Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T15:58:32.587Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Activity formation as an alternative strategy of instruction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Joachim Lompscher
Affiliation:
Potsdam University
Yrjö Engeström
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Reijo Miettinen
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki
Raija-Leena Punamäki
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Teachers' experiences and countless studies show that in the majority of classrooms real learning results do not correspond with either the expectations of society or the efforts of most of the teachers and learners. High rates of forgetting, low levels of applicability of knowledge and skills, insufficient quality of problem finding and problem solving, and aversion to school learning are often demonstrated in discussions and reports.

Results from one of our studies illustrate this point. Fourth-grade pupils (10 to 11 years of age) had to solve two text problems. Both problems had the same mathematical structure but were different in content. The mathematical structure of both problems corresponded with the students' available mental preconditions. In both cases, the same operations (addition and multiplication of whole numbers) had to be used. The first problem referred to familiar things often used in such tasks. The second problem, however, was about liquids, a type of problem with which the children had little calculatory experience.

The study was part of a larger investigation performed in normal classrooms in several schools (Reinhold, 1988). Earlier results obtained with different methods allowed us to select pupils as representatives of relatively homogeneous performance groups (high, average, and low performance). The difficulties in uncovering the mathematical structure and in finding necessary operations increased sharply from performance group I (high) to group III (low) (Figure 16.1). Students with similar psychological characteristics but taught on the basis of another instructional strategy reached significantly higher results, although differences between the performance groups show the same trend that occurs in the classes that received traditional instruction.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1999

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×