Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T03:23:11.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Communication about Authority and Purpose

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2023

Lisa Dellmuth
Affiliation:
Stockholms Universitet
Jonas Tallberg
Affiliation:
Stockholms Universitet

Summary

This chapter examines how information on the authority and purpose of international organizations influences citizen legitimacy beliefs toward global governance. Advancing on previous research that primarily has studied effects of procedures and performances on citizens legitimacy beliefs, this chapter uses a conjoint experimental design to assess how different institutional qualities matter when simultaneously communicated to citizens. The chapter explores this issue across hypothetical international organizations in two countries (Germany and the US). It finds that citizens form legitimacy beliefs in line with information about authority and purpose in international organizations. However, this relationship depends on citizens’ political priors. Information about an international organization’s authority has a weaker negative effect on legitimacy beliefs among internationalist citizens. Moreover, the effect of information about an international organization’s social purpose depends on citizens’ political values. These conditioning effects are only found in the more polarized context of the US and not in Germany.

Type
Chapter
Information
Legitimacy Politics
Elite Communication and Public Opinion in Global Governance
, pp. 177 - 206
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

When global and domestic elites communicate about IOs, they do not only invoke IOs’ procedures and performances in support of their positions but also the authority and purposes of these organizations. The authority of IOs is frequently cited when critics accuse IOs of undermining state sovereignty, or when supporters praise IOs as arenas for transnational problem-solving. Consider the slogan of the Leave campaign in the run-up to the British referendum on EU membership – “Take back control!” – which accused the EU of being too powerful and urges the UK to resurrect its national sovereignty. Conversely, others, like Guy Verhofstadt, Belgian Member of the European Parliament, have called for more authority for the EU in the fight against the coronavirus: “People want the EU to act decisively, but few want the EU to have the powers to make this possible. Individual governments think they can do it better until it is too late. We are now trying to fight a pandemic with our hands tied!” (Express, April 8, 2020).

Similarly, the social purpose of an IO is an integral part of the message when elites express concern or support for the policy goals of an organization, as shown in Chapter 2. Consider how Nikki Haley, then US Ambassador to the UN, accused the UN Human Rights Council of betraying its purpose, when justifying the US decision to withdraw from the body: “I want to make it crystal clear that this step is not a retreat from human rights commitments. On the contrary, we take this step because our commitment does not allow us to remain a part of a hypocritical and self-serving organization that makes a mockery of human rights” (NPR, June 19, 2018). In contrast, the UN itself invokes human rights as one of its principal aims and purposes when presenting itself on its homepage: “In 1948, the Universal Declaration of Human Rights brought human rights into the realm of international law. Since then, the Organization has diligently protected human rights through legal instruments and on-the-ground activities” (UN 2020).

In one way, it is the authority and purpose of an IO that make it an inherently political institution – not only in terms of machinery for political decision-making but also in terms of the political aims and means of the organization. It is by setting goals for an IO and by empowering this IO that states constitute IOs as political institutions. Yet, so far, we know little about the effects on popular legitimacy of elites invoking the authority and purposes of IOs in their communication about these organizations. While earlier research presents expectations that authority (Zürn et al. Reference Zürn, Binder and Ecker-Ehrhardt2012; Zürn Reference Zürn2018) and purpose (Barnett Reference Barnett1997; Lenz and Viola Reference Lenz and Viola2017) may matter for the legitimacy of IOs, it has not subjected these claims to systematic analysis. Does it matter for people’s evaluations of IOs whether these organizations possess more or less authority, and if so how? Likewise, does it matter for people’s opinions toward IOs whether these organizations work to fight poverty, ensure peace, combat climate change, or promote free trade, irrespective of other institutional features?

In this chapter, we offer a second analysis of how the content of elite communication impacts citizen legitimacy beliefs. We move beyond Chapter 6 in two important respects. First, we extend the analysis from an exclusive focus on procedure and performance to also consider the impact of authority and purpose on citizens’ legitimacy beliefs toward IOs. We thereby offer the first systematic analysis of organizational authority and purpose, as well as the most comprehensive assessment of how information on multiple qualities of IOs affects popular legitimacy. Second, we take up the challenge identified in Chapter 6 to consider the impact of institutional qualities in combination. For this purpose, we shift from a vignette experimental design to a conjoint experimental design, specifically developed to assess how a particular dimension of an object matters relative to other dimensions when their impact is assessed simultaneously (Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Yamamoto and Hopkins2014).

We evaluate the impact of communication about an IO’s authority, purpose, procedures, and performance on citizens’ legitimacy beliefs through a conjoint experiment in Germany and the US. While both are advanced industrialized democracies with federal political systems, these two countries differ in two factors that we expect could moderate the impact of authority and purpose, respectively: internationalist attitudes and political ideology. The conjoint experiment confronts respondents with two hypothetical IOs that vary in terms of the four examined institutional qualities and then asks the respondents to choose between the two, as well as evaluate the two choices. This design allows us to estimate the causal effect of information about each quality on respondents’ legitimacy beliefs, while simultaneously taking into consideration the impact of the other institutional features.

The main findings are threefold. First, communication about both the authority and the purpose of IOs matters for people’s legitimacy beliefs. When IOs are presented as having extensive authority over member states, this results in less confidence in these organizations. Similarly, the social purposes of IOs have an independent impact on legitimacy beliefs. For instance, promoting free trade has a negative effect on the perceived legitimacy of an IO, compared to ensuring peace and security. Second, the strength of these effects depends on citizens’ attitudes toward international cooperation and their political beliefs in the US but not in Germany, suggesting that IOs’ substantive goals can be a boost or a drag on their legitimacy, depending on people’s ideological priors and the country. Third, procedure and performance remain influential as sources of legitimacy when the effects of all four institutional features are assessed simultaneously in a conjoint design. In fact, communication about an IO’s procedures and performance has larger effects than information about its authority and purpose.

The chapter proceeds in four steps. We begin by developing the theoretical argument for why an IO’s authority and purpose may matter for people’s legitimacy beliefs and why these effects are likely to depend on people’s attitudes toward international cooperation and their political ideology. The chapter then presents the survey experimental design, laying out the merits of conjoint experiments, the execution of the survey, and the design of the experimental component. The third section presents the empirical results, beginning with the general effect of information on the four institutional qualities of IOs in Germany and the US, before considering how internationalist attitudes and political ideology condition these effects and reporting a number of validity and robustness checks. The fourth part of the chapter engages in a broader discussion of the findings, where we consider different interpretations and relate these results to earlier research. The chapter concludes by discussing the broader implications of our findings.

Hypotheses

Why would information about the authority and purposes of IOs affect citizens’ legitimacy beliefs? Like in Chapter 6, we start from the assumption that people are sensitive to information about an IO’s institutional qualities. However, different from previous research, we extend the range of qualities theorized to the authority and purpose of an IO, which we expect could have independent effects on citizens’ legitimacy beliefs.

Authority

We conceptualize authority and legitimacy as distinct but related entities. Whereas authority refers to an organization having the right to make decisions within a particular area, legitimacy refers to the perception that these rights are appropriately exercised (Tallberg and Zürn Reference Tallberg and Zürn2019, 586). This analytical separation of legitimacy from authority is well anchored in parts of social theory.Footnote 1 Weber (Reference Weber1922/1978, 213), for instance, speaks of how every system of authority “attempts to establish and to cultivate a belief in its legitimacy.” At the same time, authority and legitimacy are related, in so far as legitimacy only becomes an issue once an institution possesses authority. In the absence of authority, the question of legitimacy becomes uninteresting.

Empirically, the authority of IOs is captured by three components (Hooghe et al. Reference Hooghe, Marks, Lenz, Bezuijen, Ceka and Derderyan2017; Zürn et al. Reference Zürn, Tokhi and Binder2021). First, IOs enjoy greater authority when they have been conferred greater policy-making competences in issue domains that previously were regulated at the domestic level or not at all (Zürn et al. Reference Zürn, Binder and Ecker-Ehrhardt2012). Second, IOs enjoy greater authority when the member states move away from intergovernmental cooperation by delegating increasing power to autonomous supranational bodies (Tallberg Reference Tallberg2002; Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Lake, Nielson and Tierney2006). Third, IOs have greater authority when the member states pool power within intergovernmental bodies by shifting toward forms of majority voting that remove each state’s veto over decisions (Keohane and Hoffmann Reference Keohane and Hoffmann1991; Moravcsik Reference Moravcsik1998).

By these criteria, the authority of IOs has expanded considerably over recent decades (Hooghe et al. Reference Hooghe, Marks, Lenz, Bezuijen, Ceka and Derderyan2017; Zürn et al. Reference Zürn, Tokhi and Binder2021). States have empowered IOs with authority in more policy domains, delegated more authority to supranational bodies, and pooled more authority in collective decision-making. The growth in IO authority is particularly notable after the end of the Cold War. That said, IOs continue to vary in the authority they possess, ranging from greatly empowered organizations such as the EU, which scores high on all three components, to less empowered organizations such as NAFTA (and its successor, the United States–Mexico–Canada Agreement [USMCA]), which scores low on all components.

We expect information about the authority of IOs to matter for citizens’ legitimacy beliefs. Specifically, we anticipate that IOs with greater authority will have a harder time securing legitimacy from citizens, all else equal (Zürn Reference Zürn2018; Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Bernauer and Kachi2019; Tallberg and Zürn Reference Tallberg and Zürn2019). When IOs enjoy extensive authority, they also have to meet demanding procedural and performance standards, or they will suffer from legitimacy deficits. When IOs enjoy less authority, the procedural and performance requirements they have to meet to be deemed legitimate are less demanding.

The EU is often said to offer an illustration of this logic (Banchoff and Smith Reference Banchoff and Smith1999; Hooghe and Marks Reference Hooghe and Marks2009; de Wilde and Zürn Reference De Wilde and Zürn2012). The greater the authority of the EU, the higher the demands on the organization to take decisions democratically and to solve problems effectively. As the EU often has fallen short of these expectations, despite more democratic procedures and effective performance than most IOs, legitimacy problems have arisen, reflected in low turnouts in European elections, rejections of new EU treaties in national referenda, and a decision on the part of the UK to leave the organization.

There is to date no systematic empirical evidence for a negative authority-legitimacy linkage, as the only empirical study so far on the relationship between authority and legitimacy finds no effect in either negative or positive direction. As Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Bernauer and Kachi2019, 663) conclude, based on a survey experiment in the context of global environmental governance: “[E]ven important shifts of authority from the national to the global level, such as majority decision making at the international level and automatic implementation of international decisions domestically, do not significantly affect citizens’ legitimacy perceptions on average.”

However, given that greater authority should set the bar higher for IOs to fare well in peoples’ perceptions, we expect a negative relationship between IO authority and legitimacy beliefs. In addition, we expect this negative relationship to be moderated by the degree to which citizens hold internationalist attitudes (Schlipphak et al. Reference Schlipphak, Schäfer and Treib2021). When citizens are more positive toward international cooperation in general terms, we expect the negative effect of authority on legitimacy to be weaker. Conversely, when citizens are more negative toward international cooperation in general terms, we expect this attitude to strengthen the negative effect of IO authority on legitimacy beliefs.

We advance two hypotheses on the basis of this argument. First, we formulate a general expectation about the effect of IO authority on people’s legitimacy beliefs. Second, we formulate a conditional expectation about heterogeneity in effects depending on the degree to which citizens hold internationalist attitudes.

H1a: Communication about an IO’s level of authority affects its perceived legitimacy.

H1b: The effect of communication about an IO’s level of authority on legitimacy beliefs (H1a) is conditioned by people’s attitudes toward international cooperation.

Purpose

The notion that an organization’s social purpose would affect perceptions of its legitimacy is not novel, even if the logic has never been fully theorized or tested. The earliest considerations of purpose hark back to pioneers in the general study of political legitimacy. Easton (Reference Easton1975, 452), for instance, argued that political institutions may obtain legitimacy on the grounds of people’s ideological beliefs or moral convictions, next to their beliefs in the appropriateness of institutions and the personal qualities of rulers. Dowling and Pfeffer (Reference Dowling and Pfeffer1975, 126), in another seminal account, distinguished between the operation, output, and “the goals or domain of activity” of the organization as three sources of legitimacy. Scott (Reference Scott, Walter and DiMaggio1991, 169), similarly, speaks of how legitimacy for an institution may derive primarily from “societal evaluations of organizational goals.” In the study of global governance, Barnett (Reference Barnett1997, 539) offers an early discussion of the “substantive legitimacy” of IOs, understood as the “ends that are considered desirable,” to be distinguished from procedural legitimacy, or how IOs make decisions to reach those ends. Yet, despite these attempts, the idea of social purpose as an additional institutional source of legitimacy never truly took off. Possibly, such a development was stymied by Scharpf’s (Reference Scharpf1999) influential dichotomy between input (procedure) and output (performance).

In recent years, a number of contributions have again suggested that organizational purpose may present a driver of legitimacy beliefs in global governance. These accounts typically conceive of social purpose as an institutional quality on par with procedure and performance. Scholte and Tallberg (Reference Scholte, Tallberg, Tallberg, Bäckstrand and Scholte2018, 64) acknowledge that the common distinction between procedure and performance misses potential “substance-grounded” legitimacy beliefs. Lenz and Viola (Reference Lenz and Viola2017) explicitly speak of procedure, performance, and purpose as the three central organizational features of IOs that feed into assessments of their legitimacy. Nielson et al. (Reference Nielson, Hyde and Kelley2019, 692) suggest that “actors may assess organizations not merely on how they operate and whether they accomplish their goals, but on what the goals themselves are and whether these are normatively desirable.” Taken together, these contributions suggest that citizens would be sensitive to information about an IO’s social purpose when forming legitimacy beliefs vis-à-vis IOs.

Developing this intuition further, we expect the communicated purposes of IOs to matter for legitimacy beliefs because of how they activate citizens’ ideological priors. Political ideologies are systems of normative ideas that bundle ideological content in ways that help people to orient themselves on the political spectrum and to arrive at political choices (Hamill et al. Reference Hamill, Lodge and Blake1985; Sniderman et al. Reference Sniderman, Hagen, Tetlock and Brady1986). When citizens hold a particular ideological orientation, this offers them a shortcut to political positions on a whole range of issues interpretable in ideological terms (Jost et al. Reference Jost, Federico, Napier, Freeden and Stears2013).

We suggest that an IO’s social purpose often is perceived as inherently political or normative. Promoting free trade, combatting poverty, or protecting human rights may not be regarded by citizens as neutral exercises of international problem-solving, but as associated with the furthering of certain political ideals rather than others. In some cases, these ideals are closely linked to traditional political ideologies and cleavages in society, such as the left–right dimension. For instance, free trade and deregulation are often associated with market liberalism, while redistribution and social rights are associated with socialism or social democracy. When IOs promote purposes that are interpreted by citizens as political, we would expect citizens to use information about purpose when forming opinions about IOs. Organizations with purposes that accord more with a person’s political priors are more likely to be regarded as legitimate, while IOs that promote goals that diverge from a person’s ideological leanings are less likely to be seen as legitimate.

We advance two hypotheses on the basis of this argument. First, we formulate a general expectation about the effect of social purpose on people’s legitimacy beliefs. Second, we formulate a conditional expectation about heterogeneity in effects depending on people’s political priors.

H2a: Communication about an IO’s social purpose affects its perceived legitimacy.

H2b: The effect of communication about an IO’s social purpose on legitimacy beliefs (H2a) is conditioned by people’s political ideology.

Research Design

The conjoint experiment exposes participants to hypothetical IOs that differ with respect to authority, purpose, and other institutional qualities. Its primary objective is to test hypotheses about how important communicated levels of authority and social purposes are for citizens’ legitimacy beliefs toward IOs, and how internationalist attitudes and political ideology affect these relationships.

Survey Design

The experiment is embedded in an online survey with nationally representative samples of German (N = 2,044) and American respondents (N = 2,048).Footnote 2 This cross-country design extends prior experimental studies on the effects of institutional qualities on legitimacy beliefs that have focused on a single country – the US (cf. Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Bernauer and Kachi2019). An important rationale for selecting Germany and the US is the general differences between the two countries in our moderating factors – political ideology and internationalist attitudes (cf. Chapter 5). The US is a liberal market economy with a two-party system and strong public opinion polarization, as well as an ambivalent approach to international cooperation, alternating between isolationism and internationalism. Germany is a coordinated market economy with a multiparty system and less polarized public opinion, as well as a strong commitment to international cooperation. At the same time, the two countries are similar across several important contextual conditions, including their federal political systems, their high levels of economic development, and their political centrality in most IOs of which they are members, allowing us to hold potentially confounding context factors constant. The survey was implemented by YouGov in May 2019 (see Online Appendix A).

Experimental Design

We use a conjoint experiment to test our hypotheses about the effect of communication about an IO’s authority and purpose, respectively, on legitimacy beliefs. Conjoint analysis methods were developed in psychology and marketing, and have become increasingly common in political science in recent years (Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Yamamoto and Hopkins2014). In a conjoint experiment, respondents typically receive two alternative descriptions of cases and are then asked to rank or rate these two hypothetical alternatives. These two alternative cases have multiple attributes with differing values. By systematically varying how these cases are described, analysts can estimate the importance of each attribute on respondents’ combined choices. In the context of global governance, scholars have used conjoint experiments to assess, for instance, which institutional qualities generate public support for international environmental agreements (Bechtel and Scheve Reference Bechtel and Scheve2013; Bernauer et al. Reference Bernauer, Mohrenberg and Koubi2020).

We devised a conjoint experiment in which each respondent is shown screens with two hypothetical IOs in comparison. Each IO has a set of distinct attributes. Respondents are then asked to rate their confidence in each IO. This design allows us to assess how information about different institutional features of IOs affects respondents’ legitimacy beliefs. Using hypothetical IOs allows us to estimate the effects of communicated IO attributes systematically and with great precision. While using real-world IOs would have added an element of realism to the experiment, it would have made it impossible to vary IO attributes systematically in the comparisons in such a way that effects could be established confidently. In addition, respondents may have been influenced by preexisting beliefs and knowledge about IOs when asked to choose between them.

This experimental component of the survey was introduced through a text providing context and instructions (Figure 7.1).

Figure 7.1 Example screen with survey instructions

After this introductory screen, each respondent received four randomly allocated screens. Each of these four screens compared two hypothetical IOs and asked the respondent to choose between them and to indicate its level of confidence in them. This comparison worked as follows. The order of the institutional qualities of the two IOs was randomly assigned across respondents, but consistent across the four binary comparisons for each respondent to avoid confusion. The values of the institutional qualities were fully randomized, with two exceptions. First, respondents were never given the same value on an institutional quality in a comparison across two IOs. Second, respondents were never confronted with the same screen twice.

The number of institutional qualities presented to the respondents in the experiment is well in line with the number of items respondents can meaningfully evaluate in the context of a conjoint experiment (Bansak et al. Reference Bansak, Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2018). Authority is operationalized through a categorical variable capturing the power an IO exerts over member states. This measure includes both formal – codified – and informal – social – power (Barnett and Duvall Reference Barnett and Duvall2005). We focus on IO authority as degrees of power over member states, partly because this conceptualization captures the implications of delegation and pooling for individual states (Hooghe and Marks Reference Hooghe and Marks2015) and partly because it captures how IO authority typically is expressed in elite communication. Purpose is measured using descriptions of hypothetical core mandates of IOs that are relevant in global governance, such as the protection of human rights or poverty reduction. This measure captures the moral dimension of purpose (cf. Lenz and Viola Reference Lenz and Viola2017). When measuring procedures, we highlight two central aspects of IOs – transparency and participation. While we could have selected other procedural features, such as accountability and fairness, transparency and participation are two central procedural dimensions that have received much attention in prior studies in international relations. Similarly, we select two central aspects of performance – fair outcomes and problem-solving capacity (cf. Scholte and Tallberg Reference Scholte, Tallberg, Tallberg, Bäckstrand and Scholte2018). Table 7.1 summarizes the institutional features varied in the conjoint design. Figure 7.2 offers an example of what such a screen might look like.

Table 7.1 Institutional qualities varied in the experiment

Institutional quality

Values

Authority

The organization […]

has limited power over member countries

has some power over member countries

has extensive power over member countries

Purpose

The organization works to […]

protect human rights

promote public health

reduce poverty

promote free trade

ensure peace and security

combat climate change

Procedures

  1. (i) Transparency: Information about the organization’s decision-making […]

is public

is partially public

is confidential

  1. (ii) Participation: In the organization’s decision-making […]

citizens have a say

NGOs have a say

all countries have an equal say

only the powerful countries have a say

Performance

  1. (i) Fair outcome: The decisions of the organization […]

benefit all countries equally

benefit some countries more than others

  1. (ii) Problem-solving: The decisions of the organization […]

solve most important problems

solve some important problems

solve few important problems

Figure 7.2 Example screen with conjoint experiment

In operationalizing these institutional qualities, we faced a choice between nuanced and stark alternatives. We opted for more nuanced alternatives, since we believe those better capture the variation that exists in real-world global governance. For example, we chose to operationalize the dimension of authority by distinguishing between an IO having “limited,” “some,” or “extensive” power over member states, rather than simply “no” or “extensive” power.

The two outcome variables of interest tap into individuals’ confidence in IOs. Our preferred measure of legitimacy beliefs is the degree of confidence (see Chapter 3). We measure this degree on a scale from 1 (no confidence at all) to 9 (complete confidence) (Figure 7.2). For the purpose of robustness checks and to assess the sensitivity of experimental results across subgroups, we also ask respondents to indicate which of the two hypothetical IOs they would prefer. Answering this latter question only requires choosing between two, which is cognitively less demanding for respondents than indicating their confidence in either IO.

The experiment was preceded by indicators for the purpose of balance tests and additional robustness checks: intentional media consumption, cognitive mobilization, generalized trust, confidence in domestic government, and knowledge about global governance. The experiment was followed by an attention check. We use the information from this attention check to limit the sample in the main analysis to those respondents who correctly passed this test. Finally, YouGov provides demographic and political data on the respondents as background information, such as information on gender, age, education, and geographical region (see Online Appendix R for the entire questionnaire in English and German).

Measuring Internationalist Attitudes and Political Ideology

To explore H1b and H2b about a conditioning effect of attitudes toward international cooperation and political ideology on treatment effects, the survey assessed respondents’ opinions on international cooperation and partisan identification. The first indicator asked respondents to indicate if they think that international cooperation is a “good thing,” a “bad thing,” or “neither good nor bad.” The answers to this question reveal that similarly high shares of the population in both countries (between 71 and 74 percent) indicate that they think international cooperation is a good thing (Figure 7.3).

Figure 7.3 Internationalist attitudes in Germany and the US

Notes: Weighted percentage of those thinking that international cooperation is a bad thing, neither good nor bad, or a good thing.

The indicator for partisan identification is created based on a standard question about whether there is a particular political party they feel closer to than all the other parties. We described the patterns for this indicator in Chapter 5, which uses the same respondents for its two experiments. In short, partisan identification differs between Germany and the US. US public opinion is more polarized, since only about 18 percent of US citizens are estimated to be independents and the rest are either Democrats or Republicans. In Germany, about 22 percent are independents and the rest identify with a much larger number of political parties (Figure 5.1). That US opinion is more polarized than German public opinion can also be seen when looking at the distribution of left–right ideology (Figure 7.4).

Figure 7.4 Left–right ideology in Germany and the US

Notes: Weighted percentage.

Results

We begin by presenting the effects of communication about authority and purpose, and then turn to an analysis of the conditioning impact of internationalist attitudes and partisanship on these effects. We conclude by presenting a range of validity and robustness checks.

Effects of Communication about Authority and Purpose

Figures 7.5 and 7.6 show our estimates of authority and purpose as well as the other institutional qualities on confidence. The dots represent the estimated average marginal component effects (AMCEs) of a given value for each quality on individual confidence toward the packaged IO profile, relative to a reference category or baseline. In other words, the AMCEs express the degree to which an IO feature increases (or decreases) citizen confidence in an IO. The bars indicate 95 percent confidence intervals, and the points without bars indicate the baseline for a given value of an institutional quality. The interpretation of each estimate is relative to the baseline for that dimension.

Figure 7.5 Effects of institutional qualities in the US

Notes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Vignette descriptions shortened for the sake of presentation.

To examine if authority and purpose matter, we need to decide on a baseline for each indicator. For all indicators except purpose, we use the lowest category as a baseline. That is, unlike in other experiments such as vignette experiments, there is no control group but a baseline. For example, for authority, we compare the effects of “some” and “extensive” power over member states to “limited” power. For the purpose, this logic is not applicable and we need another motivation. Here, we assume that there may be different understandings of social purpose among citizens, where ensuring peace and security arguable is one of the least contentious purposes of an IO. We thus use the protection of peace and security as a baseline.

The results clearly show that authority matters, thereby supporting H1a in both Germany and the US. Moving from an IO with limited power over its member states to one with extensive power over its member states decreases legitimacy beliefs by 0.221 in the US (p < 0.000, Figure 7.5) and 0.182 points in Germany (p < 0.000, Figure 7.6) on the 1–9 confidence scale. By contrast, moving from limited to some power does not have any effect on confidence. This finding suggests that respondents react to the formulation “extensive power” and not “some power.”

Figure 7.6 Effects of institutional qualities in Germany

Notes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Vignette descriptions shortened for the sake of presentation.

In addition, the results indicate that social purpose matters, thereby corroborating H2a in both countries. In the US (Figure 7.5), the purpose of combating climate change leads to a 0.309 point decrease on the confidence scale (p < 0.000). This is the strongest purpose effect on legitimacy beliefs when compared to the baseline of ensuring peace and security. We also find that moving from the baseline to the purpose of promoting free trade leads to a 0.177 point decrease (p < 0.007) – the second strongest effect.

In Germany, two effects are significant (Figure 7.6). The strongest effect is recorded for free trade. Moving from the baseline to the promotion of free trade represents a 0.401 point decrease on the confidence scale (p < 0.000). Similarly, moving from the baseline to the promotion of public health leads to a decrease of 0.267 points (p < 0.000). Taken together, these findings suggest similar results for Germany and the US in the sense that social purpose matters for legitimacy beliefs.

Communication of other institutional qualities also matters for legitimacy beliefs, further strengthening our confidence in the findings in Chapter 6. The effects are very similar in the two countries. We start with procedure-related qualities. In the case of transparency, moving from the baseline of a confidential organization to one that is public increases confidence by about 0.457 points (p < 0.000) in both the US and Germany. In the case of participation, moving from the baseline of an IO in which only powerful countries have a say to an IO in which citizens have a say increases confidence by an estimated 0.554 points in the US (p < 0.000) and 0.634 in Germany (p < 0.000). We then turn to performance-related qualities. In the case of fair outcomes, moving from the baseline of an IO that benefits some countries more than others to an IO that benefits all members equally raises confidence by 0.316 points (p < 0.000) in the US and 0.374 points (p < 0.000) in Germany. Finally, in the case of problem-solving, moving from the baseline of an IO that solves few important problems to an IO that solves the most important problems increases confidence by 0.293 points (p < 0.000) in the US and 0.393 points (p < 0.000) in Germany.

Can we expect information about the institutional design of IOs to lead to substantial shifts in confidence in the real world? We examine this by predicting levels of confidence for two hypothetical IOs (cf. Bechtel and Scheve Reference Bechtel and Scheve2013). By prediction, we mean the computation of levels of confidence for each of the hypothetical IOs based on 1,000 country-specific simulations using the results of respondents’ confidence ratings (see King Reference King, Michael and Wittenberg2000, for a discussion of the methodology). The first IO is one that has unattractive characteristics based on our experimental results: Its purpose is to combat climate change in the US (and promote free trade in Germany), it has extensive power over member states, it solves few important problems, it yields benefits for specific countries at the expense of others, it provides only powerful states with a say, and it is confidential. The second IO has all the features of an attractive design based on our experimental results: Its mandate is to ensure peace and security, it has limited power over member states, it solves most important problems, it yields equal benefits for all countries, it provides citizens with a say, and it is public.

This additional analysis suggests that information on how IOs are designed, from the most unpopular design to the most popular, may lead to noteworthy shifts in legitimacy beliefs. In Germany, average confidence is predicted to be 4.2 for an IO with an unattractive design on the 1–9 confidence scale, but as much as 5.9 in the case of an IO with popular design features. Similar results are found in the US, where we predict an average confidence level of about 4.3 for the unattractive IO design and about 6 for the attractive IO design.

Finally, we are interested in whether the findings on authority and purpose depend on respondents’ level of political awareness, as theorized in Chapter 3. To this end, we examine differences in effect sizes for subgroups that differ in their level of education and knowledge about global governance, respectively. Looking at differences in AMCEs, we do not find any systematic moderating effects of either indicator on authority and purpose effects (Online Appendices S1 and S2).

Interaction Analysis

Next, we examine H1b and H2b, which predict that the effects of authority and purpose on legitimacy beliefs are moderated by people’s preexisting political beliefs. To test H1b, we focus on attitudes toward international cooperation as a potential moderating factor. To test H2b, we concentrate on partisan identification.

For this analysis, we use a different way of calculating and comparing treatment effects, since each subgroup will have a different average value on the baseline quality used to identify effect strength. We, therefore, complement AMCEs (used to infer differences in causal effects within subgroups) with marginal means (MMs) (used to infer if subgroups differ in how they value specific institutional qualities) (Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Yamamoto and Hopkins2014; Leeper et al. Reference Leeper, Hobolt and Tilley2020). MMs express the preferences of respondents for all institutional features; these quantities are column and row mean outcomes for each institutional feature level, averaging across all other features. We calculate differences between MMs to check whether observed differences between MMs of two subgroups are statistically significant with regard to a specific institutional feature. If they are, then that feature can be assumed to shape confidence more in a particular subgroup than in another. As suggested by Leeper et al. (Reference Leeper, Hobolt and Tilley2020), we use the discrete choice outcome variable to estimate AMCEs for each institutional feature separately and then compare those estimates to MMs to ascertain the sensitivity of the analysis (see Online Appendix S for detailed results).

Figure 7.7 shows the results for H1b in the US. We find that attitudes toward international cooperation, indeed, moderate the effects of authority on IO legitimacy beliefs. However, the effect is partly the opposite of what we theorized. The AMCEs suggest that Americans with positive or neutral attitudes toward international cooperation react with weaker confidence to information that IOs have extensive power compared to the baseline of limited power. This negative effect is not found among citizens with more nationalist attitudes. The MMs indicate that this negative effect on confidence is stronger among those people who are neutral toward international cooperation than among those with internationalist attitudes (p < 0.023).

Figure 7.7 Effects of authority in the US, by internationalism

Notes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Do you think international cooperation is: a bad thing, a good thing, or neither good nor bad?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S3 for detailed results.

This result is puzzling in view of our expectation that people with more internationalist attitudes would be more tolerant of greater IO authority. However, the finding makes more sense if we consider that people with negative attitudes toward international cooperation generally are less receptive to information about IOs than people with positive attitudes, as we discovered in Chapter 5. Those people who are negatively predisposed appear to already have strong opinions that are less malleable. That said, the results also provide some evidence consistent with our original expectation, since people with neutral attitudes toward international cooperation appear to react more negatively to information about IOs having extensive power than people with positive attitudes.

Figure 7.8 shows the corresponding results for Germany, where we do not find that internationalist attitudes systematically condition the relationship between authority and confidence, which contradicts H1b. While we find a negative effect of an IO having extensive authority among those neutral toward international cooperation (as in the US), differences in MMs among subgroups are not statistically significant, indicating that the three subgroups do not hold different preferences regarding the authority of IOs in Germany.

Figure 7.8 Effects of authority in Germany, by internationalism

Notes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Do you think international cooperation is: a bad thing, a good thing, or neither good nor bad?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S4 for detailed results.

Next, we are interested in whether partisan identification moderates the effects of an IO’s social purpose on citizens’ confidence in this organization (H2b). The results for the US are in line with this hypothesis (Figure 7.9). The AMCEs indicate that Democrats respond with greater confidence when informed that an IO engages in poverty alleviation compared to the baseline of ensuring peace and security, and with weaker confidence when the IO promotes free trade. Republicans, on the one hand, respond negatively to information about IOs fighting poverty and climate change compared to the baseline. The differences in MMs between Democrats and Republicans are statistically significant (except in the area of public health), indicating that partisan identification systematically moderates the effects of different social purposes on IO confidence.

Figure 7.9 Effects of social purpose in the US, by partisanship

Notes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Is there a particular political party you feel closer to than all the other parties?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S5 for detailed results.

In Germany, we examine H2b across partisans of the historically two largest parties, the CDU/CSU and SPD, which are also featured in Chapter 5, to make the analysis more comparable to the one of the US. The results do not support H2b (Figure 7.10). The AMCEs show that the effects of social purpose on confidence are quite similar across all subgroups, largely reflecting the aggregate pattern in Figure 7.6. The exception is the group of partisans who are neither SPD nor CDU/CSU, which appear to be more easily affected by an IO’s social purpose. In this group, all purposes except poverty alleviation lead to lower IO confidence compared to the baseline purpose of ensuring peace and security. However, the MMs for each purpose are not statistically different from each other across subgroups, suggesting that different groups of partisans in Germany do not have different preferences regarding the social purposes of IOs.

Figure 7.10 Effects of social purpose in Germany, by partisanship

Notes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Is there a particular political party you feel closer to than all the other parties?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S6 for detailed results.

Taken together, the evidence from the interaction analysis is mixed in that H1b and H2b are only supported in the US. We attribute this result partially to the level of polarization in public opinion and the political party landscape in the US. Against this backdrop, it is not so surprising that we do not find CDU and SPD partisans to react differently to information about the authority and purpose of IOs.

Validity and Robustness Checks

We perform several validity and robustness checks, which corroborate our findings about the effects of communicated authority and purpose on legitimacy beliefs (Figure 7.5). First, we conduct balance checks to assess whether the randomization produced well-balanced experimental groups (cf. Hainmueller et al. Reference Hainmueller, Yamamoto and Hopkins2014), which indicate that the attributes are jointly balanced (Online Appendices T1–T2).

Second, we replicate the analyses using the alternative dependent variable indicating whether respondents chose organization 1 or 2. Respondents were asked to make this discrete choice right after they were presented with the different institutional qualities, as described earlier. Respondents tended to be consistent in indicating relatively high levels of confidence in the organization they chose, and lower levels of confidence in the organization they did not choose. Interestingly, in Germany, a larger number of social purpose cues have effects when examining organization choice than when examining confidence in an IO, potentially because it is an easier task to choose between two organizations than to rate confidence in both. The social purposes of human rights, free trade, climate change, and public health all make an organization less likely to be chosen compared to the baseline security IO (Online Appendix T1).

Third, we checked whether our results in Figures 7.5 and 7.6 are conditional upon other individual characteristics than those we hypothesized, such as confidence in government. For this purpose, we use responses to a question about a respondent’s confidence in the government on a scale from no confidence at all (0) to complete confidence (10). Results suggest that there are no differences in AMCEs at different levels of confidence in government, so we do not investigate this further through MMs (Online Appendix T2).

Fourth and finally, we run the analysis from Figure 7.5 by including both attentive and nonattentive respondents. Our attention check asked respondents the following question after they had completed about 70 percent of the survey: “We are interested in learning about your preferences on a variety of topics, including colors. To demonstrate that you’ve read this much, just go ahead and select both red and green among the alternatives below, no matter what your favorite color is. Yes, ignore the question below and select both of those options. What is your favorite color?” Correct answers were coded as one and incorrect answers as zero (Bechtel and Scheve Reference Bechtel and Scheve2013). About 66 percent of US respondents were attentive, while only 54 percent of German respondents were attentive. When we test the robustness of the experimental results using the full sample, which also includes the nonattentive respondents, results vary slightly. In the US, a free trade purpose does not have the same negative effect when including nonattentive respondents as well; however, a climate change purpose continues to have a negative effect on confidence in IOs compared to the baseline security purpose. In Germany, the main results are robust, with the addition that also a human rights purpose has a negative effect of confidence on IOs compared to the baseline security purpose (Online Appendix T3).

Discussion

The analysis supports our expectation that communication about an IO’s authority and purpose impacts citizen legitimacy beliefs. First, the findings suggest that authority shapes legitimacy beliefs in the sense that IOs presented as having extensive power over member states are perceived as less legitimate than IOs with limited power over member states. This finding is robust in both Germany and the US, and consistent with previously untested expectations that authority breeds contestation and legitimacy deficits (Zürn et al. Reference Zürn, Binder and Ecker-Ehrhardt2012; Zürn Reference Zürn2018). However, it is only in the US that authority has a weaker negative effect on legitimacy beliefs among citizens more in favor of international cooperation, as we expected. In this respect, our findings deviate somewhat from results in another recent study, which concludes that authority has a positive effect on legitimacy beliefs among more internationalist citizens and a negative effect among more nationalist citizens (Schlipphak et al. Reference Schlipphak, Schäfer and Treib2021).

Second, the evidence strongly suggests that information about an IO’s social purpose matters for legitimacy beliefs. This finding supports the supposition that an organization’s social purpose is important in and of itself – irrespective of other institutional qualities (Scott Reference Scott, Walter and DiMaggio1991; Barnett Reference Barnett1997; Lenz and Viola Reference Lenz and Viola2017). In both countries, free trade cues stand out as having particularly strong effects. In addition, in the US, presenting an IOs as involved in climate change decreases perceptions of IO legitimacy, while, in Germany, the same effect results when presenting an IO as involved in health. Political ideology conditions the effects of social purpose in predictable ways in the US, with Democrats being positively affected, and Republicans negatively affected, by a poverty alleviation purpose.

Third, these findings confirm the positive results from Chapter 6 about the impact of communication regarding the procedures and performances of IOs. This finding is also in line with a large literature on legitimacy in domestic and global governance emphasizing the importance of procedure and performance (e.g., Tyler Reference Tyler2006; Dellmuth and Tallberg Reference Dellmuth and Tallberg2015; Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Bernauer and Kachi2019; Esaiasson et al. Reference Esaiasson, Persson, Gilljam and Lindholm2019). Importantly, this chapter now establishes the effects of procedure and performance also when assessing them simultaneously with each other and with the effects of authority and purpose. The results demonstrate that people are more sensitive to information about an IO’s procedure and performance when forming legitimacy beliefs compared to information about its authority and purpose. While both procedure and performance have strong effects, procedure-related qualities appear particularly important in both countries.

Taken together, these results suggest that citizens care about the inherently political nature of IOs manifested in their power and purpose. When forming beliefs about the legitimacy of IOs, citizens do not only consider how IOs take decisions and whether those decisions are effective but also if IOs pursue social aims citizens agree with and whether the authority of these organizations clashes with state autonomy. Whether and to what extent the authority and purpose of IOs function as a boost or a drag on legitimacy depends on citizens’ political priors.

While our findings are reasonably similar across the two countries, there is also some variation in effects, which calls for interpretation. First, we observe that internationalist attitudes condition the negative effects of authority on legitimacy beliefs in the US but not in Germany. One explanation for the absence of a moderating effect of internationalist attitudes in Germany might be that citizens in this country already are accustomed to an IO with high levels of authority (the EU), which potentially could reduce the differences across subgroups.

Second, we observe that the specific purposes of IOs which matter for citizens’ legitimacy beliefs partly vary between the two countries. In order to understand this variation, we need to consider how these issues unite and divide citizens differently in the two countries. In this respect, the aggregate effects at the country level hide partisan dynamics that are quite different in the US and Germany. In the US, the purposes of IOs divide citizens along partisan lines: While climate change and poverty mandates have negative effects on legitimacy beliefs among Republicans compared to a baseline of ensuring peace and security, a poverty mandate has a positive effect, and a free trade purpose a negative effect among Democrats. In Germany, systematic partisan divisions are not found: CDU/CSU and SPD partisans respond in very similar ways to communicate about purpose. We suspect that these differences reflect the varying degrees of partisan polarization in the US and Germany, as discussed in Chapter 5.

This combination of extensive similarities and some variations in effects underlines the importance of examining the effects of authority and purpose in a comparative setting. Our findings confirm that these effects are not specific to a single country but also suggest that country context may shape their exact nature. Future studies could fruitfully build on our study of the US and Germany to examine how IO authority and purpose matter in a broader sample of IOs and countries. Likewise, future research could usefully extend the range of social purposes examined to other issues salient in public debate, such as migration.

Conclusion

This chapter has explored how communication about the authority and purpose of IOs affects citizens’ legitimacy beliefs toward these organizations. While a growing literature has examined a variety of institutional qualities linked to the procedures and performances of IOs, this chapter is the first attempt to assess systematically the effects of an IO’s authority and purpose. Theoretically, we have developed arguments for why citizens should be sensitive to information about an IO’s authority and purpose, which in many ways are highly political aspects of IOs. Empirically, we have evaluated the effects of institutional features related to authority, purpose, procedure, and performance on legitimacy beliefs in Germany and the US, using a conjoint experimental design with hypothetical IOs.

The central findings are threefold. First, communication about an IO’s authority and purpose matters for citizens’ legitimacy beliefs. When IOs are presented as having more authority over member states, this results in more negative assessments of their legitimacy. And when IOs are presented as serving some social purposes rather than others, this shapes how citizens perceive their legitimacy. Second, the strength of these effects depends on citizens’ political priors in the US (but not in Germany), in terms of their attitudes toward international cooperation and their partisanship. Third, information about procedure and performance remains very important as a source of legitimacy for IOs when the effects of all four institutional features are assessed simultaneously in a conjoint design.

These findings suggest two broader implications. First, they indicate that elites’ efforts to invoke the authority and purpose of IOs when communicating about these organizations are hitting home. When Boris Johnson calls on the UK to take back control from the EU, or when Guy Verhofstadt explains that the EU needs more power to fight the COVID-19 pandemic, they tap into concerns that people care about. Likewise, the way in which elites present the social purposes of IOs has predictable effects on people’s perceptions of these organizations. While, for sure, elites cannot stray too far from IOs’ actual authority and purpose, their communication can frame these features of IOs in ways that make people more or less positive toward them. Do IOs control member states or have the authority to tackle joint cross-border problems? Are IOs seeking to ensure peace and security or are they engaged in military interventions? Recent mobilization of public opinion against IOs by antiglobalist elites successfully exploits people’s concerns with these highly political features of IOs and the scope for communication to shape attitudes (De Vries et al. Reference De Vries, Hobolt and Walter2021).

Second, this chapter’s examination of authority and purpose exemplifies how research on politics in the global realm can take us into novel territory in scholarship on the sources of political legitimacy in general. Studies in comparative politics typically take the authority of governments as given and do not consider purpose, since governments by nature have general-purpose orientations. In contrast, task-specific orientations are more common in global governance (Lenz et al. Reference Lenz, Bezuijen, Hooghe and Marks2015; Hooghe et al. Reference Hooghe, Lenz and Marks2019). With the exception of the UN and a number of regional IOs, which approximate general-purpose organizations, other IOs are specialized vehicles for the advancement of particular political goals. Consider the WTO (free trade), ILO (labor rights), IMF (financial stability), UNFCCC (climate sustainability), and UN Women (female empowerment). These organizations not only present arenas for dealing with the specific policy problems but usually also have these goals inscribed into their mandates and are known to actively “teach” these norms to state and nonstate audiences (Finnemore Reference Finnemore1993). By exploiting variation that exists in the global realm, we can thus contribute novel knowledge about the importance of organizational purpose for legitimacy beliefs.

Footnotes

1 In an alternative view, legitimacy is a prerequisite for authority (see Hurd Reference Hurd2007, 60–61; Lake Reference Hainmueller and Hiscox2007).

2 The experiment is preregistered with EGAP (No. 20190507AA). See: http://egap.org/registration/5711.

Figure 0

Figure 7.1 Example screen with survey instructions

Figure 1

Table 7.1 Institutional qualities varied in the experiment

Figure 2

Figure 7.2 Example screen with conjoint experiment

Figure 3

Figure 7.3 Internationalist attitudes in Germany and the USNotes: Weighted percentage of those thinking that international cooperation is a bad thing, neither good nor bad, or a good thing.

Figure 4

Figure 7.4 Left–right ideology in Germany and the USNotes: Weighted percentage.

Figure 5

Figure 7.5 Effects of institutional qualities in the USNotes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Vignette descriptions shortened for the sake of presentation.

Figure 6

Figure 7.6 Effects of institutional qualities in GermanyNotes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Vignette descriptions shortened for the sake of presentation.

Figure 7

Figure 7.7 Effects of authority in the US, by internationalismNotes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Do you think international cooperation is: a bad thing, a good thing, or neither good nor bad?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S3 for detailed results.

Figure 8

Figure 7.8 Effects of authority in Germany, by internationalismNotes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Do you think international cooperation is: a bad thing, a good thing, or neither good nor bad?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S4 for detailed results.

Figure 9

Figure 7.9 Effects of social purpose in the US, by partisanshipNotes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Is there a particular political party you feel closer to than all the other parties?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S5 for detailed results.

Figure 10

Figure 7.10 Effects of social purpose in Germany, by partisanshipNotes: Sample includes attentive respondents only. AMCEs and MMs with their respective 95 percent confidence intervals. Weighted data. Answers to the question: “Is there a particular political party you feel closer to than all the other parties?” Dependent variable: Discrete choice between two organizations. See Online Appendix S6 for detailed results.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×