It is well-established that psychotherapies can effectively treat depression. In the past four decades, more than 400 randomized controlled trials have been conducted (Cuijpers, Reference Cuijpers2015), showing that different types of psychotherapy are effective (Barth et al. Reference Barth, Munder, Gerger, Nüesch, Trelle, Znoj, Jüni and Cuijpers2013), that the effects do not or only marginally differ from each other, that they are comparable with those of antidepressants, and that combined treatment is more effective than psychotherapy or medication alone (Cuijpers, Reference Cuijpers2014). However, psychotherapies are probably less effective in chronic depression and dysthymia, when patients have comorbid alcohol problems, and in subthreshold depression, although in these patients therapies can prevent the onset of full-blown major depressive disorders (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, Koole, van Dijke, Roca, Li and Reynolds2014).
The two Editorials in this issue give interesting perspectives on where we are with research on these therapies and what should happen in the future (Hollon, Reference Hollon2015; Solomonov & Barber, Reference Solomonov and Barber2015). The first thing that stands out is that both agree that although psychotherapies are effective, there is still considerable room for improvement. The Editorials point at the problem that the effects of psychotherapy have been overestimated because of publication bias, but also that the longer term effects of psychotherapies are not well known, except maybe for cognitive-behavioural therapy where longer-term effects have been shown (Karyotaki et al. Reference Karyotaki, Smit, Cuijpers, Debauche, De Keyser, Habraken, Pitchot, Raes, Salomez, Gillain, Fairon, Paulus, Robays and Holdt Henningsen2014) in several trials. The small effects of psychotherapies and treatments in general in chronic and treatment-resistant depression (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, van Straten, Schuurmans, van Oppen, Hollon and Andersson2010d ) and the high relapse rates after successful recovery (Vittengl et al. Reference Vittengl, Clark, Dunn and Jarrett2007) are also important problems.
I want to underscore that although therapies are effective, there is still much room for improvement. The effect sizes found for treatments for mental disorders do not differ very much from those in general medical disorders (Leucht et al. Reference Leucht, Hierl, Kissling, Dold and Davis2012), but still it is estimated that current treatments cannot take away more than one-third of the disease burden of depression, and then only in optimal conditions (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Issakidis, Sanderson, Corry and Lapsley2004). And then there is the problem that more than 40% of the patients do not or only partially respond to treatment and less than one-third of the patients are completely recovered after treatment (Hollon et al. Reference Hollon, Muñoz, Barlow, Beardslee, Bell, Bernal, Clarke, Franciosi, Kazdin, Kohn, Linehan, Markowitz, Miklowitz, Persons, Niederehe and Sommers2002). And after successful response the relapse rates are estimated to be 50% after 2 years and up to 80% after 5 years (Vittengl et al. Reference Vittengl, Clark, Dunn and Jarrett2007). These problems are in addition to the problem that much of the earlier research has considerably overestimated the effects of acute treatments of depression, not only because of publication bias, but also because of the low quality of many trials in the field (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, van Straten, Bohlmeijer, Hollon and Andersson2010c ) and probably researcher allegiance (Munder et al. Reference Munder, Brütsch, Leonhart, Gerger and Barth2013).
Both articles give indications for how future research can contribute to a further reduction of the disease burden. In addition to that, however, I would like to stress that there is also much research that is not needed anymore in the future. In the broader biomedical field it is estimated that about 85% of research is wasted, because of irrelevant questions, inappropriate designs and methods, and biased reporting (Chalmers & Glasziou, Reference Chalmers and Glasziou2009). There is no reason to assume that this is very different in randomised trials of psychotherapies for depression. We found earlier that of 115 trials in this field only 11 met all nine generally accepted quality criteria, and that the effects of these 11 studies were considerably smaller than those of lower-quality studies (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, van Straten, Bohlmeijer, Hollon and Andersson2010c ). We also found that adjusting for unpublished trials reduces the effect size of psychotherapies with more than one-third (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, Smit, Bohlmeijer, Hollon and Andersson2010b ), and reporting in these trials also is often biased (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Cuijpers, Horder, Koole and Munafò2014).
In addition to that, a considerable number of studies report on the effects of newly developed psychotherapies for depression (e.g., Bédard et al. Reference Bédard, Felteau, Marshall, Cullen, Gibbons, Dubois, Maxwell, Mazmanian, Weaver, Rees, Gainer, Klein and Moustgaard2014; Giosan et al. Reference Giosan, Cobeanu, Mogoase, Muresan, Malta, Wyka and Szentagotai2014) and on comparative trials comparing two types of psychotherapies. It is well-established, however, that all therapies for depression are about equally effective (Barth et al. Reference Barth, Munder, Gerger, Nüesch, Trelle, Znoj, Jüni and Cuijpers2013) and that if a new therapy would indeed be more effective than an existing therapy, it would take a new trial with about 800 patients to show its superiority (Cuijpers & van Straten, Reference Cuijpers and van Straten2011). If a therapy is found to be superior to an existing therapy in an underpowered trial that would rather raise doubts about the validity of the trial than trust that this new therapy is indeed more effective.
Other trials have focused on examining psychotherapies for depression in specific target populations, like older adults, women with postpartum depression and patients with comorbid general medical disorders. However, meta-analyses consistently show that psychotherapies are effective in all such target groups (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, Van Straten, Warmerdam and Smits2008). By far the majority of trials examining psychotherapies for depression find positive effects, whether these are in specific target groups or in unselected populations of adults. It seems therefore not necessary to spend resources on examining these therapies in all such specific target groups.
In the same way it can be argued that it is no longer needed to examine different treatment formats in randomised controlled, because all research consistently suggests that individual, group, guided self-help and guided Internet-based therapies formats result in comparable outcomes (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, Donker, van Straten, Li and Andersson2010a ; Andersson et al. Reference Andersson, Cuijpers, Carlbring, Riper and Hedman2014).
In order to reduce the disease burden of depression much research is needed in many areas. The two articles give several directions for future directions in research on psychological treatments, including research how to reduce drop-out rates for psychotherapies, because they use considerable resources with apparently little benefits (Solomonov & Barber, Reference Solomonov and Barber2015). But also more research on the long-term effects of therapies, treatment-resistant and chronic depression, the high relapse rates and more knowledge on who benefits from which treatment are important goals for future research, as justly indicated in these two articles in this issue.
In this context it could be added that it is also important to examine how treatments work, because if we understand the processes of how therapies work we may finally start improving the effects of therapies. However, examining working mechanisms of treatments is notoriously complicated (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007), and requires different types of research, including not only research on mediators in randomised trials, but also experimental studies, theoretical work and animal studies. Although this will require quite some resources, it will be one of the few ways how we can improve the effects of treatments. It has been shown that all pharmacological therapies are about equally effective, that all psychological treatments are equally effective, and that pharmacological and psychological treatments are also equally effective. That means that all treatments we have are about equally effective, while we hardly know anything about who benefits from which treatment. This basically implies that the effects of treatments have not been improved since the 1950s when the tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) were first tested.
The other approach to a further reduction of the disease burden of depression is to focus on improvement of how to apply treatments better in routine care and how to scale treatment up. Although the effects of treatments may not have improved since their first development, mental health care has made considerable progress in terms of reaching depressed patients and applying therapies to people who can benefit from therapies. That is important because large group of patients who may benefit from treatment still do not use these services enough, like adolescents, older adults, minority and lower socioeconomic groups. And in low- and middle income countries treatments are hardly available at all. It is important therefore to simplify therapies and make them more accessible.
The suggestion by Solomonov and Barber to develop stepwise treatment models offers therefore an interesting perspective, with the simplest and cheapest treatments first, followed by more complicated and intensive therapies. That is in line with more generic models that are aimed at improving existing treatments and making them more efficient, like stepped care (van Straten et al. Reference van Straten, Hill, Richards and Cuijpers2015) and collaborative care models (Coventry et al. Reference Coventry, Hudson, Kontopantelis, Archer, Richards, Gilbody, Lovell, Dickens, Gask, Waheed and Bower2014). But there are also other ways to improve access and simplify treatments without reducing their effects, like using trained lay health counsellors for the delivery of treatments in low-resourced countries, as has been done in India (Patel et al. Reference Patel, Weiss, Chowdhary, Naik, Pednekar, Chatterjee, De Silva, Bhat, Araya, King, Simon, Verdeli and Kirkwood2010), or using Internet-based guided self-help therapies which cost much less time than traditional, individual therapies (Andersson & Cuijpers, Reference Andersson and Cuijpers2009).
Psychotherapies and other treatments for adult depression are effective, but there is also much room for improvement. The two Editorials in this issue give a nice overview of where we are and where we should be heading. We need much research, on the long-term effects, on treatments of chronic and treatment-resistant depression, on relapse, and on working mechanisms. But there is also much research which we no longer need because it has been examined sufficiently, like new therapies that promise superior effects over existing therapies while they always show equal effects. Resources are scarce and using them for unnecessary research will slow down progress and let patients with depression suffer more than needed.
Acknowledgement
None.
Financial Support
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
Conflict of Interest
None.