Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T16:56:47.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The influence of physical activity on appetite control: an experimental system to understand the relationship between exercise-induced energy expenditure and energy intake

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2011

Phillipa Caudwell
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
Catherine Gibbons
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
Mark Hopkins
Affiliation:
Sport, Health, Leisure and Nutrition, Leeds Trinity University College, Brownberrie Lane, Horsforth, Leeds LS18 5HD, UK
Erik Naslund
Affiliation:
Clinical Sciences (Division of Surgery), Danderyd Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden
Neil King
Affiliation:
Human Movement Studies & Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia
Graham Finlayson
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
John Blundell*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Professor John Blundell, fax +44 113 343 6674, email j.e.blundell@leeds.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Investigations of the impact of physical activity on appetite control have the potential to throw light on the understanding of energy balance and therefore, upon body weight regulation and the development of obesity. Given the complexity of the landscape influencing weight regulation, research strategies should reflect this complexity. We have developed a research approach based on the concept of the psychobiological system (multi-level measurement and analysis) and an experimental platform that respects the operations of an adaptive regulating biological system. It is important that both sides of the energy balance equation (activity and diet) receive similar detailed levels of analysis. The experimental platform uses realistic and fully supervised levels of physical activity, medium-term (not acute) interventions, measurement of body composition, energy metabolism (indirect calorimetry), satiety physiology (gut peptides), homeostatic and hedonic processes of appetite control, non-exercise activity, obese adult participants and both genders. This research approach has shown that the impact of physical activity on appetite control is characterised by large individual differences. Changes in body composition, waist circumference and health benefits are more meaningful than changes in weight. Further, we are realising that the acute effects do not predict what will happen in the longer term. The psychobiological systems approach offers a strategy for simultaneously investigating biological and behavioural processes relevant to understanding obese people and how obesity can be managed. This experimental platform provides opportunities for industry to examine the impact of foods under scientifically controlled conditions relevant to the real world.

Type
Symposium on ‘Nutrition: getting the balance right in 2010’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011

Abbreviations:
EB

energy balance

EE

energy expenditure

EI

energy intake

FM

fat mass

FFM

fat-free mass

NR

non-responders

PA

physical activity

RMR

resting metabolic rate

Background

For several years both research and theorising in the area of energy balance (EB) have been dominated by the issue of obesity. The so-called obesity epidemic has influenced funding for research as well as strategic health objectives set by the various governments. Underlying much thinking is the general view that obesity (weight gain) is a condition arising from a mismatch between energy expenditure (EE) and energy consumed. However, the neat simplicity of this statement takes on a different meaning when seen against the complexity of the Foresight Obesity Map. Despite the complicated nature of the interacting determinants that lead to weight gain, the prevailing concept of management is to exert control over dietary intake and physical activity (PA) (eat less, move more, for example). This is the case even though the consensus view seems to be that over consumption rather than under activity has been the major force leading to weight gain and the crisis in obesity(Reference Swinburn, Sacks and Ravussin1Reference Egger, Vogels and Westerterp3). It is important to keep in mind when considering this dual-approach that the PA contribution to total EE is approximately 20% to 30% behaviour (resting metabolic rate (RMR) and dietary induced thermogenesis, together make the largest contribution to total EE), but this varies somewhat according to the amount of PA (exercise plus non-exercise activity) achieved. On the other hand, dietary intake is 100% behaviour. This behaviour is called eating. Consequently, behaviour makes a huge contribution to the EB equation (EE v. energy intake (EI)). It is also worth noting that although PA and EI are two contributors to EB, they could also interact with each other to influence EB indirectly.

It therefore appears that exerting control over behaviour (dietary intake and PA) is the key to managing obesity. However, the behaviours in question are not isolated acts but are expressed as integrated sequences often maintained through a process of learning and referred to as habits. It is known that habitual forms of behaviour are extremely resistant to change(Reference Blundell4). The task of persuading people to change their rigid patterns of behaviour invokes the issues of compliance and ambivalence. Compliance is often far from perfect; people want to change their behaviour (diet or PA), they have an intention to change, but they fail to behave in accordance with their intentions. Therefore, from a public health perspective achieving compliance is the major issue.

However, even if people comply fully with a requirement to increase PA, the theoretical reduction in body weight might not be achieved(Reference King, Hopkins and Caudwell5, Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6). This is because the ‘system’ that embodies the processes of EB is an adaptive regulatory biological system. However, the EB system should not be conceived as a simple mechanical balance that automatically responds to additions or subtractions of energy. The system shows autoregulatory adjustments that occur whenever the dietary input or the level of PA is markedly shifted(Reference Stubbs, Hughes and Johnstone7, Reference Stubbs, Hughes and Johnstone8). In the case of PA this raises the issue of compensation. There is an emerging interest in the various compensatory responses to PA which might partly explain the lower than expected weight loss(Reference Boutcher and Dunn9, Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins10). There is strong evidence to suggest that food intake is more sensitive to manipulations in EI than EE(Reference Hubert, King and Blundell11, Reference Borer, Wuorinen and Ku12). One view of PA is that it is futile for weight loss since it automatically leads to an increase in hunger and food intake that compensates for the energy expended. For example, over 50 years ago it was asserted that ‘ … the regulation of food intake functions with such flexibility that an increase in energy output due to exercise is automatically followed by an equivalent increase in caloric intake’(Reference Mayer, Marshall and Vitale13). This concept was given support by the findings on workers in the Bengal jute mills(Reference Mayer, Roy and Mitra14). In addition, Edholm held a similar view, arguing that ‘the mechanisms that relate intake to expenditure … are what regulates appetite in fact’(Reference Edholm, Fletcher and Widdowson15). This issue of compensation formed the basis for negative articles in the media that appeared during 2009(16, Reference Telegraph17) that have been discussed elsewhere(Reference Blundell18).

The attention given to compensation is probably appropriate since it can clearly be related to the operation of an actively regulating physiological system (as noted earlier) and is also prominent in the minds of people who undertake exercise for weight control (and possibly in the minds of those who do not). The idea that PA, EE and appetite control are intimately linked leads to the belief that exercise drives up food intake. It is this issue that has been at the focus of a research programme in Leeds for 18 years. The research concept is simple: apply and supervise PA, and measure EI. However, there are many experimental modalities for achieving this, and many forms that the research could undertake. The research has been designed with two considerations: first, to throw light on the processes believed to play a role in EB; and second, to achieve a level of validity that makes the outcome relevant to the management of appetite (and obesity) in the current (unhelpful) environment. Obtaining a realistic assessment of appetite is the key. Since there are many forms that the measurement of appetite can take, it is worth justifying the approach adopted here.

Note on appetite methodology

How appetite is measured under experimental circumstances depends upon the ideological and theoretical stance of the researcher, the resources available and the effort the researcher is prepared to invest to achieve validity. Of course not all research has the goal of contributing to understanding body weight or the control of obesity, but much research is justified by positioning appetite in relation to obesity, dieting and the obesogenic environment. Since a large component of appetite is a form of behaviour, the measurement of food intake suffers from most of the methodological contamination that has plagued measures of psychological and behavioural variables for well over 50 years. Unfortunately, current research is still subject to the limitations and shortcomings of early studies of behaviour and cognitions (thoughts in the head). Some of the weaknesses are as follows.

Use of undergraduate students (often from psychology departments)

Well over 50 years ago it was noted that ‘The existing science of human behaviour is largely the science of the behaviour of sophomores’(Reference McNemar19) with the implication that such participants severely restricted the usefulness of the data collected. Today between 10% and 20% of published studies on aspects of appetite are carried out on undergraduate students, many of whom are obliged to participate in experiments as a course requirement. This practice renders the research purely academic. As was noted 45 years ago ‘The widespread practice of requiring students in various psychology courses to participate in a certain number of hours worth of experiments may in some cases permit the generalisation of research findings, at least to psychology students enrolled in certain courses. In many cases, however, even this generalisation may be unwarranted’ (Rosenthal(Reference Rosenthal20)). This view is reflected in the attitude of certain food companies which, when commissioning research in universities, stipulate that studies should not be carried out on students since the results are not relevant to the majority of consumers and purchasers of food products. The motivation for researchers to recruit undergraduate students seems to be the ease and convenience of getting data which overcomes the effort and trouble required to recruit more meaningful participants. This strategy clearly minimises the relevance of research for understanding social behaviour and motivation in the obesogenic environment(Reference Levitt and List21). It should be noted also that the European Food Safety Authority in assessing claims about foods, requires that studies have been conducted on the relevant population.

Experimental context

Most studies using students are also characterised by two other features: extremely short duration (often only a few hours) consistent with the needs of the students, and measurement inside specific types of laboratories. Such confines (spatial and temporal) place limitations on the value of the data collected. Often the measures may reflect thoughts (cognitions) or behaviour occurring over the course of a few hours (following the experimenter's instructions) and are contained by the stimulus-free environment. The spatial dimension is important since much of human eating is carried out in a turbulent and invasive obesogenic environment that contains a plethora of stimuli competing for control over the purchase, selection and, finally, consumption of foods. The short time periods covered by many experimental manipulations (usually <1 d and sometimes <1 h) allow only a brief extract of some parameter that is heavily dependent on the immediate contextual state. Given that participants (particularly psychology students) carry with them into the experimental situation their own thoughts, suppositions and beliefs about the meaning of the experiment (what has been called ‘Demand Characteristics’(Reference Orne22)), the data collected are likely to be restricted in their meaning to the time and place where they were collected. The laboratory is effectively insulated (by design) from the real environment that influences eating and such data may have little to offer about the operations of thoughts and actions likely to occur in the ‘real world’. Of course, laboratory studies are likely to be strong on internal validity (but very poor on external validity) and therefore, give information about specific determinants of the behaviour measured (but of course, only under artificial circumstances).

Experimental contrivance and deception

In order to investigate certain phenomena (often with participants and under circumstances similar to those described earlier) some researchers feel the need to employ deception to dislodge certain postulated motivational determinants of consumption. The deception often carried out in psychological studies (though discouraged by the Ethical Guidelines of the British Psychological Society) sometimes takes the form of deliberately misleading labels about the energy value of foods or beverages, or may employ devices such as the automatic refillable soup bowl. This contrivance or trickery may certainly fool participants into a form of counterintuitive behaviour (data will be collected) but may be the result of attempts to resolve ambiguous and incompatible demands. The interpretation of such outcomes is far from straightforward. While the outcome is usually concluded to reveal the operation of some powerful belief or thought process (cognition) on behaviour, it is only remotely connected to real-life eating under realistic natural circumstances. Also, as noted earlier by Rosenthal, the research findings may only be generalisable to other subjects drinking from refillable soup bowls, or eating soup with a deceptive label.

These notes on appetite methodology have been set down to illustrate the shortcomings of an approach to the measurement of behaviour that were apparent to methodologists over half a century ago and are still being perpetrated in many studies today. While the limitations are not confined to eating behaviour, the combination of extreme brevity, undergraduate ‘volunteers’, sanitised environments and weird equipment serves to isolate the outcomes from contact with happenings in the natural environment under which the behaviour is naturally expressed. Such studies therefore have very little to contribute to the understanding of obesity and weight regulation. Although they could of course serve as pilot studies and throw light on the mechanistic explanations under the specific limited circumstances of the experimental situation. It also follows that appetite studies alone, in the absence of any measure of energy expenditure (metabolic or behavioural), have limited capacity to make predictions about effects on body weight or obesity.

These methodological notes were also included in response to requests from nutritionists who are often baffled about the conceptual underpinnings of some appetite research and its contribution to understanding nutritional science and the natural eating patterns which most practicing nutritionists confront in their work. The relationship between nutrition and appetite is a sensitive and complicated area, but often made more difficult to understand by the strong rhetoric often attached to psychological studies on appetite. The view of the present authors is that, just as the European Food Safety Authority has stipulated that claims about foods should not go beyond the scientific evidence(Reference Blundell23), so claims about appetite control should not go beyond the confines of the experimental circumstances.

Experimental platform to investigate physical activity and appetite control

The current methodological platform used in the University of Leeds has been developed from a series of studies started in 1992, and conducted in collaboration with James Stubbs and Stephen Whybrow of the Rowett Institute of Nutrition and Health at the University of Aberdeen. The approach is based on a number of conceptual principles and is designed to provide a comprehensive and flexible model for the study of energy expenditure and intake.

Energy balance framework

Understanding the contribution of appetite to weight regulation and obesity can only be fully understood within the framework of EB. This is necessarily so because small changes in food intake occurring during a single meal or over a period of only a few hours may not exert any effect on daily EI and may therefore, not affect EB. Moreover, induced changes in EI via modulation of energy density, which do adjust EB (Reference Stubbs, Hughes and Johnstone8), also induce adaptive compensatory adjustments to the regulatory system which oppose the direction of change. The net effect on EB, and therefore on body weight (or body composition) is not simply an effect of the imposed rise in EI but a consequence of EI and EE. This approach has also been promoted by other researchers(Reference Ravussin, Tschop and Morales24Reference Chin-Chance, Polonsky and Schoeller27). Therefore, we do not claim that our approach is unique, but it is unusual for appetite researchers. It follows that the approach can only succeed if equal attention is given to the accuracy and precision of measurements of both EE and EI.

Psychobiological system approach

For many years it has been argued that human appetite is the expression of a system(Reference Blundell, Rogers and Hill28, Reference Blundell29); an approach that was reflected in the construction of the Obesity Systems Map(30). In part this represents what Richter termed the behavioural regulation of homeostasis(Reference Richter31). Since eating clearly reflects demands for energy and nutrients to maintain the integrity and functioning of the bodily system, it is necessarily the case that a major drive for food arises from activity in tissue metabolism and energy utilisation. Such activity is likely to be functionally related to a signal arising from body composition and/or basal metabolism. Moreover, energy expended in PA (reflected in the PA Level as a multiple of RMR) will drive EI (with implications for the impact of volitional PA). It follows that an understanding of appetite can only take place against a background of measures of body composition (at least fat mass (FM) and fat-free mass (FFM)) and of RMR (from indirect calorimetry), since these elements set a minimal level for EI and therefore, exert control over appetite. Such control could be exerted via meal frequency or meal size, or via food selection (directional influence on taste and energy density of selected foods).

Another level of the psychobiological system is the pattern of episodes of eating and the control of this pattern by the Satiety Cascade(Reference Blundell, Rogers and Hill32). Acting jointly, the processes of satiation and satiety are considered to control the pattern of eating. This level of the system requires that the act of eating is measured in a transparent and realistic manner, with the experimental methodology keeping close to forms of natural eating (subject to the need for scientific precision in measurement). The close relationship between the intensity and duration of satiety and the management of ingested food by the gastrointestinal tract requires the simultaneous measurement of key aspects of intestinal physiology. The prominent role of gastro-intestinal peptides in episodic food intake(Reference Morton and Schwartz33, Reference Murphy and Bloom34) means that measurement of specified key peptides (ghrelin, peptide YY, glucagon-like peptide-1 and cholecystokinin) form part of the systems approach together with tonic peptides leptin and adiponectin which are closely related to the amount of adipose tissue. A formula for this approach has been described elsewhere(Reference Blundell, Levin and King35). The Satiety Cascade also incorporates roles for subjective appetite sensations such as hunger and fullness; the four scales commonly used originate from work done three decades ago(Reference Blundell36). In recent years the measurement of these sensations has been subjected to technological improvement including hand-held computers(Reference Delargy, Lawton and Smith37) and Personal Digital Assistants(Reference King, Lawton and Delargy38Reference Whybrow, Stephen and Stubbs40). A further level of analysis in a systems approach includes enduring dispositions or traits (measured by validated questionnaires) that have been shown to be related to obesity itself or to the tendency to gain weight. Key traits or factors arise from the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire and include restraint, disinhibition and hunger(Reference Stunkard and Messick41). Disinhibition has been shown to be related to weight gain(Reference Hays, Bathalon and McCrory42) and has been described as reflecting opportunistic eating(Reference Bryant, King and Blundell43), although disinhibition also interacts with restraint(Reference Yeomans and Coughlan44, Reference Bryant, Kiezebrink and King45). Other dispositions can be measured with the Binge Eating Scale(Reference Gormally, Black and Daston46); the Power of Food Scale(Reference Lowe, Butryn and Didie47) and the Craving Inventory(Reference Hill, Weaver and Blundell48).

The psychobiological systems approach provides a comprehensive formula for investigating appetite control and EB. The joint assessment of body composition, metabolic factors, gastro-intestinal peptides, patterns of eating, appetite sensations and enduring traits allows the emergence of associations between different domains of knowledge which normally belong to separate disciplines. Within the context of an interaction between PA and appetite, this approach allows for the investigation of the impact of metabolism on behaviour and for the effect of behaviour on metabolism.

Homeostasis and Hedonics: combined influence on appetite

For over 50 years the major influence on the study of food intake arose from the tradition of homeostasis and the concepts of Claude Bernard(Reference Bernard49). This tradition was expressed through early theories of motivation(Reference Morgan and Stellar50). More recently this ideology has been referred to by the term ‘energy homeostasis’(Reference Woods and Ramsay51). Although there always has been a movement postulating the influence of sensory factors on food intake (mainly food selection) it is within the last decade that it has been strongly recognised that overconsumption in the obesogenic environment is probably driven by hedonic aspects of food rather than by homeostatic principles of biology(Reference Lowe and Butryn52). It has also focused attention on the hedonic aspects of eating and the influence of reward sensitivity(Reference Davis, Levitan and Kaplan53), brain reward systems(Reference Berridge and Kringelbach54) and the functional properties of liking and wanting(Reference Finlayson, King and Blundell55, Reference Finlayson, King and Blundell56). Given the importance attached to the role of food palatability in overconsumption and the power of hedonic processes, it is important to incorporate this aspect of eating into the systems approach. It then becomes possible to assess the impact of PA on both the homeostatic and hedonic aspects of appetite under identical conditions.

Contextual aspects: participants, time period, nutrients

Given the criticism of appetite methodology in an earlier section, it is important that an experimental platform for investigations related to obesity incorporates plausible contextual features. Therefore, an important aspect of the platform is the use of overweight and obese adults drawn from the community. This gives the results greater relevance and applicability (i.e. external validity) to those individuals impacted by the obesogenic environment.

The time period for study should also be realistic in relation to the operations of adaptive metabolism and physiology. Experiments that last only a few hours can only assess transient changes which may have no long-term consequences. We have therefore, designed the platform around a 12-week intervention in which participants engage several days each week with research staff. Over this period, all variables forming part of the psychobiological systems approach are measured periodically. The longitudinal approach allows the emergence of adaptive changes that can be observed in various multi-dimensional layers of the system. An extended time period means that acute effects can be compared with effects emerging over the longer term. This longitudinal approach also allows the investigation of mediators and modulators on the outcome variables; that is, those factors that predict changes and those which are associated with the process of change(Reference Teixeira, Silva and Coutinho57Reference Palmeira, Teixeira and Branco59).

It is also important that research designed to have a high degree of relevance should reflect certain features of the obesogenic environment, and especially the high-risk factors. This requirement creates a dilemma since comprehensive measurement of the obesogenic landscape is not compatible with accurate scientific measurement. The issue has been partially resolved by recognising energy density as a major (nutritional) aspect of the obesogenic environment and a key risk factor for overconsumption(Reference Ledikwe, Blanck and Kettel Khan60, Reference Ello-Martin, Roe and Ledikwe61). The platform therefore incorporates exposure to high-fat and low-fat probe days to assess the phenomenon of passive overconsumption(Reference Lawton, Burley and Wales62, Reference Blundell and Macdiarmid63) and to assess the impact of this on EB under the influence of PA.

The nature of the experimental platform has been described at some length since it is proposed that this framework provides a plausible model for appetite control (within the context of a psychobiological system and with respect to EB). The platform also respects the complexity of the system (biological, psychological and environmental) that influences weight regulation and the obesogenic nature of the system in which obese people (and the non-obese) are obliged to manage their lives.

Procedural aspects of the platform

Several cohorts of participants have now completed investigations using the platform. The major focus of research has been to evaluate the impact of PA on appetite control, and the consequences of this for body weight. The core procedure involves mandatory supervised sessions of PA of a sufficient frequency (five per week), volume (60 min at 70% VO2max) and period (12 weeks). The platform is therefore not used as a measure of compliance, but is used to assess the effect of PA that has definitely been performed. The details of the procedure have been described in several publications(Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6, Reference King, Hopkins and Caudwell64, Reference Caudwell, Hopkins and King65) and will not be described here. However, it is useful to provide a synopsis of certain conclusions that can be drawn from using the extensive experimental platform described earlier.

Outcomes

Individual variability

Figure 1 shows the spectrum of individual changes in body weight and FM that occurs during 12 weeks of supervised exercise. This degree of variability has been observed in three previous cohorts using overweight and obese adult participants(Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6, Reference King, Hopkins and Caudwell64, Reference Hopkins, Caudwell and King66). It is clear that the imposed PA exerted has markedly differing effects in different participants. Indeed this heterogeneity is one of the most striking outcomes and demonstrates underlying differences in the way that the biological system responds to the physiological challenge imposed by the exercise sessions. This degree of variability has been observed 20 years ago in response to both an energy surfeit (imposed by overfeeding)(Reference Deriaz, Fournier and Tremblay67) and an energy deficit (imposed by exercise) in groups of twins(Reference Bouchard, Tremblay and Despres68); however, the phenomenon of individual variability has not been exploited.

Fig. 1. Individual changes in body weight (BW) and fat mass (FM) following 12 weeks of supervised aerobic exercise in a cohort of fifty-eight overweight and obese individuals (data from King et al. (Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6)).

Since then it has been emphasised that individual variability in response to nutrition(Reference Williams, Ordovas and Lairon69) and to exercise(Reference Teran-Garcia, Rankinen and Bouchard70) is a major outcome variable of experimental interventions. Clearly it can be concluded that when individuals take up PA (of this type, intensity and duration) many different outcomes are possible.

Responders and non-responders

The spectrum of body weight and body composition changes clearly indicate the extremes of the response to an extended PA intervention. Because the cohorts always display such heterogeneity, we have developed an analysis based on the amount of body composition change relative to the amount of exercise-induced EE which in turn allows us to identify responders (R) and non-responders (NR). In essence, R experience changes in FM equal to or more than the expected amount, whereas the NR lose less FM than is expected. The NR show a much greater degree of food intake compensation and increase their daily EI by approximately 1 MJ(Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6). This means that at the end of the 12-week programme the difference in the daily EI between the NR and R is approximately 1 MJ/d. This difference in EI is sufficient to account for the changes in body composition observed between the R and NR. These distinct categories of response to exercise suggest the existence of specific phenotypes which are susceptible or resistant to weight loss.

Does exercise induce compensation?

The observation (Fig. 1) that over 50% of participants failed to lose the amount of weight that should have ensued from the amount of energy expended over 12 weeks, indicates that some form of compensatory process is operating(Reference King, Hopkins and Caudwell64). Such compensation, to offset the anticipated weight loss, could arise due to increased hunger and food intake, decreased PA (non-exercise activity) in the non-exercising parts of the day, or metabolic adjustment through RMR. All of these were measured objectively throughout the study. Neither non-exercise activity nor RMR significantly changed, whereas there was an overall significant increase in measured hunger (fasting level and area under the curve) and in daily EI(Reference King, Hopkins and Caudwell71). This indicates that, in general, when people undertake this level of PA, the system does induce a compensatory response; this is primarily via food intake and it is very variable; some people compensate strongly whereas others do not. However, there may also be some diminution in the level of non-exercise activity(Reference Stubbs, Sepp and Hughes72).

Surprisingly, about 15% of participants gained weight over the 12-week intervention, despite the fact that they had actually performed the exercise prescribed. The characteristics of the weight gainers are shown in Table 1. Noticeable is the fact that most of these participants still lost body fat and the weight gained comprised FFM (probably lean tissue). This effect, which has been demonstrated in every cohort that has been studied using this experimental platform, indicates that for some people exercise is an anabolic experience which drives the build up of tissue. The weight gainers also show large increases in food intake in response to exercise.

Table 1. Anthropometric variables (mean values and standard deviations) in those participants who gained weight, measured at four time periods across the 12-week exercise intervention (data from King et al. (Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6))

Body composition and body weight

The effect of exercise on the body composition of the weight gainers represents the strongest effect of exercise on lean tissue. However, considering the entire cohort, it is clear that the overall mean reduction in body weight (approximately 3·3 kg) is accounted for entirely by a reduction in FM while lean mass remains unchanged (Table 2). Consequently, in contrast to weight lost by dietary restriction (which normally reduces both FM and FFM in a proportion of 70:30) exercise preserves lean tissue. This is an important consideration particularly when overall body weight change may appear trivial. Recent evidence to support this comes from a study which manipulated PA by imposing inactivity in the form of being confined to a bed for 60 d(Reference Bergouignan, Momken and Schoeller73). The negligible effect 60 d of imposed inactivity had on body weight (0·4 kg) was due to maintenance of FM but attrition of FFM. In PA interventions however, the reverse usually occurs; that is maintenance of FFM and reduction in FM. The loss of FM and the preservation of FFM, strongly implies that body composition is a far more useful measure of the efficacy of PA interventions than gross change in body weight. This implication is enhanced when it is recognised that exercise also leads to a significant reduction in waist circumference (see Table 2). A reduction in waist circumference is an indicator of a lowering of the risk for heart disease(Reference Janiszewski, Janssen and Ross74Reference Ross and Janiszewski76) and is a more relevant marker of health than body weight(Reference King, Hopkins and Caudwell71).

Table 2. Anthropometric measures (mean values and standard deviations) at weeks 0, 4, 8 and 12 for a complete cohort including those participants who lost, maintained or gained weight (n 58) (data from King et al. (Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6))

Dual-process of appetite control

One major objective for the development of this experimental platform is to allow investigation of mechanisms of appetite control. The physiological impetus created by a significant level of EE can be used to drive changes in appetite processes occurring over the 12-week period. The methodology for the intense probe days comprised a combination of self-selected, self-determined meals together with meals of fixed size accompanied by ratings of hunger and other sensations throughout the whole day. With this arrangement it is possible to measure the strength of an orexigenic drive (fasting hunger together with overall volume (area under the curve) of hunger), the effect on satiation (meal size) and the effect on satiety (post-prandial action measured by the satiety quotient(Reference Green, Delargy and Joanes77Reference Chapman, Parker and Doran79). Together these measures indicate a dissociation of processes underlying the expression of appetite. Exercise induces an increase in fasting hunger, but this is significantly greater (and enduring) in NR than R. Hunger is not driven up equally in all participants. Exercise also improves post-prandial satiety reflected by changes in the satiety quotient, and this finding is consistent with previous studies(Reference Long, Hart and Morgan80, Reference Martins, Morgan and Bloom81). This effect seems to reflect an up-regulation in the sensitivity of the satiety signalling mechanisms induced by the physiological stimulus of exercise; it occurs in R and NR. The experimental platform has therefore revealed two processes underlying appetite control; the overall orexigenic drive and post-prandial satiety(Reference King, Caudwell and Hopkins6). Taken together these effects determine the degree to which an individual will increase food intake in response to exercise which, in turn, contributes to the degree of compensation displayed.

Acute v. long-term outcomes

The effects derived from the experimental platform described here (12 week formula within an EB framework) can be compared with a long series of experiments of various durations ranging from 1 d to 19 d which we have conducted over the last 15 years. These studies have shown that a single acute exercise session has little effect on subsequent food intake(Reference Hubert, King and Blundell11, Reference King, Burley and Blundell82Reference King, Snell and Smith84) and no consequences are observed even in response to a significant amount of high-intensity exercise(Reference King, Lluch and Stubbs85). These outcomes are consistent with conclusions drawn by Edholm(Reference Edholm, Fletcher and Widdowson15) in his classic EB studies, and suggested a loose coupling between EE and EI(Reference Blundell and King86). However, when our exercise intervention was extended to 7 d in participants observed in a residential unit, a degree of food intake compensation began to occur, but more strongly in women(Reference Stubbs, Sepp and Hughes87) than in men(Reference Stubbs, Sepp and Hughes72). Subsequent studies under carefully controlled conditions in a human calorimeter demonstrated that for an additional 2 MJ of exercise per day, an average compensatory response of about 0·3 MJ EI was observed after the end of 1 week(Reference Stubbs, Hughes and Johnstone7, Reference Stubbs, Hughes and Johnstone8). A subsequent study in which total EE was measured by doubly labelled water over a period of 14 d, EI began to track EE in a dose-dependent manner and reached approximately 30% of the induced additional EE after 2 weeks(Reference Whybrow, Hughes and Ritz88) but it was very variable. Taken together this series of studies indicate that the outcome of a single acute intervention is not a good predictor of what happens when exercise persists for days or weeks. This is a good example of single acute experiments (of brief duration and carried out within a single day) having little relevance for what happens in the long term.

Exercise and dietary fat intake

The obesogenic environment provides a continuous stimulation for the appetite system and is widely agreed to provoke inappropriate eating and overconsumption. Some researchers argue that the food component of the obesogenic environment is responsible for the obesity epidemic(Reference Swinburn, Sacks and Ravussin89). The food environment is clearly complex and a number of routes to obesity can be envisaged. It would therefore be impossible to incorporate the full panoply of the food environment in any methodological approach; however, the experimental platform allows for systematic nutrition exposures at different times during the 12-week exercise period. Moreover, it has been argued that much overconsumption is not deliberate or intentional, but passive(Reference Blundell and Macdiarmid63). That is, the consumption occurs inexorably due to the high energy density of the diet. This type of diet (typified by high-fat foods) leads to the rapid intake of large amounts of food energy before satiety signals can operate to prevent the prolongation of eating. This leads to a phenomenon known as the ‘fat paradox’(Reference Blundell and Macdiarmid63). Because of the potency of passive overconsumption, we have currently investigated the effect on self-determined consumption of a full day's exposure to a particular high- or low-fat diet. Interestingly, in obese participants, persistent exercise reduced the intake of high-fat (but not low-fat) foods and therefore, curtailed passive overconsumption(Reference King, Caudwell and Gibbons90). This outcome demonstrates the sensitivity of the experimental platform to detect the effects of dietary manipulations, and indicates a further beneficial effect of exercise, namely, a tendency to reduce the self-selection of meals composed of fatty foods.

Exercise and food reward

Eating is a source of pleasure for most people, and reward plays an important role in the initiation, maintenance and cessation of eating. Food reward is perceived as an acute, conscious, sensation of pleasure or desire to eat but also operates at a level that can influence motivation to choose and consume food, without being readily perceived. These distinct components of reward are referred to as ‘liking’ (explicit evaluation of pleasure) and ‘wanting’ (implicit motivational forces) and are measured in our laboratory using a psychological tool(Reference Finlayson, King and Blundell56). By incorporating a measure of liking and wanting into the experimental platform it is possible to examine their role as underlying mechanisms involved in compensatory eating in the acute setting(Reference Finlayson, Bryant and Blundell91) and in resistance to weight loss in the longer term. We recently examined the effect of an acute bout of exercise on liking and wanting for foods in overweight and obese participants, before and following the 12-week exercise schedule(Reference Finlayson, Caudwell and Gibbons92). We found that those who experienced an immediate post-exercise increase in liking for food and more specifically an increased wanting for high-fat sweet foods also demonstrated the least fat loss after 12 weeks. This acute response was not attenuated by regular exercise (or change in body composition) suggesting it is a strong habitual trait. The implication is that the effect of acute exercise on food reward is more implicit when it comes to high-risk foods and that vulnerable people are largely unaware of the influences determining the degree of compensatory eating. Furthermore, the results demonstrate that the tool can be used to identify hedonically susceptible individuals which in turn may help to optimise strategies for weight control with dietary modification or pharmacotherapy.

Overview

The psychobiological systems approach has demonstrated a potential to describe relationships between domains of biology and behaviour not normally measured simultaneously; it has already disclosed novel processes that influence the behavioural output of the human system under the challenge of persistent supervised exercise. The data presented earlier have provided a sample of the published outcomes that have been generated by the platform. Other features (not described here) include the influence of enduring traits such as disinhibition and binge eating on exercise-induced weight loss(Reference Bryant93, Reference Gibbons, Caudwell and Finlayson94) and the determining effects of substrate oxidation on fat loss(Reference Hopkins, Caudwell and Gibbons95). The experimental platform is versatile and can be adapted to evaluate the influence of other interventions superimposed upon the basic formulation. Of particular importance for research and development in the food industry is the potential to assess additional dietary manipulations (e.g. high v. low protein exposures) and to evaluate the response to general types of foods (or individual food products) inserted into the nutritional environment. This feature of the experimental platform can therefore provide a useful resource for food companies to monitor the effect of novel foods on human appetite and EB under realistic conditions that are relevant for overweight and obese people struggling to manage their body weight within an unhelpful obesogenic environment.

Acknowledgements

The work described in this paper is largely based on the outcome of two research project grants funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council, BBS/B/05079 ‘Individual variability and characterisation of compensatory responses to exercise interventions’ and BB/G530141/1 ‘Drivers of eating behaviour during chronic overconsumption: Role of food hedonics (liking and wanting) and peptide biomarkers on satiation and satiety’ (DRINC Programme). The authors declare no conflicts of interest. P. C., C. G. and M. H. performed the experimental studies based on the platform, prepared the figure and tables and contributed to the manuscript. G. F., N. K. and E. N. supervised the experimental studies and contributed to the manuscript. J. B. supervised the studies, prepared the first draft of the manuscript and finalised the text.

References

1.Swinburn, B, Sacks, G & Ravussin, E (2009) Increased food energy supply is more than sufficient to explain the US epidemic of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 14531456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Westerterp, K & Speakman, J (2008) Physical activity energy expenditure has not declined since the 1980s and matches energy expenditures of wild mammals. Int J Obes 32, 12561263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3.Egger, G, Vogels, N & Westerterp, K (2001) Estimating historical changes in physical activity levels. Med J Aust 175, 635636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Blundell, JE (2000) What foods do people habitually eat? A dilemma for nutrition, an enigma for psychology. Am J Clin Nutr 71, 35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.King, N, Hopkins, M, Caudwell, P et al. (2007) Individual variability following 12 weeks of supervised exercise: identification and characterization of compensation for exercise-induced weight loss. Int J Obes 32, 177184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.King, N, Caudwell, P, Hopkins, M et al. (2009) Dual-process action of exercise on appetite control: increase in orexigenic drive but improvement in meal-induced satiety. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 921927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Stubbs, R, Hughes, D, Johnstone, A et al. (2004) A decrease in physical activity affects appetite, energy, and nutrient balance in lean men feeding ad libitum. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 6269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Stubbs, R, Hughes, D, Johnstone, A et al. (2004) Rate and extent of compensatory changes in energy intake and expenditure in response to altered exercise and diet composition in humans. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 286, R350R358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Boutcher, S & Dunn, S (2009) Factors that may impede the weight loss response to exercise-based interventions. Obes Rev 10, 671680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.King, N, Caudwell, P, Hopkins, M et al. . (2007) Automatic and volitional compensatory responses to exercise interventions: metabolic and behavioral barriers to weight loss. Obesity 15, 13731383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Hubert, P, King, N & Blundell, J (1998) Uncoupling the effects of energy expenditure and energy intake: appetite response to short-term energy deficit induced by meal omission and physical activity. Appetite 31, 9–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Borer, K, Wuorinen, E, Ku, K et al. (2009) Appetite responds to changes in meal content, whereas ghrelin, leptin, and insulin track changes in energy availability. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 94, 22902298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Mayer, J, Marshall, N, Vitale, J et al. (1954) Exercise, food intake and body weight in normal rats and genetically obese adult mice. Am J Physiol 177, 544548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Mayer, J, Roy, P & Mitra, K (1956) Relation between caloric intake, body weight, and physical work: studies in an industrial male population in West Bengal. Am J Clin Nutr 4, 169175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.Edholm, OG, Fletcher, JG, Widdowson, EM et al. (1955) The energy expenditure and food intake of individual men. Br J Nutr 9, 286300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Time Magazine (2009) Why exercise won't make you thin [cited 23-08-2010]; available at http://www.time.com/time/health/article/0,8599,1914857,00.htmlGoogle Scholar
17.Telegraph, D (2009) Health warning – Exercise makes you fat [cited 23-08-2010]; available at http://www.telegraph.co.uk/science/6083234/Health-warning-exercise-makes-you-fat.htmlGoogle Scholar
18.Blundell, J (2009) Exercise makes you fat – what's going on? Nutr Bull 34, 380382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19.McNemar, Q (1946) Opinion-attitude methodology. Psychol Bull 43, 289374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Rosenthal, R (1965) The volunteer subject. Hum Relations 18, 389406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21.Levitt, S & List, J (2007) What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? J Econ Perspect 21, 153174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22.Orne, MT (1962) On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics. Amer Psychol 17, 776783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23.Blundell, J (2010) Making claims: functional foods for managing appetite and weight. Nat Rev Endocrinol 6, 5356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Ravussin, E, Tschop, M, Morales, S et al. (2001) Plasma ghrelin concentration and energy balance: overfeeding and negative energy balance studies in twins. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 86, 45474551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Livingstone, M, Coward, W, Davies, P et al. (1992) Daily energy expenditure in free living children: comparison of heart rate monitoring with doubly labelled water method. Am J Clin Nutr 56, 343352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Schrauwen, P & Westerterp, KR (2000) The role of high fat diets and physical activity in the regulation of body weight. Br J Nutr 84, 417427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Chin-Chance, C, Polonsky, K & Schoeller, D (2000) Twenty-four-hour leptin levels respond to cumulative short-term energy imbalance and predict subsequent intake. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 85, 26852691.Google ScholarPubMed
28.Blundell, J, Rogers, P & Hill, A (1985) Behavioural structure and mechanisms of anorexia: calibration of natural and abnormal inhibition of eating. Brain Res Bull 15, 371376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Blundell, J (1991) Pharmacological approaches to appetite suppression. Trends Pharmacol Sci 12, 147157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Foresight (2007) Tackling obesities: future choices – Project report. In: Science. Government Office for Science, pp. 1153. http//www.foresight.gov.ukGoogle Scholar
31.Richter, C (1956) Loss of appetite for alcohol and alcoholic beverages produced in rats by treatment with thyroid preparations. Endocrinology 59, 472478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.Blundell, J, Rogers, P & Hill, A (1987) Evaluating the Satiating Power of Foods: Implications for Acceptance and Consumption. London: Food Acceptance and Nutrition Academic Press. p. 205219.Google Scholar
33.Morton, G & Schwartz, M (2001) The NPY/AgRP neuron and energy homeostasis. International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders. J Int Assoc Study Obes 25, S56S62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34.Murphy, KG & Bloom, SR (2006) Gut hormones and the regulation of energy homeostasis. Nature 444, 854859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Blundell, J, Levin, F, King, N et al. (2008) Overconsumption and obesity: peptides and susceptibility to weight gain. Regul Pept 149, 3238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Blundell, JE (1979) Hunger, Appetite and Satiety – Constructs in Search of Identities, pp. 2142. London: Applied Science Publishers.Google Scholar
37.Delargy, H, Lawton, C, Smith, F et al. (1996) Electronic appetite rating system (EARS): validation of continuous automated monitoring of motivation to eat. Int J Obes 20, 104.Google Scholar
38.King, N, Lawton, C, Delargy, H et al. (1997) The electronic appetite rating system (EARS): a portable computerised method of continuous automated monitoring of motivation to eat and mood. In Ingestive Behaviour Protocols, pp. 7176 [P Wellman and BG Hoebel, editors]. Illinois: Society for the Study of Ingestive Behaviour.Google Scholar
39.Stratton, R, Stubbs, R, Hughes, D et al. (1998) Comparison of the traditional paper visual analogue scale questionnaire with an Apple Newton electronic appetite rating system (EARS) in free living subjects feeding ad libitum. Eur J Clin Nutr 52, 737741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Whybrow, S, Stephen, J & Stubbs, R (2006) The evaluation of an electronic visual analogue scale system for appetite and mood. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 558560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Stunkard, A & Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 29, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Hays, N, Bathalon, G, McCrory, M et al. (2002) Eating behavior correlates of adult weight gain and obesity in healthy women aged 55–65 y. Am J Clin Nutr 75, 476483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Bryant, E, King, N & Blundell, J (2008) Disinhibition: its effects on appetite and weight regulation. Obes Rev 9, 409419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Yeomans, M & Coughlan, E (2009) Mood-induced eating. Interactive effects of restraint and tendency to overeat. Appetite 52, 290298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Bryant, E, Kiezebrink, K, King, N et al. (2010) Interaction between disinhibition and restraint: implications for body weight and eating disturbance. Eat Weight Disord 15, e43e51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Gormally, J, Black, S, Daston, S et al. (1982) The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addict Behav 7, 4755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Lowe, M, Butryn, M, Didie, E et al. . (2009) The power of food scale. A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment. Appetite 53, 114118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
48.Hill, A, Weaver, C & Blundell, J (1991) Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite 17, 187197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Bernard, M (1865) Leçons de physiologie expérimentale appliquée à la médecine faites au Collège de France.Google Scholar
50.Morgan, C & Stellar, E (1943) Physiological Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
51.Woods, S & Ramsay, D (2007) Homeostasis: beyond Curt Richter. Appetite 49, 388398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52.Lowe, M & Butryn, M (2007) Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol Behav 91, 432439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
53.Davis, C, Levitan, R, Kaplan, A et al. (2008) Reward sensitivity and the D2 dopamine receptor gene: a case-control study of binge eating disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32, 620628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Berridge, K & Kringelbach, M (2008) Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology 199, 457480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55.Finlayson, G, King, N & Blundell, J (2007) Liking vs. wanting food: importance for human appetite control and weight regulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31, 987–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56.Finlayson, G, King, N & Blundell, J (2008) The role of implicit wanting in relation to explicit liking and wanting for food: implications for appetite control. Appetite 50, 120127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57.Teixeira, P, Silva, M, Coutinho, S et al. (2010) Mediators of weight loss and weight loss maintenance in middle-aged women. Obesity 18, 725735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58.Teixeira, P, Going, S, Sardinha, L et al. (2005) A review of psychosocial pre-treatment predictors of weight control. Obes Rev 6, 4365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59.Palmeira, A, Teixeira, P, Branco, T et al. (2007) Predicting short-term weight loss using four leading health behavior change theories. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity 20, 4–14.Google Scholar
60.Ledikwe, J, Blanck, H, Kettel Khan, L et al. (2006) Dietary energy density is associated with energy intake and weight status in US adults. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 13621368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61.Ello-Martin, J, Roe, L, Ledikwe, J et al. (2007) Dietary energy density in the treatment of obesity: a year-long trial comparing 2 weight-loss diets. Am J Nutr 85, 14651477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62.Lawton, C, Burley, V, Wales, J et al. (1993) Dietary fat and appetite control in obese subjects: weak effects on satiation and satiety. Int J Obes 17, 409416.Google Scholar
63.Blundell, J & Macdiarmid, J (1997) Fat as a risk factor for overconsumption: satiation, satiety, and patterns of eating. J Am Diet Assoc 97, S63S69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64.King, N, Hopkins, M, Caudwell, P et al. (2008) Individual variability following 12 weeks of supervised exercise: identification and characterization of compensation for exercise-induced weight loss. Int J Obes 32, 177184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65.Caudwell, P, Hopkins, M, King, N et al. (2009) Exercise alone is not enough: weight loss also needs a healthy (Mediterranean) diet? Public Health Nutr 12, 16631666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66.Hopkins, M, Caudwell, P, King, N et al. (2006) Individual variability in body weight, metabolic and behavioural responses to supervised exercise in overweight and obese individuals. J Sports Sci 23, S1149S1303.Google Scholar
67.Deriaz, O, Fournier, G, Tremblay, A et al. (1992) Lean-body-mass composition and resting energy expenditure before and after long-term overfeeding. Am J Clin Nutr 56, 840847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
68.Bouchard, C, Tremblay, A, Despres, J et al. (1994) The response to exercise with constant energy intake in identical twins. Obes Res 2, 400410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69.Williams, C, Ordovas, J, Lairon, D et al. . (2008) The challenges for molecular nutrition research 1: linking genotype to healthy nutrition. Genes Nutr 3, 4149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70.Teran-Garcia, M, Rankinen, T & Bouchard, C (2008) Genes, exercise, growth, and the sedentary, obese child. J Appl Physiol 105, 988–1001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71.King, N, Hopkins, M, Caudwell, P et al. (2009) Beneficial effects of exercise: shifting the focus from body weight to other markers of health. Br J Sports Med 43, 924927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
72.Stubbs, R, Sepp, A, Hughes, D et al. . (2002) The effect of graded levels of exercise on energy intake and balance in free-living men, consuming their normal diet. Eur J Clin Nutr 56, 129140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73.Bergouignan, A, Momken, I, Schoeller, D et al. (2010) Regulation of energy balance during long-term physical inactivity induced by bed rest with and without exercise training. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 95, 10451053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74.Janiszewski, P, Janssen, I & Ross, R (2007) Does waist circumference predict diabetes and cardiovascular disease beyond commonly evaluated cardiometabolic risk factors? Diab Care 30, 31053109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
75.Janssen, I, Katzmarzyk, P & Ross, R (2004) Waist circumference and not body mass index explains obesity-related health risk. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 379384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
76.Ross, R & Janiszewski, P (2008) Is weight loss the optimal target for obesity-related cardiovascular disease risk reduction? Can J Cardiol 24, 25D31D.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77.Green, S, Delargy, H, Joanes, D et al. (1997) A satiety quotient: a formulation to assess the satiating effect of food. Appetite 29, 291304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78.Gray, R, French, S, Robinson, T et al. (2002) Dissociation of the effects of preload volume and energy content on subjective appetite and food intake. Physiol Behav 76, 5764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79.Chapman, I, Parker, B, Doran, S et al. . (2005) Effect of pramlintide on satiety and food intake in obese subjects and subjects with type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia 48, 838848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
80.Long, S, Hart, K & Morgan, L (2002) The ability of habitual exercise to influence appetite and food intake in response to high-and low-energy preloads in man. Br J Nutr 87, 517523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
81.Martins, C, Morgan, LM, Bloom, SR et al. (2007) Effects of exercise on gut peptides, energy intake and appetite. J Endocrinol 193, 251258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
82.King, N, Burley, V & Blundell, J (1994) Exercise-induced suppression of appetite: effects on food intake and implications for energy balance. Eur J Clin Nutr 48, 715724.Google ScholarPubMed
83.King, N & Blundell, J (1995) High-fat foods overcome the energy expenditure induced by high-intensity cycling or running. Eur J Clin Nutr 49, 114123.Google ScholarPubMed
84.King, N, Snell, L, Smith, R et al. (1996) Effects of short-term exercise on appetite responses in unrestrained females. Eur J Clin Nutr 50, 663667.Google ScholarPubMed
85.King, N, Lluch, A, Stubbs, R et al. (1997) High dose exercise does not increase hunger or energy intake in free living males. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, 478483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
86.Blundell, J & King, N (1998) Effects of exercise on appetite control: loose coupling between energy expenditure and energy intake. Int J Obes Suppl 22, S22S29.Google ScholarPubMed
87.Stubbs, R, Sepp, A, Hughes, D et al. . (2002) The effect of graded levels of exercise on energy intake and balance in free-living women. Int J Obes 26, 866869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
88.Whybrow, S, Hughes, D, Ritz, P et al. . (2008) The effect of an incremental increase in exercise on appetite, eating behaviour and energy balance in lean men and women feeding ad libitum. Br J Nutr 100, 11091115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
89.Swinburn, B, Sacks, G & Ravussin, E (2009) Increased food energy supply is more than sufficient to explain the US epidemic of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 14531456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
90.King, N, Caudwell, P, Gibbons, C et al. (2010) Exercise-induced reduction in passive overconsumption in females. Obes Rev 311.Google Scholar
91.Finlayson, G, Bryant, E, Blundell, JE et al. (2009) Acute compensatory eating following exercise is associated with implicit hedonic wanting for food. Physiol Behav 97, 6267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
92.Finlayson, G, Caudwell, P, Gibbons, C et al. (2010) Low fat loss response after medium-term supervised exercise in obese is associated with exercise-induced increase in food reward. J Obes 2011, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
93.Bryant, E (2009) High trait disinhibition associated with better success at exercise-induced weight loss. Obes Facts 2(S2), 211.Google Scholar
94.Gibbons, C, Caudwell, P, Finlayson, G et al. (2010) Psychometric traits predict reduction in fat mass following a supervised exercise intervention. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 205.Google Scholar
95.Hopkins, M, Caudwell, P, Gibbons, C et al. (2010) Fat oxidation during exercise is associated with medium-term exercise-induced fat loss. Obes Rev 11(51), 112.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Individual changes in body weight (BW) and fat mass (FM) following 12 weeks of supervised aerobic exercise in a cohort of fifty-eight overweight and obese individuals (data from King et al.(6)).

Figure 1

Table 1. Anthropometric variables (mean values and standard deviations) in those participants who gained weight, measured at four time periods across the 12-week exercise intervention (data from King et al.(6))

Figure 2

Table 2. Anthropometric measures (mean values and standard deviations) at weeks 0, 4, 8 and 12 for a complete cohort including those participants who lost, maintained or gained weight (n 58) (data from King et al.(6))