Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:40:55.476Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Is Language Aptitude Immune to Experience?

Divergent Evidence from Bilingualism vs. Blindness

from Part II - Aptitude Testing of Diverse Groups

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 May 2023

Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Peter Skehan
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, University of London
Richard L. Sparks
Affiliation:
Mount St Joseph University
Get access

Summary

In this contribution, we explore the question of whether there is reason to maintain the traditional view of language aptitude as a relatively fixed trait that is resistant to experience, or if it should instead be seen as a rather flexible and acquirable skill. Both bilingualism and visual loss have been reported to have enhancing effects on language-related as well as non-linguistic cognition, but few studies have focused on their effects on language aptitude specifically, especially in the case of blindness. In a study of 80 blind and sighted L1 and L2 speakers of Swedish, we compare the relative experiential effects on the phonological aptitude of (1) having learned an L2 and having been a long-term functional and fluent bilingual in adulthood with (2) having lived with total visual deprivation for a significant period of life. The chapter closes with a discussion on what it would mean for current views on the role of age of L2 acquisition and critical period(s) if the above-average language aptitude hitherto robustly associated with adult near-native L2 learning should turn out to be nothing but an effect of L2 learning itself.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahamsson, N. (2012). Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(2), 187214.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. (2008). The robustness of aptitude effects in near-native second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(4), 481509.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59(2), 249306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amedi, A., Floel, A., Knecht, S., Zohary, E., & Cohen, L. G. (2004). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the occipital pole interferes with verbal processing in blind subjects. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 12661270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amedi, A., Raz, N., Pianka, P., Malach, R., & Zohary, E. (2003). Early “visual” cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 758766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ameringer, V., Green, L., Leisser, D., & Turker, S. (2018). Introduction: Towards an interdisciplinary understanding of language aptitude. In Reiterer, S. M. (ed.), Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience. Springer, pp. 115.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: Where and how? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(6), 443452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedny, M. (2017). Evidence from blindness for a cognitively pluripotent cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 637648.Google Scholar
Bedny, M., Pascual-Leone, A., Dodell-Feder, D., Fedorenko, E., & Saxe, R. (2011). Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(11), 44294434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12(1), 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2016). The signal and the noise: Finding the pattern in human behavior. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6(5), 517534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143(3), 233262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bokander, L., & Bylund, E. (2020). Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning, 70(1), 1147.Google Scholar
Bylund, E., Hyltenstam, K., & Abrahamsson, N. (2021). Age of acquisition – not bilingualism – is the primary determinant of less than nativelike L2 ultimate attainment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24(1), 1830.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1958). A factor analysis of two foreign language aptitude batteries. Journal of General Psychology, 59(1), 39.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1962). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In Glaser, R. (ed.), Training Research and Education. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 87136.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1973). Implications of aptitude test research and psycholinguistic theory for foreign language teaching. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 2, 514.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 83118.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. M. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on third language acquisition: Focus on multilingualism. Language Teaching, 46(1), 7186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, J. (2017). How stable is second language aptitude? Effects of second language learning and language analysis training on second language aptitude test scores. Doctoral dissertation, Griffith University, USA.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G., Lynch, J. M., Mendes, N., & Zhai, C. (2019). On bilingual aptitude for learning new languages: The roles of linguistic and nonlinguistic individual differences. Language Learning, 69(2), 478514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R., & Koeth, J. (2011). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning. In Hinkel, E. (ed.), Handbook of Research in Second Language Teaching and Learning. New York, NY: Routledge, pp. 395406.Google Scholar
Dick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., et al. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the nationally representative ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 692701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., & Ackermann, H. (2013). Ultra-fast speech comprehension in blind subjects engages primary visual cortex, fusiform gyrus, and pulvinar – a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. BMC Neuroscience 14 , 74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J., & Homer, B. D. (2019). Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 11221147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C. (2019). Cognitive language aptitude. Language Learning, 69(S1), 101126.Google Scholar
Eisenstein, M. (1980). Childhood bilingualism and adult language learning aptitude. International Review of Applied Psychology 29(1–2), 159172.Google Scholar
Forsberg-Lundell, F., & Sandgren, M. (2013). High-level proficiency in late L2 acquisition. Relationships between collocational production, language aptitude and personality. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins, pp. 231258.Google Scholar
Frasnelli, J., Collignon, O., Voss, P., & Lepore, F. (2011). Crossmodal plasticity in sensory loss. Progress in Brain Research, 191, 233249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganschow, L., & Sparks, R. (1995). Effects of direct instruction in Spanish phonology on the native-language skills and foreign-language aptitude of at-risk foreign-language learners. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(2), 107120.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social Psychology and Second Language Learning: The Role of Attitudes and Motivation. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2013). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA language aptitude test. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 105130.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. (2013). Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29(3), 311343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Sternberg, R. J., & Ehrman, M. E. (2000). A theory-based approach to the measurement of foreign language learning ability: The Canal-F theory and test. The Modern Language Journal, 84(3), 390405.Google Scholar
Harley, B., & Hart, D. (1997). Language aptitude and second language proficiency in classroom learners of different starting ages. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(3), 379400.Google Scholar
Harley, B., & Hart, D. (2002). Age, aptitude, and second language learning on a bilingual exchange. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 301–30.Google Scholar
Herdina, P., & Jessner, U. (2002). A Dynamic Model of Multilingualism: Perspectives of Change in Psycholinguistics. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T. (2017). Direct and indirect effects of multilingualism on novel language learning: An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 892916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), F9F15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hugdahl, K., Ek, M., Takio, F., et al. (2004). Blind individuals show enhanced perceptual and attentional sensitivity for identification of speech sounds. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(1), 2832.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K. (2021). Language aptitude and language awareness: Polyglot perspectives. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 41, 5575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioup, G., Boustagui, E., El Tigi, M., & Moselle, M. (1994). Reexamining the critical period hypothesis: A case study in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16(1), 7398.Google Scholar
Jessner, U. (2006). Linguistic Awareness in Multilinguals: English as a Third Language. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317324.Google Scholar
Kormos, J. (2013). New conceptualizations of language aptitude in second language attainment. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 131152.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1977). The monitor model of adult second language performance. In Burt, M., Dulay, H., & Finocchiaro, M. (eds.), Viewpoints on English as a Second Language. New York: Regents, pp. 152161.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1981). Aptitude and attitude in relation to second language acquisition and learning. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 155175.Google Scholar
Lamendella, J. T. (1977). General principles of neurofunctional organization and their manifestation in primary and nonprimary language acquisition. Language Learning, 27(1), 155196.Google Scholar
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., et al. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leivada, E., Westergaard, M., Andoni Duñabeitia, J., & Rothman, J. (2021). On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition: (How) should we proceed? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(1), 197210.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2016). The construct validity of language aptitude. A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(4), 801842.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2019). Six decades of language aptitude research. A comprehensive and critical review. In Wen, Z. (E.), Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. L. (eds.), Language Aptitude. Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York: Routledge, pp. 7896.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63(3), 530566.Google Scholar
Loiotile, R., Lane, C., Omaki, A., & Bedny, M. (2020). Enhanced performance on a sentence comprehension task in congenitally blind adults. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(8), 10101023.Google Scholar
Lowe, C. J., Cho, I., Goldsmith, S. F., & Morton, J. B. (2021). The bilingual advantage in children’s executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Science, 32(7), 11151146. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797621993108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ma, D., Yao, T., & Zhang, H. (2018). The effect of third language learning on language aptitude among English-major students in China. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 39(7), 590601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meara, P. (2005a). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005b). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests: The Manual. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Meara, P., Milton, J., & Lorenzo-Dus, N. (2003). Swansea Language Aptitude Tests (LAT) v2.0. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Miller, L. (1992). Diderot reconsidered: Visual impairment and auditory compensation. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 86(5), 206210.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. (1998). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In Healy, A. & Bourne, L. (eds.), Foreign Language Learning: Psycholinguistic Studies on Training and Retention. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 339364.Google Scholar
Novén, M., Olsson, H., Helms, G., et al. (2021). Cortical and white matter correlates of language-learning aptitudes. Human Brain Mapping, 42(15), 50375050 First view: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25598Google Scholar
Ottó, I. (2002). Magyar Egységes Nyelvérzékmérő-Teszt [Hungarian Language Aptitude Test]. Kaposvár: Mottó-Logic Bt.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. (2019). The bilingual advantage debate. Quantity and quality of the evidence. In Schwieter, J. W. (ed.), The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism. London: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 701735.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Mason, L., Zimiga, B., Ayala-Silva, Y., & Frost, M. (2020). The alchemy of confirmation bias transmutes expectations into bilingual advantages: A tale of two new meta-analyses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(8), 12901299.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. R., & Al-Haik, A. R. (1976). The development of the Defense Language Aptitude Battery (DLAB). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 36(2), 369380.Google Scholar
Planchon, A., & Ellis, E. (2014). A diplomatic advantage? The effects of bilingualism and formal language training on language aptitude amongst Australian diplomatic officers. Language Awareness, 23(3), 203219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ranta, L. (2002). The role of learners’ language analytic ability in the communicative classroom. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 159180.Google Scholar
Raz, N., Amedi, A., & Zohary, E. (2005). V1 activation in congenitally blind humans is associated with episodic retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 15(9), 14591468.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (1997). Individual differences and the fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit adult second language learning. Language Learning, 47(1): 4599.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2002). Effects of individual differences in intelligence, aptitude and working memory on incidental second language learning: A replication and extension of Reber, Walkenfield and Hernstadt (1991). In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 212266.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 4673.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitudes, abilities, contexts, and practice. In DeKeyser, R. (ed.), Practice in a Second Language: Perspectives from Applied Linguistics and Cognitive Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 256286.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2012). Individual differences, aptitude complexes, SLA processes, and aptitude test development. In Pawlak, M. (ed.), New Perspectives on Individual Differences in Language Learning and Teaching. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 5775.Google Scholar
Röder, B., & Neville, H. J. (2003). Developmental functional plasticity. In Grafman, J. & Robertson, I. H. (eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology. Oxford: Elsevier Science, pp. 231270.Google Scholar
Röder, B., Rösler, F., & Neville, H. J. (2000). Event-related potentials during auditory language processing in congenitally blind and sighted people. Neuropsychologia, 38(11), 14821502.Google Scholar
Röder, B., Rösler, F., & Neville, H. J. (2001). Auditory memory in congenitally blind adults: A behavioral-electrophysiological investigation. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(2), 289303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roehr-Brackin, K., & Tellier, A. (2019). The role of language-analytic ability in children’s instructed second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(5), 11111131.Google Scholar
Rogers, V., Meara, P., Barnett-Legh, T., Curry, C., & Davie, E. (2017). Examining the LLAMA aptitude tests. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 1, 4960.Google Scholar
Rokem, A., & Ahissar, M. (2009). Interactions of cognitive and auditory abilities in congenitally blind individuals. Neuropsychologia, 47(3), 843848.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. (1983). Patterns of interaction between three blind infants and their mothers. In Mills, A. E. (ed.), Language Acquisition in the Blind Child: Normal and Deficient. San Diego: College Hill Press, pp. 114132.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. (1984). Preverbal communication of blind infants and their mothers. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 78(7), 297302.Google Scholar
Sáfár, A., & Kormos, J. (2008). Revisiting problems with foreign language aptitude. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 46(2), 113136.Google Scholar
Sawyer, M. (1992). Language aptitude and language experience: Are they related? The Language Programs of the International University of Japan Working Papers, 3, 2745.Google Scholar
Sawyer, M., & Ranta, L. (2001). Aptitude, individual differences, and instructional design. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 319353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language teaching, 10(3), 209231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 45(1), 2150.Google Scholar
Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(2), 155172.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (2014). Apt to change: The problematic of language awareness and language aptitude in age-related research. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 4(3), 557571.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (2017). Language aptitude: Desirable trait or acquirable attribute? Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 7(1), 89103.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual Differences in Second Language Learning. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2002). Theorising and updating aptitude. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 6995.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2012). Language aptitude. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (eds.), Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. London: Routledge, pp. 381395.Google Scholar
Smeds, H. (2015). Blindness and Second Language Acquisition. Studies of Cognitive Advantages in Blind L1 and L2 Speakers. Doctoral dissertation in Bilingualism. Stockholm: Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Snow, R. E. (1994). Abilities in academic tasks. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (eds.), Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 337.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (1993). The effects of multisensory structured language instruction on native language and foreign-language aptitude skills of at-risk high-school foreign-language learners: A replication and follow-up-study. Annals of Dyslexia, 43(1), 194216.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Artzer, M., Patton, J., et al. (1998). Benefits of multisensory structured language instruction for at-risk foreign language learners: A comparison study of high school Spanish students. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 239270.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M., & Patton, J. (1997). Foreign language proficiency of at-risk and not-at-risk learners over 2 years of foreign language instruction: A follow-up study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30(1), 9298.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Ganschow, L., Fluharty, K., & Little, S. (1995). An exploratory study on the effects of Latin on the native language skills and foreign language aptitude of students with and without learning disabilities. The Classical Journal, 91(2), 165184.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Humbach, N., Patton, J., & Ganschow, L. (2011). Subcomponents of second‐language aptitude and second‐language proficiency. The Modern Language Journal, 95(2), 253273.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Pohlman, J., Skinner, S., & Artzer, M. (1992). The effects of multisensory structured language instruction on native language and foreign language aptitude skills of at-risk high school foreign language learners. Annals of Dyslexia, 42(1), 2553.Google Scholar
Speciale, G., Ellis, N. C., & Bywater, T. (2004). Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(2), 293321.Google Scholar
Spolsky, B. (1989). Conditions for Second Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, A. A., & Weaver, K. (2005). Auditory perceptual consolidation in early-onset blindness. Neuropsychologia, 43(13), 19011910.Google Scholar
Stevens, A. A., & Weaver, K. E. (2009). Functional characteristics of auditory cortex in the blind. Behavioural Brain Research, 196(1), 134138.Google Scholar
Thompson, A. S. (2013). The interface of language aptitude and multilingualism: Reconsidering the bilingual/multilingual dichotomy. The Modern Language Journal, 97(3), 685701.Google Scholar
Tomasello, R., Wennekers, T., Garagnani, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2019). Visual cortex recruitment during language processing in blind individuals is explained by Hebbian learning. Scientific Reports, 9, 3579.Google Scholar
Turker, S. (2019). Exploring the Neuroanatomical and Behavioural Correlates of Foreign Language Aptitude. Doctoral dissertation, Karl-Franzens-Universität Graz. UniPub.Google Scholar
Turker, S., Reiterer, S. M., Schneider, P., & Seither-Preisler, A. (2018). The neuroanatomical correlates of foreign language aptitude. In: Reiterer, S. (ed.), Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 119148). Springer, Cham, pp. 119–148.Google Scholar
Turker, S., Reiterer, S. M., Schneider, P., & Seither-Preisler, A. (2019). Auditory cortex morphology predicts language learning potential in children and teenagers. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 824. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00824Google Scholar
Turker, S., Reiterer, S. M., Seither-Preisler, A., & Schneider, P. (2017). “When music speaks”: Auditory Auditory cortex morphology as a neuroanatomical marker of language aptitude and musicality. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2096.Google Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M. (2016). No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2), 246258.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., Biedroń, A., & Skehan, P. (2017). Foreign language aptitude theory: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Language Teaching, 50(1), 131.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Skehan, P. (2011). A new perspective on foreign language aptitude research: Building and supporting a case for “working memory as language aptitude”. Ilha do Desterro: A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies, 60, 1544.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Skehan, P. (2021). Stages of acquisition and the P/E model of working memory: Complementary or contrasting approaches to foreign language aptitude? Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 41, 624.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×