler, K., & Dagher, M. (2021) Seeing is disbelieving: the depths and limits of factual misinformation in war. International Studies Quarterly, 65(3), 798810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, M. J., Ellenberg, S. S., Bell, L. M., & Rubin, D. M. (2008) Media coverage of the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine and autism controversy and its relationship to MMR immunization rates in the United States. Pediatrics, 121(4), e836e843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1997) Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language, 12(1), 6783.Google Scholar
Szegőfi, Á. (unpublished document) Blood Libels as Evolved Coalition Signals.Google Scholar
Torrey, N. L. (1961) Les philosophes. the philosophers of the enlightenment and modern democracy. Capricorn Books.Google Scholar
Uscinski, J. E. (2020) Conspiracy theories: a primer. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×