Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T14:55:20.637Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond simple utility in predicting self-control fatigue: A proximate alternative to the opportunity cost model

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2013

Michael Inzlicht
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Scarborough, Toronto, Ontario M1C 1A4, Canada. michael.inzlicht@utoronto.cawww.michaelinzlicht.com
Brandon J. Schmeichel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Texas A & M University, College Station, TX 77843-4235. schmeichel@tamu.eduhttps://sites.google.com/site/bjschmeichel/

Abstract

The opportunity cost model offers an ultimate explanation of ego depletion that helps to move the field beyond biologically improbable resource accounts. The model's more proximate explanation, however, falls short of accounting for much data and is based on an outdated view of human rationality. We suggest that our own process model offers a better proximate account of self-control fatigue.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainslie, G. (1991) Derivation of “rational” economic behavior from hyperbolic discount curves. American Economic Review 81:134–40.Google Scholar
Alberts, H. J. E. M., Martijn, C. & de Vries, N. K. (2011) Fighting self-control failure: Overcoming ego depletion by increasing self-awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47:5862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108(3):624–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M. & Schmeichel, B. J. (2007) Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92(2):325–36.Google Scholar
Hagger, M. S. & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2013) The sweet taste of success: The presence of glucose in the oral cavity moderates the depletion of self-control resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 39:2741.Google Scholar
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C. & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010a) Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 136(4):495525. doi:10.1037/a0019486.Google Scholar
Hockey, G. (1983) Stress and fatigue in human performance. Wiley.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M. & Gutsell, J. N. (2007) Running on empty: Neural signals for self-control failure. Psychological Science 18(11):933–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inzlicht, M. & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012) What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7(5):450–63. doi:10.1177/1745691612454134.Google Scholar
Job, V., Dweck, C. S. & Walton, G. M. (2010) Ego depletion – Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science 21(11):1686–93.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1979) Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrica 47:263–91.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2010a) Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks? Evolutionary Psychology 8(2):244–59.Google Scholar
Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., Scholer, A. A., Meier, B. P., Noreen, E. E., D'Agostino, P. R. & Martin, V. (2012) Motivational versus metabolic effects of carbohydrates on self-control. Psychological Science 23(10): 1137–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M. & Baumeister, R. F. (2000) Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin 126(2):247–59. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748642.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., Collins, R. & Neinhaus, K. (2002) Self-control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 16:113–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M. & Slessareva, E. (2003) Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29(7):894906. doi:10.1177/0146167203029007008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmeichel, B. J., Harmon-Jones, C. & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010) Exercising self-control increases approach motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99:162–73.Google Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E. & West, S. A. (2011) Evolutionary theory and the ultimate-proximate distinction in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6:3847.Google Scholar
Strack, F. & Deutsch, R. (2004) Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review 8:220–47.Google Scholar
Stucke, T. S. & Baumeister, R. F. (2006) Ego depletion and aggressive behavior: Is the inhibition of aggression a limited resource? European Journal of Social Psychology 36(1):113.Google Scholar
Teper, R. & Inzlicht, M. (2013) Meditation, mindfulness, and executive control: The importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience 8:8592. doi:10.1093/scan/nss045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wan, E. W. & Sternthal, B. (2008) Regulating the effects of depletion through monitoring. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34:3246.Google Scholar