Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T13:44:16.977Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Influencing and modifying children's energy intake: the role of portion size and energy density

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2014

L. Kirsty Pourshahidi
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health, University of Ulster, Coleraine BT52 1SA, UK
Maeve A. Kerr
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health, University of Ulster, Coleraine BT52 1SA, UK
Tracy A. McCaffrey
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Monash University, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences, VIC 3168, Australia
M. Barbara E. Livingstone*
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health, University of Ulster, Coleraine BT52 1SA, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Professor B. Livingstone, fax +44(0)2870123023, email mbe.livingstone@ulster.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Childhood obesity is of concern worldwide. The portion size (PS) and energy density (ED) of food are two major determinants of children's energy intake (EI). Trends towards increasing PS are most apparent and best documented in the USA, where PS of numerous food products have increased in the marketplace over the past three decades, particularly high-energy dense foods. Analyses of population-level dietary surveys have confirmed this trend in children for both in- and out-of-home eating, and a plethora of observational evidence positively associates PS, ED and adiposity in children. A limited number of intervention studies provide clear evidence that children, even as young as 2 years, respond acutely to increasing PS, with some studies also demonstrating the additive effects of increased ED in promoting excessive EI. However, most of the evidence is based on children aged 3–6 years and there is a paucity of data in older children and adolescents. It is unclear whether decreasing PS can have the opposite effect on children's EI but recent acute studies have demonstrated that the incorporation of lower energy dense foods, such as fruit and vegetables, into children's meals down-regulates EI. Although a direct causal link between PS and obesity remains to be established, the regular consumption of larger PS of energy dense foods do favour obesity-promoting eating behaviours in children. Further research is required to establish the most feasible and effective interventions and policies to counteract the deleterious impact of PS and ED on children's EI.

Type
Conference on ‘Childhood nutrition and obesity: current status and future challenges’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2014 

The growing prevalence of childhood obesity over the past decades has become a major public health issue worldwide. Notwithstanding the underlying biology and/or genetic predisposition to obesity, a myriad of environmental factors are associated with the increase of obesity in children. In effect, the pace of technological change has outstripped human evolution and as a consequence, children are ill-equipped to handle the modern food environment, especially in the face of decreasing energy expenditure. Among the factors in the food environment, the contribution of increasing portion size (PS) and energy density (ED: kJ/g), to energy intake (EI) has become the focus of intense investigation.

Food PS have been steadily increasing in parallel with obesity since the 1970s( Reference Young and Nestle 1 ). The trends towards increasing PS are the most apparent and best documented in the USA, where PS of numerous food products, especially those of high ED have increased in the marketplace over the past three decades( Reference Young and Nestle 2 ). More extensive analyses of nationally representative dietary data in the USA have also confirmed this trend, not only for out-of-home eating, but also for in-home consumption by both adults( Reference Nielsen and Popkin 3 Reference Nielsen and Popkin 5 ) and children( Reference Nielsen and Popkin 5 Reference Piernas and Popkin 7 ). In comparison, there is a paucity of trend data on PS in Europe. Limited data from Denmark( Reference Matthiessen, Fagt and Biltoft-Jensen 8 ), the Netherlands( Reference Steenhuis, Leeuwis and Vermeer 9 ) and the UK( Reference Wrieden, Gregor and Barton 10 12 ) suggest that trends in PS are mirroring those observed in the USA, albeit that PS tend to be larger overall in the USA( Reference Rozin, Kabnick and Pete 13 ). Data from the UK and Ireland show that although the PS of many traditional products have generally remained constant, the range of PS has been extended for many food products, including savoury snacks, bakery products and those sold in fast food establishments( Reference Church 11 , Reference O'Brien, Lyons and McNulty 14 , Reference O'Brien, McNulty and Nugent 15 ).

Large PS, particularly of energy dense foods are highly likely to incite overeating, because they are relatively cheap for food industries to manufacture, and are therefore often seen by consumers as good value for money (e.g. multi- or share-sized packs). Exposure to large serving sizes is now routine and has effectively distorted consumers’ perceptions of what an appropriate serving size is. Moreover, the interchangeable use of the terms ‘PS’ and ‘serving size’ within existing guidance is confusing and can make it difficult for consumers to decide on an appropriate amount of food to consume( Reference Faulkner, Pourshahidi and Wallace 16 ). Throughout the current review, PS is defined as the amount of food intended to be consumed by an individual in a single-eating occasion, as opposed to ‘serving size’, which is the quantity recommended to be consumed in a single-eating occasion (e.g. by a manufacturer on a food label)( 17 ).

The availability of larger food PS of high-energy dense foods is a common characteristic of eating out of the home. Indeed, eating out-of-home is associated with increased EI and fat intakes( Reference O'Dwyer, McCarthy and Burke 18 Reference Lachat, Nago and Verstraeten 20 ), and is an independent risk factor for obesity( Reference Thompson, Ballew and Resnicow 21 Reference Burke, McCarthy and O'Neill 23 ). This is of particular concern given the increased frequency of out-of-home eating over time. For example, in 2004 approximately two-thirds of children from the island of Ireland ate out at least once per week, and food expenditure data demonstrate that this has increased in the past decade( 24 27 ). Recent research commissioned by safefood (the Food Safety Promotion Board, Ireland) has shown that although many restaurants and cafes have increased the provision of healthier options for children in recent years, there is still room for improvement in terms of the availability of PS offered on children's menus, e.g. by allowing children to order half portions from the main menu( 28 ).

The predisposition to overeat in response to large PS appears to be a ubiquitous phenomenon and occurs irrespective of age (child/adult), current weight status, sex and/or degree of dietary restraint or disinhibited eating behaviour( Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls 29 Reference Savage, Fisher and Marini 33 ). In young children food intake has been reported to occur primarily in response to hunger and satiety cues, with early laboratory evidence demonstrating an innate ability of young children to self-regulate their EI( Reference Birch and Deysher 34 Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch 38 ). This has also more recently been confirmed in the free-living environment( Reference Kral, Stunkard and Berkowitz 39 ). The age, however, at which children become susceptible to over eat in response to large PS remains unclear. There is evidence to suggest that by the age of 1–2 years, children may no longer be immune to the intake-enhancing effects of large PS( Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch 40 Reference Fisher 42 ). It should also be remembered that EI is a function of both the PS of a food/beverage, as well as its ED and the frequency of consumption, among other factors( Reference Duffey and Popkin 43 , Reference Duffey and Popkin 44 ). Analyses of cross-sectional population level data in the USA have confirmed that over the past three decades, the observed increase in EI by both children over age 2 years and adults has largely been driven by a combination of increased eating frequency and PS( Reference Piernas and Popkin 6 , Reference Duffey and Popkin 44 , Reference Slining, Mathias and Popkin 45 ).

The purpose of this review is to evaluate the role of PS and ED in influencing and modifying children's EI, and subsequent risk of overweight/obesity.

Portion size

Observational evidence

Although natural hunger-driven eating behaviours are known to regulate the EI of infants and toddlers, environmental cues such as large food PS, have been shown to have the ability to disrupt this innate self-regulation( Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch 38 , Reference Adair 46 Reference Mrdjenovic and Levitsky 49 ). In infants and young children (aged less than 11 months), Fox et al. (2006)( Reference Fox, Devaney and Reidy 41 ) reported a negative relationship between ED and average PS z-scores, suggesting that as the ED of diet increased, there was a corresponding down-regulation of food intake. In contrast, no such association was shown in toddlers (aged 12–24 months)( Reference Fox, Devaney and Reidy 41 ). Analysis of data from large-scale dietary surveys( Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch 40 , Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Mitchell 50 ) has also identified that large PS across many food groups are positively associated with obesity in young children. In UK adolescents, an increase in the PS of, and EI from snacks was also observed between 1997 and 2005, particularly for all drinks, crisps and savoury snacks and breakfast cereals( Reference Kerr, Rennie and McCaffrey 51 ). Further research has shown that the PS of meals is positively associated with BMI percentiles in boys aged 6–11 years and in children aged 12–19 years( Reference Huang, Howarth and Lin 52 ), and overall PS is consistently positively associated with both EI and body-weight in children( Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch 40 ). However, these observational data, cannot establish causality.

Intervention studies

To date nine studies have been conducted in children to assess their responsiveness to increasing PS (Table 1). The majority of these studies have been conducted in the USA, and demonstrate that doubling the PS of a macaroni and cheese entree resulted in a 10–40 % increase in EI( Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch 38 , Reference Fisher 42 , Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch 53 Reference Fisher, Arreola and Birch 55 ), whereas a 4-fold increase in entree PS increased the total meal EI by 61 %( Reference Savage, Fisher and Marini 33 ). These observations were first reported in 5-year-old preschool children, but not in 3-year-old children( Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch 38 ), supporting the self-regulation hypothesis in younger children( Reference Adair 46 Reference Birch and Fisher 48 ). Subsequent studies, however, have demonstrated significant positive effects of larger PS on EI in children even as young as 2 years( Reference Savage, Fisher and Marini 33 , Reference Fisher 42 , Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch 53 , Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch 54 , Reference Looney and Raynor 56 , Reference Smith, Conroy and Wen 57 ). Moreover, there is evidence to suggest that these effects are sustained for up to 24 h( Reference Fisher, Arreola and Birch 55 ).

Table 1. Studies investigating children's energy intake (EI) response to a change in portion size (PS)

Δ, change; yr, year; B, boys; G, girls; AA, African-American; wk, week; ↑, increase; NS, not significant; H, Hispanics; NR, not reported ↓, decrease.

* Net effect on energy intake (i.e. over total eating occasion/study).

Study also investigated the effect of self-serve portions on children's energy intake.

Study also investigated the effect of energy density (5·4 and 7·5 kJ/g) on children's energy intake.

§ Study also investigated the effect of energy density (6·7 and 5·0 kJ/g) on children's energy intake.

Study also investigated the effect of energy density (1·8 and 5·0 kJ/g) on children's energy intake.

Study only reported change in food intake (g) which significantly ↓ in 4-yr-old children (179 and 183 v. 256 g), but significantly ↑ in 6-yr-old children (252 v. 325 v. 441 g).

In the only studies investigating the impact of ‘reduced’ PS in children, while no change in EI was observed when the PS of the entree decreased by 25 %( Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 ), a positive effect on EI was apparent in other studies when age-appropriate PS were assessed( Reference Savage, Fisher and Marini 33 , Reference Smith, Conroy and Wen 57 ). In the study by Smith et al. (2013)( Reference Smith, Conroy and Wen 57 ), it is also noteworthy that, unlike the 6-year-old children, the younger children (4-year olds) did not respond to the larger PS by eating more. In fact, they consumed significantly less food when served the large PS, in comparison with the smaller (reference) PS of the same meal (183 (sd 76) g v. 256 (sd 75) g; P≤0·01)( Reference Smith, Conroy and Wen 57 ).

These short-term studies, although limited in number, provide supportive evidence that from an early age children are susceptible to PS cues. In the short-term children will immediately respond to increasing PS by consuming more, but there is limited evidence to establish whether they will compensate for this at subsequent eating occasions( Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch 54 , Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 ). Although evidence from adults suggests larger PS result in a sustained increased in EI for up to 11 d( Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs 59 , Reference Kelly, Wallace and Robson 60 ), no experimental or free-living studies to date have explored the longer-term effects of PS manipulation on the quantity of food consumed in children.

Energy density

Observational evidence

A recent systematic review commissioned by the US Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee (2010)( 61 ) has concluded that the available evidence consistently supports a positive relationship between ED and body-weight in children and adolescents as well as adults. The evidence for the association in young people was based on four methodologically rigorous longitudinal studies, whose key strengths were: (1) use of objective measures of adiposity (including dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry or doubly-labelled water) rather than reliance on proxy measures such as BMI; (2) mis-reporting of dietary EI was appropriately adjusted for; (3) ED was calculated by recommended methods that excluded all or most beverages, to avoid attenuation of results( Reference Johnson, Mander and Jones 62 Reference McCaffrey, Rennie and Kerr 65 ). The latter issue is of critical importance in evaluating associations between ED and adiposity( Reference McCaffrey, Rennie and Kerr 65 ). These authors convincingly demonstrated that when the calculation of ED included liquids (i.e. water, energy-free and energy-containing beverages), there was no association between ED and the change in adiposity between baseline and follow-up in their study cohort. In contrast, when the ED was calculated based on solid foods, including milk as a food, the ED of the diet at baseline did positively predict the change in adiposity over time. Furthermore, in this study it was the ED of the total diet rather than any particular part of the dietary pattern (e.g. the ED of snacks) that was associated with the change in adiposity( Reference McCaffrey, Rennie and Kerr 65 ).

Intervention studies

Studies investigating children's responsiveness to changes in ED are shown in Table 2. Although manipulating the ED of a single snack did not significantly affect children's EI at that eating occasion( Reference Looney and Raynor 56 ), reducing the ED of an entree has been shown to reduce children's total EI at that meal( Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch 54 , Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 , Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls 66 ). These studies were similar in design to the PS interventions described earlier and the effect of decreasing ED on EI was also of a similar magnitude (17–18 % decrease). Further research has shown that this effect on EI can be sustained when the ED of multiple meals were manipulated over 2 d( Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls 67 ).

Table 2. Studies investigating children's energy intake (EI) response to a change in energy density (ED)

Δ, change; yr, year; B, boys; G, girls; H, Hispanics; wk, week; ↑, increase; NR, not reported; ↓, decrease; AA, African-American.

* Net effect on energy intake (i.e. over total eating occasion/study).

Study also investigated the effect of portion size (250 and 500 g) on children's energy intake.

Study also investigated the effect of portion size (300 and 400 g) on children's energy intake.

§ Information only provided for twenty-five out of the twenty-six children.

Study also investigated the effect of portion size (150 and 300 g) on children's energy intake.

Reductions in ED appear to have a positive effect on adiposity in the longer-term, particularly when children and parents are provided with more positively focused messages( Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher 68 ). Decreases in BMI z-scores were significantly greater in children advised to ‘increase their intake of healthy foods’ compared to those children advised to ‘reduce intake of high ED foods’ at both 12 (−0·30 v. −0.15 zBMI units; P=0·01) and 24 months (−0·36 v. −0·13 zBMI units; P=0·04)( Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher 68 ). From a public health perspective this study has revealed key insights about the importance of using positive messages to communicate dietary messages about weight control.

Strategies to reduce children's energy intake

A number of observational studies have associated increasing PS( Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch 40 , Reference Huang, Howarth and Lin 52 , Reference Lioret, Volatier and Lafay 69 ) and ED( Reference Mendoza, Drewnowski and Christakis 70 , Reference Vernarelli, Mitchell and Hartman 71 ) with overweight and obesity in children, and not surprisingly have concluded that addressing such environmental factors is essential if children's food and EI is to be modified appropriately. While all studies that have decreased the ED of children's food agree on the positive effect on children's EI( Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 , Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls 66 , Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls 67 ), there is a paucity of data that unequivocally demonstrates the effectiveness of smaller PS.

Additive effects of portion size and energy density on energy intakes

Three studies so far have simultaneously manipulated both the PS and ED of children's food intake( Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch 54 , Reference Looney and Raynor 56 , Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 ) to investigate their independent and/or additive effects on EI (Tables 1 and 2).

In the study by Leahy et al. (2008)( Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 ), children's EI at a meal was affected by ED (but not PS). In contrast, Looney & Raynor( Reference Looney and Raynor 56 ) demonstrated an effect of PS (but not ED) on children's EI at a single snacking occasion. Leahy et al.( Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 ) showed that a decrease in ED of a meal (6·7 v. 5.0 kJ/g) resulted in a 17 % decrease in EI, irrespective of the PS served( Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher 58 ). Conversely, Looney & Raynor( Reference Looney and Raynor 56 ), showed that an increase in the PS of a snack (150 v. 300 g) resulted in an 18 % increase in EI, irrespective of the ED of the snack provided. These findings suggest that different strategies to reduce children's EI may be required depending on the type of eating occasion. However, in the study by Fisher et al. ( Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch 54 ), PS and ED had both independent and additive effects on children's EI. Overall, an increase in both PS (250 v. 500 g) and ED (5·4 v. 7.5 kJ/g) resulted in a 34 % increased EI at that meal: an effect that was approximately double that reported when the main effects of each factor were analysed individually.

Together this evidence has prompted researchers to investigate novel strategies to reduce ED of children's meals, without compromising on the PS, so that children will compensate by increasing their intake of other foods.

Novel strategies manipulating portion size and/or energy density to modify children's energy intakes

Similar to the findings in studies conducted in adults( Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs 72 ), offering children a large portion of a low-energy dense first course (e.g. vegetable soup)( Reference Spill, Birch and Roe 73 , Reference Spill, Birch and Roe 74 ) or larger portions of fruit and vegetables with a meal( Reference Kral, Kabay and Roe 75 , Reference Mathias, Rolls and Birch 76 ), were effective strategies in both promoting fruit and vegetable intake and decreasing the ED of children's meals. For example, in the study by Kral et al. ( Reference Kral, Kabay and Roe 75 ) when the PS of the broccoli and carrots side dish was doubled (75 g v. 150 g), children consumed significantly less of the pasta entree. Although the difference in overall EI at the meal was NS, the ED of the foods which children consumed at the meal did significantly decrease with the larger portions of fruit and vegetables (3·97 (sd 0.08) kJ/g v. 3.72 (sd 0.08) kJ/g; P=0·005). Moreover, others have shown that reducing the PS of high ED foods (e.g. French fries) and replacing with apple slices( Reference Wansink and Hanks 77 ) or serving dessert alongside the main meal, as opposed to after( Reference Huss, Laurentz and Fisher 78 ), can attenuate EI irrespective of the PS or choice of main entree. These studies clearly demonstrate the potential to employ more novel strategies for decreasing children's EI, while at the same time not compromising on palatability.

Spill et al. ( Reference Spill, Birch and Roe 79 ) also recently demonstrated that incorporating pureed vegetables into multiple meals, to achieve a 25 % decrease in ED, resulted in a down-regulation of preschool children's EI by 12 % (3–5-year-old boys and girls, n 40). Interestingly this reduction in EI persisted over a full day, and moreover, children did not compensate by consuming greater quantities of the un-manipulated snacks and side dishes offered throughout the day, as might have been hypothesised( Reference Spill, Birch and Roe 79 ).

Overall, these studies demonstrate that simply serving or covertly hiding more fruit and vegetables within children's meals can positively influence children's EI, at the same time as encouraging the consumption of these more healthy foods.

Parental influences

Dietary habits formed in early life are predictive of future eating patterns( Reference Birch 80 ) and evidence suggests the earlier and broader the experience with food, the healthier the child's diet will be( Reference Skinner, Carruth and Bounds 81 ). Given that obese parents are more likely to have an overweight/obese child( Reference Whitaker, Wright and Pepe 82 , Reference Lobstein, Baur and Uauy 83 ), arguably this increased risk of childhood obesity is highly likely to be influenced by the epigenetic interactions within the shared family experience, related to both the food and activity environment( Reference Faith, Scanlon and Birch 84 Reference McCaffrey, Rennie and Wallace 87 ).

Research has shown that a coercive feeding approach, e.g. encouraging children to ‘clean the plate’, may actually have a counterproductive effect on food intake( Reference Birch, McPheee and Shoba 88 Reference Wansink, Payne and Werle 91 ). Disruption of children's innate ability to self-regulate their EI and therefore encouraging eating in the absence of hunger has indeed been prospectively linked to an increased risk of becoming overweight, albeit only in girls( Reference Fisher and Birch 92 ). Furthermore, this compensation ability has also been shown to decrease with age, particularly in obesity-prone children (i.e. those born to mothers with a pregnancy BMI >66th percentile)( Reference Kral, Stunkard and Berkowitz 39 ).

Further research has shown that the amount of food parents or caregivers serve their children is directly related to the amount of food served( Reference Mrdjenovic and Levitsky 49 ) and is also predicted by the amount they serve themselves( Reference Johnson, Hughes and Cui 93 ). In a repeated-measures cross-sectional study (145 parents and their preschool children), Johnson et al. (2014)( Reference Johnson, Hughes and Cui 93 ) reported a positive association between the amount of food parents served themselves and the amount of food served to and consumed by their children during three at-home evening meals. These findings add support to studies conducted in the laboratory setting, by showing that in a more natural family environment, children will also respond to larger PS by eating more.

Other environmental cues

Again, similar to findings in studies conducted in adults( Reference Wansink and Cheney 94 Reference Raynor and Wing 98 ), children's PS can also be influenced by food preferences and visual cues, such as self-served portions and the size of tableware.

The self-service of food or beverages requires some form of conscious effort and thought from an individual about how they are going to serve it, as well as the PS they wish to consume. For example, an individual may be perceptually driven to completely fill a cereal bowl, if they perceive an increased level of hunger at the time( Reference van Ittersum and Wansink 99 ). Analysis of large-scale survey data from 4966 fifth grade US students (aged 10–11 years) showed that children's liking for higher-energy dense foods (e.g. French fries, meats and potato chips) led to a preference for larger-than-recommended portions compared with that for lower-energy dense foods (e.g. vegetables)( Reference Colapinto, Fitzgerald and Taper 100 ). However, the results of experimental studies in this area are equivocal suggesting that self-serving may not provide a ‘one-size-fits-all’ approach to facilitate the avoidance of overeating in response to larger PS( Reference Fisher 42 , Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch 53 , Reference Savage, Haisfield and Fisher 101 ).

The PS of self-served food is also partly influenced by the size of the plate, bowl or glass used( Reference van Ittersum and Wansink 99 ). Early research in this area suggests that children perceive taller containers to hold more of a food/beverage than shorter, wider containers( Reference Piaget, Inhelder and Szeminska 102 ). Wansink & van Ittersum( Reference Wansink and van Ittersum 103 ) subsequently showed that this visual illusion caused children to pour and consume significantly more energy from fruit and soft drinks when they were given a short, wide glass compared with a taller narrow glass (of the same volume capacity). Similar findings have since been confirmed for foods served using different sizes of spoons( Reference Fisher, Birch and Zhang 104 ) or onto larger adult-sized dinnerware( Reference DiSantis, Birch and Davey 105 , Reference Wansink, van Ittersum and Payne 106 ), with effects reported to be greater in extraverted compared with introverted children( Reference van Ittersum and Wansink 107 ).

Conclusions

Positive effects of increased PS and ED on children's food and EI have been observed in children as young as 2 years, and of particular concern, both these factors have been shown to exert independent but additive effects to promote EI. The overwhelming majority of studies manipulating PS or ED in children, however, have been acute, single-eating occasion studies, with only two studies investigating the impact of PS and/or ED on overall food intake for 24 h. Studies have been conducted in both laboratory and more naturalistic settings, for example in the usual classroom setting, but have tended to focus on similar foods/entree meals.

In general, children tend to eat proportionally more as the PS and/or ED increases, but as most of the evidence is based on children aged 3–6 years, it remains unclear how early young children will begin to overrule their self-regulation of EI when exposed to such environmental triggers. Furthermore, there is a paucity of data in older children and adolescents, particularly conducted across other countries outside of the USA, where the increase in food PS within the marketplace may not be so apparent.

Although a direct causal link between PS and obesity remains to be established, the regular consumption of large PS of energy dense foods do favour obesity-promoting eating behaviours in children. The cumulative evidence relating to strategies to reduce children's EI and outlined in the current review, provides a strong basis for the provision of food PS advice to parents. In addition, such advice should be cognisant of the array of environmental cues consistently reported to inadvertently increase children's food PS, from as young as age 2 years. More emphasis on parental education on PS distortion and appropriate child-sized portions is clearly merited given their role in determining their children's food intake. Current childhood obesity prevention campaigns in the UK( 108 ) and Ireland( 109 ), have highlighted the key importance of offering child-sized portions on appropriately-sized dishware, as well as encouraging lower energy dense snack foods and beverages. More research, however, is required to establish the most feasible and effective intervention and policies to counteract the deleterious impact of PS and ED on children's EI.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the Irish Section of the Nutrition Society for inviting this symposium presentation.

Financial support

None.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Authorship

L. K. P. reviewed the literature and wrote the manuscript; T. A. M., M. A. K. and M. B. E. L. provided guidance and commented on drafts of the review.

References

1. Young, LR & Nestle, M (2002) The contribution of expanding portion sizes to the US obesity epidemic. Am J Public Health 92, 246249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2. Young, LR & Nestle, M (2003) Expanding portion sizes in the US marketplace: implications for nutrition counseling. J Am Diet Assoc 103, 231234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Nielsen, SJ & Popkin, BM (2003) Patterns and trends in food portion sizes, 1977–1998. J Am Med Assoc 289, 450453.Google Scholar
4. Smiciklas-Wright, H, Mitchell, DC, Mickle, SJ et al. (2003) Foods commonly eaten in the United States, 1989–1991 and 1994–1996: are portion sizes changing? J Am Diet Assoc 103, 4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Nielsen, SJ & Popkin, BM (2004) Changes in beverage intake between 1977 and 2001. Am J Prev Med 27, 205210.Google Scholar
6. Piernas, C & Popkin, BM (2011) Food portion patterns and trends among U.S. children and the relationship to total eating occasion size, 1977–2006. J Nutr 141, 11591164.Google Scholar
7. Piernas, C & Popkin, BM (2011) Increased portion sizes from energy-dense foods affect total energy intake at eating occasions in US children and adolescents: patterns and trends by age group and sociodemographic characteristics, 1977–2006. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 13241332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Matthiessen, J, Fagt, S, Biltoft-Jensen, A et al. (2003) Size makes a difference. Public Health Nutr 6, 6572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Steenhuis, IH, Leeuwis, FH & Vermeer, WM (2010) Small, medium, large or supersize: trends in food portion sizes in The Netherlands. Public Health Nutr 13, 852857.Google Scholar
10. Wrieden, W, Gregor, A & Barton, K (2008) Have food portion sizes increased in the UK over the last 20 years? Proc Nutr Soc 67, E211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11. Church, S (2008) Trends in Portion Sizes in the UK – A Preliminary Review of Published Information. London, UK: Food Standards Agency.Google Scholar
12. British Heart Foundation (2013) Portion Distortion: How Much are We Really Eating? UK: British Heart Foundation.Google Scholar
13. Rozin, P, Kabnick, K, Pete, E et al. (2003) The ecology of eating: smaller portion sizes in France than in the United States help explain the French paradox. Psychol Sci 14, 450454.Google Scholar
14. O'Brien, S, Lyons, J, McNulty, BA et al. (2012) Trends in the portion size of savoury snack intakes in Irish adults during 2001 and 2011. Proc Nutr Soc 71, E211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. O'Brien, S, McNulty, BA, Nugent, AP et al. (2012) A comparison of portion size differences in unpackaged bakery products in Ireland from 1997–2011. Proc Nutr Soc 71, E176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16. Faulkner, GP, Pourshahidi, LK, Wallace, JMW et al. (2012) Serving size guidance for consumers: is it effective? Proc Nutr Soc 71, 610621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Institute of Grocery Distribution (2008) Portion Size: a Review of Existing Approaches. UK: Institute of Grocery Distribution.Google Scholar
18. O'Dwyer, NA, McCarthy, SN, Burke, SJ et al. (2005) The temporal pattern of the contribution of fat to energy and of food groups to fat at various eating locations: implications for developing food-based dietary guidelines. Public Health Nutr 8, 249257.Google Scholar
19. Orfanos, P, Naska, A, Trichopoulou, A et al. (2009) Eating out of home: energy, macro- and micronutrient intakes in 10 European countries. The European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition. Eur J Clin Nutr 63, Suppl. 4, S239S262.Google Scholar
20. Lachat, C, Nago, E, Verstraeten, R et al. (2012) Eating out of home and its association with dietary intake: a systematic review of the evidence. Obes Rev 13, 329346.Google Scholar
21. Thompson, OM, Ballew, C, Resnicow, K et al. (2004) Food purchased away from home as a predictor of change in BMI z-score among girls. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 28, 282289.Google Scholar
22. Taveras, EM, Berkey, CS, Rifas-Shiman, SL et al. (2005) Association of consumption of fried food away from home with body mass index and diet quality in older children and adolescents. Pediatrics 116, e518e524.Google Scholar
23. Burke, SJ, McCarthy, SN, O'Neill, JL et al. (2007) An examination of the influence of eating location on the diets of Irish children. Public Health Nutr 10, 599607.Google Scholar
24. Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs (2005) Family Food: A Report on the 2003–04 Expenditure and Food Survey. London: TSO.Google Scholar
25. Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs (2011) Family Food 2010. London: TSO.Google Scholar
26. Central Statistics Office (2001) Household Budget Survey 1999/2000. Dublin: CSO.Google Scholar
27. Central Statistics Office (2012) Household Budget Survey 2009/2010. Dublin: CSO.Google Scholar
28. safefood (2013) Do you Have a Kids Menu? A Report into Kids Meals When Eating Out (Summary Report). Cork, Ireland: safefood.Google Scholar
29. Ello-Martin, JA, Ledikwe, JH & Rolls, BJ (2005) The influence of food portion size and energy density on energy intake: implications for weight management. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 236S241S.Google Scholar
30. Fisher, JO & Kral, TV (2008) Super-size me: portion size effects on young children's eating. Physiol Behav 94, 3947.Google Scholar
31. Steenhuis, IH & Vermeer, WM (2009) Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 6, 58.Google Scholar
32. Rodrigues, AG, Proenca, RP, Calvo, MC et al. (2012) Overweight/obesity is associated with food choices related to rice and beans, colors of salads, and portion size among consumers at a restaurant serving buffet-by-weight in Brazil. Appetite 59, 305311.Google Scholar
33. Savage, JS, Fisher, JO, Marini, M et al. (2012) Serving smaller age-appropriate entree portions to children aged 3–5 y increases fruit and vegetable intake and reduces energy density and energy intake at lunch. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 335341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Birch, LL & Deysher, M (1986) Caloric compensation and sensory specific satiety: evidence for self regulation of food intake by young children. Appetite 7, 323331.Google Scholar
35. Birch, LL, Johnson, SL, Andresen, G et al. (1991) The variability of young children's energy intake. N Engl J Med 324, 232235.Google Scholar
36. Birch, LL, McPhee, LS, Bryant, JL et al. (1993) Children's lunch intake: effects of midmorning snacks varying in energy density and fat content. Appetite 20, 8394.Google Scholar
37. Araya, H, Vera, G & Alvina, M (1999) Effect of the energy density and volume of high carbohydrate meals on short term satiety in preschool children. Eur J Clin Nutr 53, 273276.Google Scholar
38. Rolls, BJ, Engell, D & Birch, LL (2000) Serving portion size influences 5-year-old but not 3-year-old children's food intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 100, 232234.Google Scholar
39. Kral, TV, Stunkard, AJ, Berkowitz, RI et al. (2007) Daily food intake in relation to dietary energy density in the free-living environment: a prospective analysis of children born at different risk of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 4147.Google Scholar
40. McConahy, KL, Smiciklas-Wright, H, Birch, LL et al. (2002) Food portions are positively related to energy intake and body weight in early childhood. J Pediatr 140, 340347.Google Scholar
41. Fox, MK, Devaney, B, Reidy, K et al. (2006) Relationship between portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: evidence of self-regulation. J Am Diet Assoc 106, S7783.Google Scholar
42. Fisher, JO (2007) Effects of age on children's intake of large and self-selected food portions. Obesity 15, 403412.Google Scholar
43. Duffey, KJ & Popkin, BM (2011) Energy density, portion size, and eating occasions: contributions to increased energy intake in the United States, 1977–2006. PLoS Med 8, e1001050.Google Scholar
44. Duffey, KJ & Popkin, BM (2013) Causes of increased energy intake among children in the U.S., 1977–2010. Am J Prev Med 44, e1e8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Slining, MM, Mathias, KC & Popkin, BM (2013) Trends in food and beverage sources among US children and adolescents: 1989–2010. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 16831694.Google Scholar
46. Adair, LS (1984) The infant's ability to self-regulate caloric intake: a case study. J Am Diet Assoc 84, 543546.Google Scholar
47. Shea, S, Stein, AD, Basch, CE et al. (1992) Variability and self-regulation of energy intake in young children in their everyday environment. Pediatrics 90, 542546.Google Scholar
48. Birch, LL & Fisher, JO (1998) Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 101, 539549.Google Scholar
49. Mrdjenovic, G & Levitsky, DA (2005) Children eat what they are served: the imprecise regulation of energy intake. Appetite 44, 273282.Google Scholar
50. McConahy, KL, Smiciklas-Wright, H, Mitchell, DC et al. (2004) Portion size of common foods predicts energy intake among preschool-aged children. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 975979.Google Scholar
51. Kerr, M, Rennie, K, McCaffrey, T et al. (2009) Snacking patterns among adolescents: a comparison of type, frequency and portion size between Britain in 1997 and Northern Ireland in 2005. Br J Nutr 101, 122.Google Scholar
52. Huang, TT, Howarth, NC, Lin, BH et al. (2004) Energy intake and meal portions: associations with BMI percentile in U.S. children. Obes Res 12, 18751885.Google Scholar
53. Fisher, JO, Rolls, BJ & Birch, LL (2003) Children's bite size and intake of an entrée are greater with large portions than with age-appropriate or self-selected portions. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 11641170.Google Scholar
54. Fisher, JO, Liu, Y, Birch, LL et al. (2007) Effects of portion size and energy density on young children's intake at a meal. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 174179.Google Scholar
55. Fisher, JO, Arreola, A, Birch, LL et al. (2007) Portion size effects on daily energy intake in low-income Hispanic and African American children and their mothers. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 17091716.Google Scholar
56. Looney, SM & Raynor, HA (2011) Impact of portion size and energy density on snack intake in preschool-aged children. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 414418.Google Scholar
57. Smith, L, Conroy, K, Wen, H et al. (2013) Portion size variably affects food intake of 6-year-old and 4-year-old children in Kunming, China. Appetite 69, 3138.Google Scholar
58. Leahy, KE, Birch, LL, Fisher, JO et al. (2008) Reductions in entree energy density increase children's vegetable intake and reduce energy intake. Obesity 16, 15591565.Google Scholar
59. Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS & Meengs, JS (2007) The effect of large portion sizes on energy intake is sustained for 11 days. Obesity 15, 15351543.Google Scholar
60. Kelly, M, Wallace, J, Robson, P et al. (2009) Increased portion size leads to a sustained increase in energy intake over 4 d in normal-weight and overweight men and women. Br J Nutr 102, 470.Google Scholar
61. Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee (2010) Report of the Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee on the Dietary Guidelines for Americans. USDA.Google Scholar
62. Johnson, L, Mander, AP, Jones, LR et al. (2008) Energy-dense, low-fiber, high-fat dietary pattern is associated with increased fatness in childhood. Am J Clin Nutr 87, 846854.Google Scholar
63. Johnson, L, Mander, AP, Jones, LR et al. (2008) A prospective analysis of dietary energy density at age 5 and 7 years and fatness at 9 years among UK children. Int J Obes 32, 586593.Google Scholar
64. Johnson, L, van Jaarsveld, CH, Emmett, PM et al. (2009) Dietary energy density affects fat mass in early adolescence and is not modified by FTO variants. PLoS ONE 4, e4594.Google Scholar
65. McCaffrey, TA, Rennie, KL, Kerr, MA et al. (2008) Energy density of the diet and change in body fatness from childhood to adolescence: is there a relation? Am J Clin Nutr 87, 12301237.Google Scholar
66. Leahy, KE, Birch, LL & Rolls, BJ (2008) Reducing the energy density of an entree decreases children's energy intake at lunch. J Am Diet Assoc 108, 4148.Google Scholar
67. Leahy, KE, Birch, LL & Rolls, BJ (2008) Reducing the energy density of multiple meals decreases the energy intake of preschool-age children. Am J Clin Nutr 88, 14591468.Google Scholar
68. Epstein, LH, Paluch, RA, Beecher, MD et al. (2008) Increasing healthy eating vs. reducing high energy-dense foods to treat pediatric obesity. Obesity 16, 318326.Google Scholar
69. Lioret, S, Volatier, JL, Lafay, L et al. (2009) Is food portion size a risk factor of childhood overweight? Eur J Clin Nutr 63, 382391.Google Scholar
70. Mendoza, JA, Drewnowski, A & Christakis, DA (2007) Dietary energy density is associated with obesity and the metabolic syndrome in U.S. adults. Diab Care 30, 974979.Google Scholar
71. Vernarelli, JA, Mitchell, DC, Hartman, TJ et al. (2011) Dietary energy density is associated with body weight status and vegetable intake in U.S. children. J Nutr 141, 22042210.Google Scholar
72. Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS & Meengs, JS (2004) Salad and satiety: energy density and portion size of a first-course salad affect energy intake at lunch. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 15701576.Google Scholar
73. Spill, MK, Birch, LL, Roe, LS et al. (2010) Eating vegetables first: the use of portion size to increase vegetable intake in preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 12371243.Google Scholar
74. Spill, MK, Birch, LL, Roe, LS et al. (2011) Serving large portions of vegetable soup at the start of a meal affected children's energy and vegetable intake. Appetite 57, 213219.Google Scholar
75. Kral, TV, Kabay, AC, Roe, LS et al. (2010) Effects of doubling the portion size of fruit and vegetable side dishes on children's intake at a meal. Obesity 18, 521527.Google Scholar
76. Mathias, KC, Rolls, BJ, Birch, LL et al. (2012) Serving larger portions of fruits and vegetables together at dinner promotes intake of both foods among young children. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 266270.Google Scholar
77. Wansink, B & Hanks, AS (2014) Calorie reductions and within-meal calorie compensation in children's meal combos. Obesity 22, 630632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78. Huss, LR, Laurentz, S, Fisher, JO et al. (2013) Timing of serving dessert but not portion size affects young children's intake at lunchtime. Appetite 68, 158163.Google Scholar
79. Spill, MK, Birch, LL, Roe, LS et al. (2011) Hiding vegetables to reduce energy density: an effective strategy to increase children's vegetable intake and reduce energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 735741.Google Scholar
80. Birch, LL (1998) Development of food acceptance patterns in the first years of life. Proc Nutr Soc 57, 617624.Google Scholar
81. Skinner, JD, Carruth, BR, Bounds, W et al. (2002) Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav 34, 310315.Google Scholar
82. Whitaker, RC, Wright, JA, Pepe, MS et al. (1997) Predicting obesity in young adulthood from childhood and parental obesity. N Engl J Med 337, 869873.Google Scholar
83. Lobstein, T, Baur, L, Uauy, R et al. (2004) Obesity in children and young people: a crisis in public health. Obes Rev 5, Suppl. 1, 4104.Google Scholar
84. Faith, MS, Scanlon, KS, Birch, LL et al. (2004) Parent–child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes Res 12, 17111722.Google Scholar
85. Campbell, KJ, Crawford, DA & Ball, K (2006) Family food environment and dietary behaviors likely to promote fatness in 5–6 year-old children. Int J Obes 30, 12721280.Google Scholar
86. Spruijt-Metz, D, Li, C, Cohen, E et al. (2006) Longitudinal influence of mother's child-feeding practices on adiposity in children. J Pediatr 148, 314320.Google Scholar
87. McCaffrey, TA, Rennie, KL, Wallace, JMW et al. (2007) Dietary determinants of childhood obesity: the role of the family. Curr Med Lit: Clin Nutr 15, 5156.Google Scholar
88. Birch, LL, McPheee, L, Shoba, BC et al. (1987) ‘Clean up your plate’: effects of child feeding practices on the conditioning of meal size. Learn Motiv 18, 301317.Google Scholar
89. Fisher, JO & Birch, LL (1999) Restricting access to palatable foods affects children's behavioral response, food selection, and intake. Am J Clin Nutr 69, 12641272.Google Scholar
90. Galloway, AT, Fiorito, LM, Francis, LA et al. (2006) ‘Finish your soup’: counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect. Appetite 46, 318323.Google Scholar
91. Wansink, B, Payne, C & Werle, C (2008) Consequences of belonging to the ‘clean plate club’. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 162, 994995.Google Scholar
92. Fisher, JO & Birch, LL (2002) Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 226231.Google Scholar
93. Johnson, SL, Hughes, SO, Cui, X et al. (2014) Portion sizes for children are predicted by parental characteristics and the amounts parents serve themselves. Am J Clin Nutr 99, 763770.Google Scholar
94. Wansink, B & Cheney, MM (2005) Super Bowls: serving bowl size and food consumption. J Am Med Assoc 293, 17271728.Google Scholar
95. Wansink, B & Kim, J (2005) Bad popcorn in big buckets: portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav 37, 242245.Google Scholar
96. Wansink, B, van Ittersum, K & Painter, JE (2006) Ice cream illusions bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med 31, 240243.Google Scholar
97. Wansink, B, Painter, JE & Lee, YK (2006) The office candy dish: proximity's influence on estimated and actual consumption. Int J Obes 30, 871875.Google Scholar
98. Raynor, HA & Wing, RR (2007) Package unit size and amount of food: do both influence intake? Obesity 15, 23112319.Google Scholar
99. van Ittersum, K & Wansink, B (2007) Do children really prefer large portions? Visual illusions bias their estimates and intake. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 11071110.Google Scholar
100. Colapinto, CK, Fitzgerald, A, Taper, LJ et al. (2007) Children's preference for large portions: prevalence, determinants, and consequences. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 11831190.Google Scholar
101. Savage, JS, Haisfield, L, Fisher, JO et al. (2012) Do children eat less at meals when allowed to serve themselves? Am J Clin Nutr 96, 3643.Google Scholar
102. Piaget, J, Inhelder, B & Szeminska, A (1960) The Child's Conception of Geometry. New York: Harper Tourchbooks.Google Scholar
103. Wansink, B & van Ittersum, K (2003) Bottom's up! The influence of elongation on pouring and consumption volume. J Consum Res 30, 455463.Google Scholar
104. Fisher, JO, Birch, LL, Zhang, J et al. (2013) External influences on children's self-served portions at meals. Int J Obes 37, 954960.Google Scholar
105. DiSantis, KI, Birch, LL, Davey, A et al. (2013) Plate size and children's appetite: effects of larger dishware on self-served portions and intake. Pediatrics 131, e1451e1458.Google Scholar
106. Wansink, B, van Ittersum, K & Payne, CR (2014) Larger bowl size increases the amount of cereal children request, consume, and waste. J Pediatr 164, 323326.Google Scholar
107. van Ittersum, K & Wansink, B (2013) Extraverted children are more biased by bowl sizes than introverts. PLoS ONE 8, e78224.Google Scholar
108. Change for Life (2013) Change for Life: Eat well, move more, live longer. http://www.nhs.uk/change4life/Pages/change-for-life.aspx (accessed March 2014).Google Scholar
109. safefood (2013) Let's Take on Childhood Obesity. http://www.safefood.eu/Childhood-Obesity/Welcome.aspx (accessed March 2014).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Studies investigating children's energy intake (EI) response to a change in portion size (PS)

Figure 1

Table 2. Studies investigating children's energy intake (EI) response to a change in energy density (ED)