Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T03:05:56.597Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - The integration of body representations and other inferential systems in infancy

from Part II - The bodies of others

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 October 2011

Virginia Slaughter
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Celia A. Brownell
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Get access

Summary

Do infants reason about the object kind human, as represented by the human body, in a more mature fashion than they do other object kinds? When adults recognize an object as belonging to a certain kind (e.g. dog), this licenses a wide variety of inferences, including kind-specific inferences, like whether the object barks, and more abstract inferences, such as whether the object’s behavior is goal-directed. It also allows adults to track or count individuals in ambiguous contexts (Bonatti et al., 2002; Hall, 1998; Hirsch, 1982; MacNamara, 1986; Spelke, 1990; Xu, 1999, 2007; Xu and Carey, 1996; Xu et al., 1999). For example, if a small, furry, white dog goes out the door and sometime later a small, furry, white cat comes back in, adults know they have encountered two individuals because adults understand that dogs do not become cats. Even within a single point in time, kind designations help adults to individuate objects even when they are partially occluded. A tail and a nose poking out from behind opposite sides of a bush suggest one dog, but a tail poking out from behind each side indicates two dogs. Infants, on the other hand, are considerably less able to use kind information in such situations (Xu, 1999, 2007; Xu and Carey, 1996; Xu et al., 1999).

The current studies examine whether infants reason more maturely when the object in question belongs to the kind human. This possibility was suggested by Bonatti and colleagues (2002) on the basis of infants’ skill with human faces. One suggested explanation for this advantage is the status of humans as the prototypical goal-directed object. The current studies rule out this explanation by showing that (1) when the exemplar object used to represent the kind was a human hand rather than a doll’s face, the advantage disappeared, and (2) this was so even when goal attributions to the hand were deliberately primed. Specifically, this series of studies showed that although 9- to 10-month-old infants recognize and represent the human hand “well enough” to encode its behavior in terms of goals, they do not recognize or represent it “well enough” to track a hand through occlusions on the basis of its appearance alone. We use this dissociation to argue for a lack of integration across different types of representational/inferential systems in infancy, specifically with respect to body representations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baillargeon, R. 1986 Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infantsCognition 23 21Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I.Proffitt, D. R.Spetner, N. B.Thomas, M. A. 1985 The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motionsChild Development 56 531Google Scholar
Binkofski, F.Buccino, G. 2006 The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understandingJournal of Physiology – Paris 99 396Google Scholar
Blangero, A.Menz, M. M.McNamara, A.Binkofski, F. 2009 Parietal modules for reachingNeuropsychologia 47 500Google Scholar
Bonatti, L.Frot, E.Zangl, R.Mehler, J. 2002 The human first hypothesis: Identification of conspecifics and individuation of objects in the young infantCognitive Psychology 44 388Google Scholar
Bower, T. G. R. 1974 Development in infancySan Francisco, CAW. H. Freeman and Company
Carey, S.Xu, F. 2001 Infants’ knowledge of objects: Beyond object files and object trackingCognition 80 179Google Scholar
Caron, A. J. 2009 Comprehension of the representational mind in infancyDevelopmental Review 29 69Google Scholar
DeCasper, A.Fifer, W. 1980 Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother’s voicesScience 208 174Google Scholar
Downing, P. E.Jiang, Y.Shuman, M.Kanwisher, N. 2001 A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human bodyScience 293 470Google Scholar
Downing, P. E.Peelen, M. V.Wiggett, A. J.Tew, B. D. 2006 The role of the extrastriate body area in action perceptionSocial Neuroscience 1 52Google Scholar
Ellsworth, C.Muir, D.Hains, S. 1993 Social competence and person–object differentiation: An analysis of the still-face effectDevelopmental Psychology 29 63Google Scholar
Fabbri-Destro, M.Rizzolatti, G. 2008 Mirror neurons and mirror systems in monkeys and humansPhysiology 23 171Google Scholar
Filimon, F.Nelson, J. D.Hagler, D. J.Sereno, M. I. 2007 Human cortical representations for reaching: Mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imageryNeuroimage 37 315Google Scholar
Fox, R.McDaniel, C. 1982 The perception of biological motion by human infantsScience 218 486Google Scholar
Frye, D.Rawling, P.Moore, C.Meyers, I. 1983 Object–person discrimination and communication at 3 and 10 monthsDevelopmental Psychology 19 303Google Scholar
Gallese, V.Goldman, A. 1998 Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-readingTrends in Cognitive Science 2 493Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A.Milner, A. D. 1992 Separate visual pathways for perception and actionTrends in Neurosciences 15 20Google Scholar
Grol, M. J.Majdandzić, J.Stephan, K. E.Verhagen, L.Dijkerman, H. C.Bekkering, H. 2007 Parieto-frontal connectivity during visually guided graspingJournal of Neuroscience 27 877Google Scholar
Hall, D. G. 1998 Continuity and the persistence of objects: When the whole is greater than the sum of the partsCognitive Psychology 37 28Google Scholar
Haxby, J. V.Hoffman, E. A.Gobbini, M. I. 2000 The distributed human neural system for face perceptionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 4 223Google Scholar
Hirsch, E. 1982 The Concept of IdentityNew YorkOxford University Press
Jellema, T.Baker, C. I.Wicker, B.Perrett, D. I. 2000 Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actionsBrain and Cognition 44 280Google Scholar
Kaldy, Z.Leslie, A. 2003 Identification of objects in 9-month-old infants: Integrating “what” and “where” informationDevelopmental Science 6 360Google Scholar
Klein, R. P.Jennings, K. D. 1979 Responses to social and inanimate stimuli in early infancyThe Journal of Genetic Psychology 135 3Google Scholar
Krojgaard, P. 2000 Object individuation in 10-month-old infants: Do significant objects make a difference?Cognitive Development 15 169Google Scholar
Kuhlmeier, V. A.Bloom, P.Wynn, K. 2004 Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects?Cognition 94 95Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.Pomerleau, A.Malcuit, G.Feider, H. 1987 The development of infants’ responses to people and a doll: Implications for research in communicationInfant Behavior and Development 10 81Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. 1984 Infant perception of a manual pick-up eventBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 19Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M.Xu, F.Tremoulet, P. D.Scholl, B. J. 1998 Indexing and the object concept: Developing “what” and “where” systemsTrends in Cognitive Science 2 10Google Scholar
MacFarlane, A. 1975 Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonateCiba Foundation SymposiumParent–child InteractionNew YorkElsevier
MacNamara, J. 1986 A Border Dispute; the Place of Logic in PsychologyCambridge, MAMIT Press
Mareschal, D.Johnson, M. H. 2003 The “what” and “where” of object representations in infancyCognition 88 259Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.Moore, M. K. 1983 Newborn infants imitate adult facial gesturesChild Development 54 702Google Scholar
Morton, J.Johnson, M. H. 1991 CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognitionPsychological Review 98 164Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M.Winocur, G.Behrmann, M. 1997 What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognitionJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9 555Google Scholar
Needham, A. 1998 Infants’ use of featural information in the segregation of stationary objectsInfant Behavior and Development 21 47Google Scholar
Needham, A.Baillargeon, R. 1993 Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-old infantsCognition 47 121Google Scholar
Needham, A.Baillargeon, R. 1997 Object segregation in 8-month-old infantsCognition 62 121Google Scholar
Needham, A.Baillargeon, R. 1998 Infant Behavior and Development 21 1
Palmer, C. 1989 The discriminating nature of infants’ exploratory actionsDevelopmental Psychology 25 885Google Scholar
Pascalis, O.de Schonen, S.Morton, J.Deruelle, C.Fabre-Grenet, M. 1995 Mother’s face recognition by neonates: A replication and an extensionInfant Behavior and Development 18 79Google Scholar
Pelphrey, K. A.Morris, J. P.Michelich, C. R.Allison, T.McCarthy, G. 2005 Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: An fMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movementsCerebral Cortex 15 866Google Scholar
Pourtois, G.Schwartz, S.Seghier, M. L.Lazeyras, F.Vuilleumier, P. 2005 View-independent coding of face identity in frontal and temporal cortices is modulated by familiarity: An event-related fMRI studyNeuroimage 24 214Google Scholar
Ricard, M.Allard, L. 1993 The reaction of 9- to 10-month-old infants to an unfamiliar animalThe Journal of Genetic Psychology 154 5Google Scholar
Rothstein, P.Henson, R. N. A.Treves, A.Driver, J.Dolan, R. J. 2005 Morphing Marilyn into Maggie dissociates physical and identity face representations in the brainNature Neuroscience 8 107Google Scholar
Ruff, H. 1984 Infants’ manipulative exploration of objects: Effects of age and object characteristicsDevelopmental Psychology 20 9Google Scholar
Ryu, J.Borrman, K.Chaudhuri, A. 2008 Imagine Jane and identify John: Face identity after effects induced by imagined facesPublic Library of Science ONE 3 195Google Scholar
Saxe, R.Tzelnic, T.Carey, S. 2006 Five-month-old infants know humans are solid, like inanimate objectsCognition 101 B1Google Scholar
Simon, T. J.Hespos, S. J.Rochat, P. 1995 Do infants understand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992)Cognitive Development 10 253Google Scholar
Slaughter, V.Heron, M. 2004 Origins and early development of human body knowledgeMonographs of the Society of Research in Child Development 69 1Google Scholar
Slaughter, V.Heron, M.Sim, S. 2002 Development of preferences for the human body shape in infancyCognition 85 B71Google Scholar
Slaughter, V.Stone, V. E.Reed, C. 2004 Perception of faces and bodies: Similar or different?Current Directions in Psychological Science 13 216Google Scholar
Sommerville, J. A.Woodward, A. L.Needham, A. 2005 Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actionsCognition 96 B1Google Scholar
Spelke, E. 1990 Principles of object perceptionCognitive Science 14 29Google Scholar
Spelke, E.Breinlinger, K.Macomber, J.Jacobson, K. 1992 Origins of knowledgePsychological Review 99 605Google Scholar
Spelke, E.Phillips, A.Woodward, A. 1995 Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human actionSperman, D.Premack, D.Premack, A.Causal CognitionOxford, UKOxford University Press
Van de Walle, G.Carey, S.Prevor, M. 2000 Bases for object individuation in infancy: Evidence from manual searchJournal of Cognition and Development 1 249Google Scholar
Walton, G. E.Bower, N. J. A.Bower, T. G. R. 1992 Recognition of familiar faces by newbornsInfant Behavior and Development 15 265Google Scholar
Wilcox, T.Baillargeon, R. 1998 Object individuation in infancy: The use of featural information in reasoning about occlusion eventsCognitive Psychology 37 97Google Scholar
Wilcox, T.Chapa, C. 2002 Infants’ reasoning about opaque and transparent occluders in an individuation taskCognition 85 B1Google Scholar
Woodward, A. 1998 Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reachCognition 69 1Google Scholar
Wynn, K. 1992 Addition and subtraction by human infantsNature 358 749Google Scholar
Xu, F. 1999 Object individuation and object identity in infancy: The role of spatiotemporal information, object property information, and languageActa Psychologica 102 113Google Scholar
Xu, F. 2007 Sortal concepts, object individuation, and languageTrends in Cognitive Science 11 400Google Scholar
Xu, F.Carey, S. 1996 Infants’ metaphysics: The case of numerical identityCognitive Psychology 30 111Google Scholar
Xu, F.Carey, S.Welch, J. 1999 Infants’ ability to use object kind information for object individuationCognition 70 137Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×