No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
A cognitive developmental approach is essential to understanding cumulative technological culture
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 August 2020
Abstract
Osiurak and Reynaud argue that children are not a good methodological choice to examine cumulative technological culture (CTC). However, the paper ignores other current work that suggests that young children do display some aspects of creative problem-solving. We argue that using multiple methodologies and examining how technical-reasoning develops in children will provide crucial support for a cognitive approach to CTC.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Beck, S. R., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Demery, Z., Iliffe, L., Rishi, S. & Chappell, J. (2014) Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems? Frontiers in Psychology 5:1395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, S. R., Williams, C., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A. & Chappell, J. (2016) Individual differences in children's innovative problem-solving are not predicted by divergent thinking or executive functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1690):20150190. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, K., Kendal, R. L. & Flynn, E. G. (2015) Imitate or innovate? Children's innovation is influenced by the efficacy of observed behaviour. Cognition 142:322–32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, K., Kendal, R. L. & Flynn, E. G. (2016) Eureka!: What is innovation, how does it develop, and who does it? Child Development 87(5):1505–19. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chappell, J., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A. & Beck, S. R. (2013) The development of tool manufacture in humans: What helps young children make innovative tools? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 368:20120409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Z. & Klahr, D. (1999) All other things being equal: Acquisition and transfer of control of variables strategy. Child Development 70:1098–1120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J. & Beck, S. R. (2014) The puzzling difficulty of tool innovation: Why can't children piece their knowledge together? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 125:110–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J. & Beck, S. R. (2019) Is tool modification more difficult than innovation? Cognitive Development 52:100811. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebel, S. J., Hanus, D. & Call, J. (2019) How prior experience and task presentation modulate innovation in 6-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 180:87–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D., Levine, S. C., Ping, R., Isaia, A., Dhillon, S., Bradley, C. & Honke, G. (2016) Rapid learning in a children's museum via analogical comparison. Cognitive Science 40:224–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (2012) Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical research, and policy implications. Science (New York, N.Y.) 337:1623–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L. & Lucas, C. G. (2015) When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24:87–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., O'Grady, S., Lucas, C. G., Griffiths, T. L., Wente, A., Bridgers, S., Aboody, R., Fung, H. & Dahl, R. E. (2017) Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114:7892–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, A. J., Burdett, E. R., Burgess, V., McGuigan, N., Wood, L. A., Harris, P. L. & Whiten, A. (2017) Children's selective copying of their mother versus an expert. Child Development 88:2026–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuigan, N., Burdett, E., Burgess, V., Dean, L., Lucas, A., Vale, G. & Whiten, A. (2017) Innovation and social transmission in experimental micro-societies: Exploring the scope of cumulative culture in young children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 372:1735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reindle, E., Apperly, I. A., Beck, S. R. & Tennie, C. (2017) Young children copy cumulative technological design in the absence of action information. Scientific Reports 7:1788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reindle, E. & Tennie, C. (2018) Young children fail to generate an additive ratchet effect in an open-ended construction task. PLoS ONE 13:e0197828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tecwyn, E. C., Thorpe, S. K. S. & Chappell, J. (2014) Development of planning in 4- to 10-year-old children: Reducing inhibitory demands does not improve performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 125:85–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voigt, B., Pauen, S. & Bechtel-Kuehne, S. (2019) Getting the mouse out of the box: Tool innovation in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 184:65–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalley, C. I., Cutting, N. & Beck, S. R. (2017) The effect of prior experience on children's tool innovation. Journal of Child Experimental Psychology 161:81–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture
Related commentaries (26)
A cognitive approach to cumulative technological culture is useful and necessary but only if it also applies to other species
A cognitive developmental approach is essential to understanding cumulative technological culture
A cognitive transition underlying both technological and social aspects of cumulative culture
A little too technical: The threat of intellectualising technical reasoning
A long view of cumulative technological culture
A theory limited in scope and evidence
Causal learning in CTC: Adaptive and collaborative
Chimpanzees' technical reasoning: Taking fieldwork and ontogeny seriously
How will we find the elephant in the room?
Human tool cognition relies on teleology
Implications for technological reserve development in advancing age, cognitive impairment, and dementia
Missing in action: Tool use is action based
New Caledonian crows afford invaluable comparative insights into human cumulative technological culture
Putting social cognitive mechanisms back into cumulative technological culture: Social interactions serve as a mechanism for children's early knowledge acquisition
Refining our understanding of the “elephant in the room”
Shared intentionality shapes humans' technical know-how
Supporting the weight of the elephant in the room: Technical intelligence propped up by social cognition and language
Taking into account the wider evolutionary context of cumulative cultural evolution
Technical reasoning alone does not take humans this far
The blind men and the elephant: What is missing cognitively in the study of cumulative technological evolution
The crow in the room: New Caledonian crows offer insight into the necessary and sufficient conditions for cumulative cultural evolution
The social side of innovation
The technical reasoning hypothesis does not rule out the potential key roles of imitation and working memory for CTC
Tools as “petrified memes”: A duality
What matters emotionally: The importance of pride for cumulative culture
Where does the elephant come from? The evolution of causal cognition is the key
Author response
The elephant in the China shop: When technical reasoning meets cumulative technological culture