Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T14:17:51.130Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Opportunity prioritization, biofunctional simultaneity, and psychological mutual exclusion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2013

Asghar Iran-Nejad
Affiliation:
Educational Studies in Psychology, Research Methodology, and Counseling, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL 35487. airannej@bamaed.ua.edu
Sally Ann Zengaro
Affiliation:
Counselor Education and Psychology, Delta State University, Cleveland, MS 38732. szengaro@deltastate.edu

Abstract

We argue that prioritization, simultaneity, and mutual exclusion are mind-body integration functions that can't be addressed meaningfully at the psychological (computational) level alone. We describe the outlook for an integration between Kurzban et al.'s profound discussion of opportunity cost/benefit prioritization and decades of related development in biofunctional science.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brehm, J. W. & Miron, A. (2007) Evidence that positive and negative affect are mutually exclusive. Paper presented at the Association for Psychological Science 19th Annual Convention, Washington, D.C., May 24–27, 2007.Google Scholar
Diener, E. & Iran-Nejad, A. (1986) The relationship in experience between various types of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50:1031–38. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.50.5.1031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. & Haidt, J. (2002) How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(12):517–23.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. (1980) The schema: A structural or a functional pattern. Center for the Study of Reading Technical Report No. 159. University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED181449).Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. (1989) A nonconnectionist schema theory of understanding surprise-ending stories. Discourse Processes 12:127–48. doi:110.1080/01638538909544723.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. (2000) Knowledge, self-regulation, and the brain-mind cycle of reflection. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 21:6788.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. (2012) Affect, self-regulation and moral behavior in body-mind cycle of reflection. Paper presented at the Association for Psychological Science Annual Convention, Chicago, IL, May 24–27, 2012.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. & Bordbar, F. (2013) The paradox of the missing function: How similar is moral mutualism to biofunctional understanding? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(1):9394. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000957.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A., Clore, G. L. & Vondruska, R. J. (1984) Affect: A functional perspective. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 5:279310.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. & Gregg, M. (2001) The brain-mind cycle of reflection. Teachers College Record 103:868–95.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. & Gregg, M. (2011) The nonsegmental context of segmental understanding: A biofunctional systems perspective. American Journal of Educational Studies 4(1):4160.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A., Marsh, G. E. & Clements, A. C. (1992) The figure and the ground of constructive brain functioning: Beyond explicit memory processes. Educational Psychologist 27:473–92.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. & Ortony, A. (1984) A biofunctional model of distributed mental content, mental structures, awareness, and attention. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 5:171210.Google Scholar
Iran-Nejad, A. & Ortony, A. (1985) Qualitative and quantitative sources of affect: How valence and unexpectedness relate pleasantness and preference. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 6(3):257–78.Google Scholar
Prawat, R. S. (2000) Keep the solution, broaden the problem: Commentary on “Knowledge, self-regulation, and the brain-mind cycle of reflection.” The Journal of Mind and Behavior 21:8996.Google Scholar
Satyadas, A., Iran-Nejad, A., Chissom, B. & Chen, H. C. (1993) Intelligence: Exact computation or biofunctional cognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31(3):175–78.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U. (2001) Pleasure, displeasure, and mixed feelings: Are semantic opposites mutually exclusive? Cognition and Emotion 15(1):8197. doi:10.1080/0269993004200123.Google Scholar