Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:04:08.729Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - The Role of Word-Formation Families and Subfamilies in the Organization of German Diminutive Compounds

from Part III - Corpus-Based Case Studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2020

Lívia Körtvélyessy
Affiliation:
P. J. Šafárik University, Košice, Slovakia
Pavol Štekauer
Affiliation:
P. J. Šafárik University, Košice, Slovakia
Get access

Summary

Not only formal word families are important for the organization of the morphologically complex lexicon, but also their semantic (or rather conceptual) subfamilies. This is shown with eighteen Austrian German diminutive compound families with the diminutive suffix -chen, and for their correspondent formal word families with the diminutive suffix -erl. For all of them, their competition (leading to mutual avoidances and semantic specializations), degree of morphosemantic transparency/opacity, productivity and left- vs. right-branching are accounted for. Our electronic database consists of nearly 13,000 different diminutive types which have at least five tokens within the Austrian Media Corpus.

Type
Chapter
Information
Complex Words
Advances in Morphology
, pp. 293 - 310
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bauer, L. (1997). Evaluative morphology: a search for universals. Studies in Language, 21, 533575.Google Scholar
Bauer, L. (2001). Morphological Productivity, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benveniste, É. (1963). Une valeur du diminutif. Prace Filologiczne. Warszawa: Państwowe wydawnictwo naukowe, 18, 911.Google Scholar
Chanpira, E. I. (1966). Ob okkazional'nom slove i okkazional'nom slovoobrazovanii [On occasional word and occasional word formation]. In Zemskaja, E. A. and Šmelev, D. N., eds., Razvitie slovoobrazovanija sovremennogo russkogo jazyka [Development of Word Formation of the Present Russian Language], Moskva: Nauka, pp. 153166.Google Scholar
Coseriu, E. (1978). Einführung in die strukturelle Betrachtung der Wortschatzes [Introduction into the structural study of the lexicon]. In Geckeler, H., ed., Strukturelle Bedeutungslehre, Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.Google Scholar
Dal, G. (1997). Grammaire du suffixe -et(te), Paris: Didier Érudition.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U. and Merlini Barbaresi, L. (1994). Morphopragmatics, Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Merlini Barbaresi, L., Schwaiger, S., Ransmayr, J. and Korecky-Kröll, K. (2019). Rivalry and lack of blocking among Italian and German diminutives in adult and child language. In Rainer, F., Gardani, F., Luschützky, H.-C. and Dressler, W. U., eds., Competition in Inflection and Word-Formation, Cham: Springer, pp. 123143.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U. and Tumfart, B. (2017). New corpus-linguistic approaches to the investigation of poetic occasionalisms: the case of Johann Nepomuk Nestroy. Yearbook of the Poznan Linguistic Meeting, 3, 155166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fradin, B. (2007). Le traitement de la suffixation en -etLangages 37, 5177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fradin, B., Hathout, N. and Meunier, F. (2003). French suffixation in -et and the question of productivity. Langue française, 140, 5678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagné, C. L. and Spalding, T. L. (2014). Conceptual composition: the role of relational competition in the comprehension of modifier-noun phrases and noun-noun compounds. In Ross, B., ed., The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, New York: Elsevier, pp. 97130Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L. and Spalding, T. L. (2015). Semantics, concepts, and meta-cognition: Attributing properties and meanings to complex concepts. In Bauer, L., Körtvélyessy, L. and Stekauer, P., eds., Semantics of Complex Words, New York: Springer, pp. 925.Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L., Spalding, T. L. and Kostelecky, M. (2017). Conceptual combination, property inclusion, and the Artistotelian-Thomistic view of concepts. In Hampton, J. and Winter, Y., eds., Compositionality and Concepts in Linguistics and Psychology: Language, Cognition and Mind, New York: Springer, pp. 223245.Google Scholar
Hay, J. B. and Baayen, H. R. (2002). Parsing and productivity. In Booij, G. and van Marle, J., eds., Yearbook of Morphology 2001, Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 203235.Google Scholar
Jurafsky, D. (1996). Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language, 72, 533578.Google Scholar
Mattiello, E. and Dressler, W. U. (2019a). The morphosemantic transparency/opacity of novel English analogical compounds and compound families. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia, 53, 67114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattiello, E. and Dressler, W. U. (2019b). Dualism and superposition in the analysis of English synthetic compounds ending in -er [manuscript].Google Scholar
Mulder, K., Dijkstra, T., Schreuder, R. and Baayen, H. R. (2014). Effects of primary and secondary morphological family size in monolingual and bilingual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 5984.Google Scholar
Ransmayr, J., Schwaiger, S. and Dressler, W. U. (forthcoming). Semantische Untersuchungen zu österreichischen Diminutiven und Diminutivkomposita: Begriffsfelder und Graduierung der morphosemantischen Opazität. Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik.Google Scholar
Ransmayr, J., Schwaiger, S., Ďurčo, M., Pirker, H. and Dressler, W. U. (2016). Graduierung der Transparenz von Diminutiven auf -chen: Eine korpuslinguistische Untersuchung. Deutsche Sprache, 44, 261286.Google Scholar
Schwaiger, S., Barbaresi, A., Korecky-Kröll, K., Ransmayr, J. and Dressler, W. U. (2019). Diminutivvariation in österreichischen elektronischen Korpora. In Bülow, L., Fischer, A. K. and Herbert, K., eds., Dimensions of Linguistic Space: Variation – Multilingualism – Conceptualisations. Dimensionen des sprachlichen Raums: Variation – Mehrsprachigkeit – Konzeptualisierung. Schriften zur deutschen Sprache in Österreich 45, Berlin: Peter Lang, pp. 147162.Google Scholar
Schwaiger, S., Ransmayr, J., Korecky-Kröll, K., Sommer-Lolei, S. and Dressler, W. U. (2017). Scaling morphosemantic transparency/opacity. A corpus-linguistic and acquisitionist study of German diminutives. Yearbook of the Poznan Linguistics Meeting, 3, 141153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953/2001). Philosophische Untersuchungen [Philosophical Investigations]. Kritisch-genetische Edition, Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×