Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:35:02.511Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Connecting Conspiracy Beliefs and Experiences of Social Exclusion

from Part III - Topics Related to the Exclusion–Extremism Link

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 May 2024

Michaela Pfundmair
Affiliation:
Federal University of Administrative Sciences, Germany
Andrew H. Hales
Affiliation:
University of Mississippi
Kipling D. Williams
Affiliation:
Purdue University, Indiana
Get access

Summary

Both ostracism experiences and conspiracy beliefs have been discussed as formative ingredients of radicalization trajectories and violent extremism. The present chapter provides a brief introduction to the psychology of conspiracy beliefs and the connections made to violent extremism. In its central part, it discusses the connection between the two in discussing (1) direct evidence for ostracism experiences increasing conspiracy belief, (2) indirect evidence via highlighting how the four fundamental needs postulated to be threatened by ostracism have also been connected to conspiracy beliefs, and (3) a discussion of the reverse causation of expressing conspiracy beliefs leading to being ostracized. In all sections, specific emphasis will be dedicated to the question of how reliable and strong the available evidence is, with experimental evidence weighing heavier than correlational evidence and repeatedly replicated and meta-analytically robust effect weighing heavier than single demonstrations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Exclusion and Extremism
A Psychological Perspective
, pp. 287 - 307
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahadzadeh, A. S., Ong, F. S., & Wu, S. L. (2023). Social media skepticism and belief in conspiracy theories about COVID-19: The moderating role of the dark triad. Current Psychology, 42(11), 88748886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alper, S. (2023). There are higher levels of conspiracy beliefs in more corrupt countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 53(3), 503517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alper, S., & Imhoff, R. (2023). Suspecting foul play when it is objectively there: The association of political orientation with general and partisan conspiracy beliefs as a function of corruption levels. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 14, 610620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, J., & Miller, C. (2010). The power of unreason: Conspiracy theories, extremism and counter-terrorism. Demos.Google Scholar
Bertlich, T., Bräscher, A., Germer, S., Witthöft, M., & Imhoff, R. (2024). Owners of a lonely heart? Investigating the longitudinal relationship between loneliness and conspiracy beliefs. [Manuscript under review].Google Scholar
Biddlestone, M., Green, R., Cichocka, A., Douglas, K., & Sutton, R. M. (2022, April 8). A systematic review and meta-analytic synthesis of the motives associated with conspiracy beliefs. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rxjqcCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bierwiaczonek, K., Gundersen, A. B., & Kunst, J. R. (2022). The role of conspiracy beliefs for COVID-19 health responses: A meta-analysis. Current Opinion in Psychology, 46, 101346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bierwiaczonek, K., Kunst, J. R., & Pich, O. (2020). Belief in COVID‐19 conspiracy theories reduces social distancing over time. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 12(4), 12701285.Google ScholarPubMed
Bilewicz, M., & Imhoff, R. (2022). Political conspiracy beliefs and their alignment on the left–right political spectrum. Social Research: An International Quarterly, 89, 679706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowes, S. M., Costello, T. H., & Arber, T. (2023). The conspiratorial mind: A meta-analytic review of motivational and personological correlates. Psychological Bulletin, 149(5–6), 259293. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5), 475482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., & Biddlestone, M. (2022). Why do narcissists find conspiracy theories so appealing? Current Opinion in Psychology, 47, 101386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., & Golec de Zavala, A. (2016a). Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, self-esteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(2), 157166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., Golec de Zavala, A., & Olechowski, M. (2016b). “They will not control us”: In-group positivity and belief in intergroup conspiracies. British Journal of Psychology, 107, 556576. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cislak, A., Marchlewska, M., Wojcik, A. D., et al. (2021). National narcissism and support for voluntary vaccination policy: The mediating role of vaccination conspiracy beliefs. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 24(5), 701719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R., Broadnax, S., & Blaine, B. E. (1999). Belief in US government conspiracies against blacks among black and white college students: Powerlessness or system blame? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(8), 941953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2023). What are conspiracy theories? A definitional approach to their correlates, consequences, and communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 74, 271298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016). Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1), 5777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, K. M., van Prooijen, J.-W., & Sutton, R. M. (2021). Is the label “conspiracy theory” a cause or a consequence of disbelief in alternative narratives? British Journal of Psychology, 113(3), 575590. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12548CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiedler, K., Schott, M., & Meiser, T. (2011). What mediation analysis can (not) do. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 12311236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenken, M., & Imhoff, R. (2021). A uniform conspiracy mindset or differentiated reactions to specific conspiracy beliefs? Evidence from latent profile analyses. International Review of Social Psychology, 34(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenken, M., & Imhoff, R. (2022). Malevolent intentions and secret coordination. Dissecting cognitive processes in conspiracy beliefs via diffusion modeling. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 103, 104383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenken, M., & Imhoff, R. (2023). Don’t trust anybody: Conspiracy mentality and the detection of facial trustworthiness cues. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 37, 256265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilman, R., Carter-Sowell, A., DeWall, C. N., Adams, R. E., & Carboni, I. (2013). Validation of the ostracism experience scale for adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 25, 319330. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00379CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gkinopoulos, T., & Uysal, M. (2021, October 7). Ostracism and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19: Personality and existential underlying mechanisms in a quote sampling study during the first wave of the pandemic. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/q4hknCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golec de Zavala, A., Bierwiaczonek, K., & Ciesielski, P. (2022). An interpretation of meta-analytical evidence for the link between collective narcissism and conspiracy theories. Current Opinion in Psychology, 47, 101360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golec de Zavala, A., & Federico, C. M. (2018), Collective narcissism and the growth of conspiracy thinking over the course of the 2016 United States presidential election: A longitudinal analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 10111018. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2496CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graeupner, D., & Coman, A. (2017). The dark side of meaning-making: How social exclusion leads to superstitious thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 218222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, R., Toribio-Flórez, D., Douglas, K. M., Brunkow, J. W., & Sutton, R. M. (2023). Making an impression: The effects of sharing conspiracy theories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 104, 104398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hales, A. H., & Williams, K. D. (2020). Extremism leads to ostracism. Social Psychology, 51(3), 149156. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hales, A. H., & Williams, K. D. (2021). Social ostracism: Theoretical foundations and basic principles. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Higgins, E. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (3rd ed., pp. 337349). Guilford.Google Scholar
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40(2), 218227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hettich, N., Beutel, M. E., Ernst, M., et al. (2022). Conspiracy endorsement and its associations with personality functioning, anxiety, loneliness, and sociodemographic characteristics during the COVID-19 pandemic in a representative sample of the German population. PLoS ONE, 17(1), e0263301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., & Fielding, K. S. (2018). Relationships among conspiratorial beliefs, conservatism and climate scepticism across nations. Nature Climate Change, 8(7), 614620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornsey, M. J., & Pearson, S. (2022). Cross-national differences in willingness to believe conspiracy theories. Current Opinion in Psychology, 47, 101391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26(6), 655672. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027504268574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husting, G., & Orr, M. (2007). Dangerous machinery: “Conspiracy theorist” as a transpersonal strategy of exclusion. Symbolic interaction, 30(2), 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R. (2015). Beyond (right-wing) authoritarianism: Conspiracy mentality as an incremental predictor of prejudice. In Bilewicz, M., Cichocka, A., & Soral, W. (Eds.), The psychology of conspiracy (pp. 122141). Routledge.Google Scholar
Imhoff, R. (2022). Conspiracy theories through a cross-cultural lens. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 5(3).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R. (2023). The psychology of pandemic conspiracy theories. In Butter, M., & Knight, P. (Eds.) Covid conspiracy theories in global perspective (pp. 1525). Taylor & Francis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., Bertlich, T., & Frenken, M. (2022a). Tearing apart the “evil” twins: A general conspiracy mentality is not the same as specific conspiracy beliefs. Current Opinion in Psychology, 46, 101349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalized political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28, 2543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., Dieterle, L., & Lamberty, P. (2021). Resolving the puzzle of conspiracy worldview and political activism: Belief in secret plots decreases normative but increases non-normative political engagement. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12, 7179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2017). Too special to be duped: Need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 724734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2018). How paranoid are conspiracy believers? Towards a more fine-grained understanding of the connect and disconnect between paranoia and belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 909926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2020). A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11, 11101118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imhoff, R., Lamberty, P., & Klein, O. (2018). Using power as a negative cue: How conspiracy mentality affects epistemic trust in sources of historical knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 13641379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R., Zimmer, F., Klein, O., et al. (2022b). Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nature Human Behavior, 6, 392403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, J., & Sassenberg, K. (2009). Is being excluded boring? Paper presentation. The 10th Annual Society for Personality and Social Psychology Conference, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
Jaiswal, J., Singer, S. N., Siegel, K., & Lekas, H. M. (2019). HIV-related “conspiracy beliefs”: Lived experiences of racism and socio-economic exclusion among people living with HIV in New York City. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 21(4), 373386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolley, D., & Lantian, A. (2022). Bullying and conspiracy theories: Experiences of workplace bullying and the tendency to engage in conspiracy theorizing. Social Psychology, 53, 198208. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolley, D., Mari., S., & Douglas, K. M. (2020). Consequences of conspiracy theories. In Butter, M., & Knight, P. (Eds.), Routledge handbook of conspiracy theories (pp. 231241). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolley, D., & Paterson, J. L. (2020). Pylons ablaze: Examining the role of 5G COVID‐19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(3), 628640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolley, D., Paterson, J., & Thomas, R. (2023). Refusing to pay taxes: Loneliness, conspiracy theorising and non-normative political action. Social Psychology, 54(5), 308319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, C. S. (2021). The targets of all treachery: Delusional ideation, paranoia, and the need for uniqueness as mediators between two forms of narcissism and conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Research in Personality, 93, 104128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kofta, M., Soral, W., & Bilewicz, M. (2020). What breeds conspiracy antisemitism? The role of political uncontrollability and uncertainty in the belief in Jewish conspiracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(5), 900918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). “I know things they don’t know!”: The role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Social Psychology, 48(3), 160171. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., et al. (2018). Stigmatized beliefs: Conspiracy theories, anticipated negative evaluation of the self, and fear of social exclusion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(7), 939954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (1995). The need to belong. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497529.Google Scholar
Leman, P. J., & Cinnirella, M. (2007). A major event has a major cause: Evidence for the role of heuristics in reasoning about conspiracy theories. Social Psychological Review, 9(2), 1828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemmer, G., & Gollwitzer, M. (2017). The “true” indirect effect won’t (always) stand up: When and why reverse mediation testing fails. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 144149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liekefett, L., Christ, O., & Becker, J. C. (2023). Can conspiracy beliefs be beneficial? Longitudinal linkages between conspiracy beliefs, anxiety, uncertainty aversion, and existential threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(2), 167179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchlewska, M., Cichocka, A., Łozowski, F., Górska, P., & Winiewski, M. (2019). In search of an imaginary enemy: Catholic collective narcissism and the endorsement of gender conspiracy beliefs. The Journal of Social Psychology, 159(6), 766779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meuer, M., & Imhoff, R. (2021). Believing in hidden plots is associated with decreased behavioral trust: Conspiracy belief as greater sensitivity to social threat or insensitivity towards its absence? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 93, 104081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molet, M., Macquet, B., Lefebvre, O., & Williams, K. D. (2013). A focused attention intervention for coping with ostracism. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 12621270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Napolitano, M. G., & Reuter, K. (2023). What is a conspiracy theory? Erkenntnis, 88(5), 20352062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neu, C., Küpper, B., Luhmann, M., Deutsch, M., & Fröhlich, P. (2023). Extrem einsam? Die demokratische Relevanz von Einsamkeitserfahrungen unter Jugendlichen in Deutschland [Extremely lonely? The democratic relevance of loneliness experiences among adolescents in Germany]. Das Progressive Zentrum. www.progressives-zentrum.org/wp-content/uploads/2023/02/Kollekt_Studie_Extrem_Einsam_Das-Progressive-Zentrum.pdfGoogle Scholar
Oeberst, A., & Imhoff, R. (2023). Towards parsimony in bias research: Proposing a common framework of belief-consistent information processing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 18(6), 14641487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfundmair, M., Eyssel, F., Graupmann, V., Frey, D., & Aydin, N. (2015). Wanna play? The role of self-construal when using gadgets to cope with ostracism. Social Influence, 10(4), 221235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poon, K. T., Chen, Z., & Wong, W. Y. (2020). Beliefs in conspiracy theories following ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(8), 12341246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popper, K. (1966). The open society and its enemies (5th ed.). Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Pummerer, L., Ditrich, L., Winter, K., & Sassenberg, K. (2023). Think about it! Deliberation reduces the negative relation between conspiracy belief and adherence to prosocial norms. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 14(8), 952963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radnitz, S., & Underwood, P. (2017). Is belief in conspiracy theories pathological? A survey experiment on the cognitive roots of extreme suspicion. British Journal of Political Science, 47(1), 113129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rottweiler, B., & Gill, P. (2022). Conspiracy beliefs and violent extremist intentions: The contingent effects of self-efficacy, self-control and law-related morality. Terrorism and Political Violence, 34(7), 14851504. https://doi.org/10.1080/09546553.2020.1803288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousis, G. J., Richard, F. D., & Wang, D. D. (2022). The truth is out there: The prevalence of conspiracy theory use by radical violent extremist organizations. Terrorism and Political Violence, 34(8), 17391757. https://doi.org/10.1080/09546553.2020.1835654CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudert, S. C., Keller, M. D., Hales, A. H., Walker, M., & Greifeneder, R. (2020). Who gets ostracised? A personality perspective on risk and protective factors of ostracism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118, 12471268. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(3), 472480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakki, I., & Castrén, L. (2022). Dehumanization through humour and conspiracies in online hate towards Chinese people during the COVID-19 pandemic. British Journal of Social Psychology, 61, 14181438. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12543CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnepf, J., Lux, A., Jin, Z., & Formanowicz, M. (2021). Left out: Feelings of social exclusion incite individuals with high conspiracy mentality to reject complex scientific messages. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 40(5–6), 627652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siem, B., Kretzmeyer, B., & Stürmer, S. (2021). The role of self-evaluation in predicting attitudes toward supporters of COVID-19-related conspiracy theories: A direct and a conceptual replication of Cichocka et al. (2016). Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 15, 18344909211052587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stasielowicz, L. (2022). Who believes in conspiracy theories? A meta-analysis on personality correlates. Journal of Research in Personality, 98, 104229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternisko, A., Cichocka, A., Cislak, A., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2023). National narcissism predicts the belief in and the dissemination of conspiracy theories during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence from 56 countries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(1), 4865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stojanov, A., & Halberstadt, J. (2020). Does lack of control lead to conspiracy beliefs? A meta‐analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(5), 955968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, D., Landau, M. J., & Rothschild, Z. K. (2010). An existential function of enemyship: Evidence that people attribute influence to personal and political enemies to compensate for threats to control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 434449. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017457CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, R. M., & Douglas, K. M. (2022). Rabbit Hole Syndrome: Inadvertent, accelerating, and entrenched commitment to conspiracy beliefs. Current Opinion in Psychology, 48, 101462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., et al. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real‐world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 443463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C., Azevedo, F., & Jost, J. T. (2021). The paranoid style in American politics revisited: An ideological asymmetry in conspiratorial thinking. Political Psychology, 42(1), 2351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Prooijen, J. W. (2016). Sometimes inclusion breeds suspicion: Self‐uncertainty and belongingness predict belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(3), 267279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. W. (2017). Why education predicts decreased belief in conspiracy theories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31(1), 5058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Prooijen, J. W., & Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 753761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. W., Douglas, K. M., & De Inocencio, C. (2018). Connecting the dots: Illusory pattern perception predicts belief in conspiracies and the supernatural. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(3), 320335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Prooijen, J. W., & Imhoff, R. (2022). The psychological study of conspiracy theories: Strengths and limitations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 48, 101465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. W., Ligthart, J., Rosema, S., & Xu, Y. (2022). The entertainment value of conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 113(1), 2548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Prooijen, J. W., Staman, J., & Krouwel, A. P. (2018). Increased conspiracy beliefs among ethnic and Muslim minorities. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 32(5), 661667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. W., & Van Dijk, E. (2014). When consequence size predicts belief in conspiracy theories: The moderating role of perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 6373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. W., & Van Lange, P. A. (2014). The social dimension of belief in conspiracy theories. In van Prooijen, J. W., & van Lange, P. A. (Eds.) Power, politics, and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders (pp. 237253). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldeck, D., Tyndall, I., Riva, P., & Chmiel, N. (2017). How do we cope with ostracism? Psychological flexibility moderates the relationship between everyday ostracism experiences and psychological distress. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6(4), 425432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern-perception. Science, 322, 115117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, K. (2009). Ostracism: A temporal need threat model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 275314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, M. N., Marques, M. D., Hill, S. R., Kerr, J. R., & Ling, M. (2022). Why are beliefs in different conspiracy theories positively correlated across individuals? Testing monological network versus unidimensional factor model explanations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 61(3), 10111031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, K., Hornsey, M. J., Pummerer, L., & Sassenberg, K. (2022). Anticipating and defusing the role of conspiracy beliefs in shaping opposition to wind farms. Nature Energy, 7(12), 12001207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, W., Levordashka, A., Ruff, J. R., et al. (2015). Ostracism online: A social media ostracism paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 361373. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0475-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, M. J. (2016). Some dare call it conspiracy: Labeling something a conspiracy theory does not reduce belief in it. Political Psychology, 37(5), 695705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×