INTRODUCTION
To communicate successfully in a second or additional language (L2), speakers must convey their message in a way that listeners can understand. Listeners may both understand a speaker and find the speaker easy to understand or may understand a speaker while needing to expend considerable effort. This is the basis for the distinction between intelligibility, a measure of actual understanding, and comprehensibility, listeners’ perceived ease of understanding (Munro & Derwing, Reference Munro and Derwing1995; Nagle & Huensch, Reference Nagle and Huensch2020). While intelligibility is a sensible baseline, most L2 speakers want their speech to be easy to understand, a goal that is more closely aligned with the notion of comprehensibility. Comprehensibility is also an intuitive evaluation that can be assessed through simple rating scales (e.g., very difficult–very easy to understand). Comprehensibility has therefore emerged as a particularly useful construct. Multiple lexical, grammatical, and phonological features underpin comprehensibility (Saito et al., Reference Saito, Trofimovich and Isaacs2017; Trofimovich & Isaacs, Reference Trofimovich and Isaacs2012), which means that L2 speakers’ comprehensibility is likely to change as they produce varying levels of accuracy and complexity in each of these dimensions (Nagle et al., Reference Nagle, Trofimovich and Bergeron2019). Comprehensibility and the linguistic features that underlie it also depend on the characteristics of the communicative task. For instance, when speakers engage in cognitively demanding tasks, their comprehensibility may decrease, and the use of accurate and sophisticated grammar and vocabulary may take on greater importance as they strive to convey complex ideas and relationships (Crowther et al., Reference Crowther, Trofimovich, Isaacs and Saito2015).
What is missing from this body of work is a nuanced understanding of how comprehensibility unfolds over time in interactive scenarios, as speakers and listeners react and adapt to each other in real time. Recent work, which is compatible with dynamic views of language learning and use (de Bot et al., Reference de Bot, Lowie and Verspoor2007), has begun to address this challenge, showing that comprehensibility is at least partially coconstructed (Trofimovich et al., Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020). Speakers and listeners appear to calibrate their speech to one another, resulting in a dynamic coupling of their comprehensibility. In interaction, however, comprehensibility is about more than just linguistic features. Comprehensibility might also have a strong affective and behavioral dimension. Just as listeners make a range of interpersonal evaluations based on a speaker’s pronunciation (Fuertes et al., Reference Fuertes, Gottdiener, Martin, Gilbert and Giles2012), so too can the affective and behavioral dimensions of interpersonal dynamics influence interlocutors’ comprehensibility. If L2 research is to achieve a transdisciplinary perspective (The Douglas Fir Group, 2016), then speech ratings, such as comprehensibility, must be coordinated with other socioaffective and behavioral measures that address the multidimensional nature of L2 communication.
Two socioaffective and behavioral components of communication that might have relevance to comprehensibility are interlocutors’ anxiety and engagement, both of which can be conceived of as person-specific traits (i.e., some individuals are more anxious or engaged than others) and as states that emerge depending on the characteristics of the communicative setting (i.e., in certain situations, an individual may become more or less anxious or engaged). Broadly defined as a person’s negative emotional reaction experienced in a situation in which a language is used (Gardner & MacIntyre, Reference Gardner and MacIntyre1993), anxiety has been linked to lower levels of language achievement, with a medium-size effect (r = −.36), as shown in a recent meta-analysis of 97 studies (Teimouri et al., Reference Teimouri, Goetze and Plonsky2019). Increased levels of anxiety appear to inhibit the processing of linguistic stimuli at the input stage and to interfere with language production (MacIntyre & Gardner, Reference MacIntyre and Gardner1994). As a construct with a strong socioaffective component, anxiety has also been argued to impact L2 speaker attitudes and motivational dispositions (Gardner & MacIntyre, Reference Gardner and MacIntyre1993) and to undermine language development by disrupting communication processes (Dewaele, Reference Dewaele2010). More recent research investigating state- or situation-specific aspects of anxiety has linked it on a dynamic timescale to speakers’ individual experiences, such as their topic choice, their knowledge of vocabulary, and listeners’ verbal and nonverbal reactions to speakers (Gregersen et al., Reference Gregersen, MacIntyre and Meza2014). Overall, then, the accumulated body of work on anxiety suggests that experiencing high levels of anxiety in general or at particular points in an interaction might distract interlocutors, interfering with the cognitive processes that are necessary for producing and comprehending speech. This interference could then lead to decreased comprehensibility.
Another dimension relevant to comprehensibility is speaker engagement, which broadly refers to people’s degree of interest and participation in an activity (Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016). To date, various components of engagement—including cognitive (e.g., sustained attention or effort), behavioral (e.g., quantity of task-relevant talk), and social (e.g., reciprocity shown by speakers, as in turn-taking)—have been linked to contextual and situational variables in L2 communication. Speaker engagement is high when interlocutors communicate about familiar topics, rather than repeat the same task and content (Qiu & Lo, Reference Qiu and Lo2017). Engagement is also high when speakers discuss content relevant to their lives and experiences, compared to externally imposed topics (Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Philp and Nakamura2017), and engagement is greater when speakers communicate with interlocutors of higher proficiency (Dao & McDonough, Reference Dao and McDonough2018). Unlike computer-mediated communication, face-to-face interaction is particularly conducive to eliciting higher levels of engagement in L2 speakers, especially in complex tasks (Baralt et al., Reference Baralt, Gurzynski-Weiss, Kim, Sato and Ballinger2016). Seen from this perspective, engagement (broadly defined) might therefore shape interlocutors’ perception of each other’s comprehensibility. That is, whereas anxiety might interfere with the cognitive and behavioral processes that are necessary for successful (L2) communication, speaker engagement might lead to greater understanding, especially in an interactive context where one partner’s comprehensibility is at least partially dependent on the other’s.
THE PRESENT STUDY
Comprehensibility has to date been researched nearly exclusively in relation to linguistic elements of interaction, focusing on how speakers’ comprehensibility is shaped by various phonological, fluency, grammatical, and discursive features in their speech, typically across different tasks (Crowther et al., Reference Crowther, Trofimovich, Isaacs and Saito2015; Saito et al., Reference Saito, Trofimovich and Isaacs2017). The goal of this exploratory study was to extend this work beyond a strictly linguistic realm by investigating comprehensibility in interaction as a function of the affective and behavioral dimensions of anxiety and engagement. Anxiety and engagement are clearly multidimensional constructs, with multiple measures offering insight into their different facets, such as heart rate and galvanic skin response for anxiety or display of positive emotion and turn-taking frequency for engagement. Nevertheless, due to lack of systematic prior work linking comprehensibility to anxiety and engagement, we operationalized anxiety and engagement broadly, using scalar ratings, to elicit interaction-centered measures for these constructs from L2 speakers. Anxiety was defined as perceived stress, worry, or nervousness that a speaker is feeling while completing a task. Engagement was operationally defined as the perceived degree of a speaker’s collaborativeness.
To explore links between interlocutors’ comprehensibility and their perceived anxiety and collaborativeness, we revisited our dataset featuring paired interactions between L2 English speakers in three tasks, where the speakers carried out repeated assessments of themselves and each other (2.5 minutes apart) during 17 minutes of interaction. In our prior publication (Trofimovich et al., Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020), we tracked the speakers’ comprehensibility ratings across time, exploring whether the ratings converged or diverged over time and task. For this report, we analyzed previously unpublished data targeting the speakers’ self- and partner-specific ratings of anxiety and collaborativeness in relation to comprehensibility.
Because of the exploratory nature of this study, we made no specific predictions regarding the nature and strength of the relationships for comprehensibility, beyond anticipating a negative association with anxiety (a higher degree of anxiety might be associated with lower comprehensibility) and a positive association with collaborativeness (greater collaboration might co-vary with higher comprehensibility). However, because speaking task appears to impact situation-specific anxiety and engagement (Gregersen et al., Reference Gregersen, MacIntyre and Meza2014; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Philp and Nakamura2017; Qiu & Lo, Reference Qiu and Lo2017), we anticipated differences in associations across the different tasks performed by the speakers. With the overarching goal of understanding L2 speech as a dynamic, coconstructed system where socioaffective, linguistic, and behavioral factors interact to shape interlocutors’ mutual impressions, we asked the following exploratory research question: To what extent do L2 interlocutors’ impressions of one another’s anxiety and collaborativeness predict their comprehensibility ratings in interaction?
METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
The interaction data came from a corpus of L2–L2 conversations between 40 (14 female, 26 male) university-level speakers at an English-medium university in Canada (Trofimovich et al., Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020). The speakers (M age = 25.85 years, SD = 2.89), who represented 17 ethnolinguistic backgrounds, had begun learning English on average at 8.18 years (SD = 4.58) through primary and secondary instruction in their home countries and were recently accepted first-year graduate students in eight academic disciplines. Because all speakers were studying in a university with a large cohort of international students, they reported substantial daily use of English (M = 56.75%, SD = 19.79; 0–100% scale) and fairly high familiarity with accented English (M = 6.33, SD = 1.67; 1–9 scale). As part of university admission requirements, the speakers reported IELTS (31) or TOEFL (9) scores. When the nine TOEFL scores were replaced by equivalent IELTS values through validated conversion metrics (Educational Testing Service, 2017; Taylor, Reference Taylor2004), the speakers’ IELTS performance was at a mean of 6.84 (SD = 0.62) for speaking and 7.60 (SD = 0.95) for listening. To contextualize these proficiency values among other established metrics, the mean IELTS speaking score of 6.84 roughly corresponds to TOEFL iBT speaking scores in the 20–23 range and the C1 Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR) band, whereas the mean IELTS listening score of 7.60 corresponds to TOEFL iBT listening scores in the 27–28 range and the C1 CEFR band. To encourage the use of English, the 40 speakers were randomly assigned to 20 pairs, such that the paired speakers were previously unfamiliar with each other and came from different backgrounds (see online Supplementary Materials).
SPEAKING TASKS AND TARGET RATINGS
The corpus included three task performances per pair, with all tasks completed in the same order. During the first (warm-up) task, the speakers were asked to discover three things they had in common with their partner (e.g., a favorite movie). For the second task, the speakers were asked to develop a coherent shared narrative using a set of 14 scrambled pictures, with seven images randomly distributed to each partner and partners unable to see one another’s images. The 14 images told a story of a man who won the lottery but subsequently experienced a misfortune that made him realize that wealth does not always equal happiness. For the final task, the speakers were asked first to share some of the challenges they experienced as international students adjusting to life in a new academic environment (e.g., gaining access to health care, obtaining work permits) and then to provide common solutions for these challenges. The warm-up task lasted 3 minutes; the remaining two tasks lasted 7 minutes each.
During the 17-minute interaction, each speaker provided seven sets of ratings for comprehensibility (reported in Trofimovich et al., Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020) and for anxiety and collaborativeness (previously unpublished, analyzed here as time-sensitive predictors of comprehensibility). The seven sets of ratings occurred at comparable intervals: after each task (Times, 1, 4, and 7) and approximately 2.5 minutes and 5 minutes into Task 2 (Times 2 and 3) and Task 3 (Times 5 and 6). The speakers used a paper booklet to record their ratings, with continuous scales (100-millimeter lines) printed next to each dimension, one labeled “me” for the self-rating and the other labeled “my partner” for the rating of the speaker’s partner. Each scale included only endpoint labels, and the speakers marked a point on each line corresponding to their impression.
Although comprehensibility has typically been measured through 7- or 9-point Likert scales (e.g., Munro & Derwing, Reference Munro and Derwing1995), researchers have occasionally opted for continuous scales over ordinal ones, using a straight line bounded by endpoint descriptors in a paper-and-pencil format (e.g., Isaacs et al., Reference Isaacs, Trofimovich, Yu and Chereau2015), as in this study, or a slider to record the rating in a computer or online interface (e.g., Saito et al., Reference Saito, Trofimovich and Isaacs2017). Existing scale validation and scale comparison work indicates that there is little difference in the ratings of comprehensibility obtained through scales of various lengths and resolutions (Isaacs & Thomson, Reference Isaacs and Thomson2013), through different scale types (Munro, Reference Munro, Kang, Thomson and Murphy2018), or through static or dynamic assessments (Nagle et al., Reference Nagle, Trofimovich and Bergeron2019), which implied that the choice of the comprehensibility scale in this study was unlikely to have impacted rating validity. Comprehensibility was defined for the speakers as a judgment of how much effort it takes to understand what someone is saying. Anxiety was introduced as the level of stress, worry, or nervousness that someone is feeling while completing a task. Collaborativeness referred to the action of working with someone to produce or create something. Collaborating implied active participation and working together as a team, whereas not collaborating involved lack of participation and acting as an individual rather than a team member (see online Supplementary Materials).
PROCEDURE
The two speakers in each pair, participating in one audio-recorded session, were seated at opposite sides of a table, with seating determined randomly upon speaker arrival. A low barrier was placed between the speakers to prevent them from seeing one another’s materials while allowing for an unobstructed view of gestures and facial expressions. After completing a background questionnaire, the speakers heard a research assistant (RA) define each rated dimension and explain how to use the rating booklet, which included instructions for each task and seven sets of scales (one per page). The speakers were told that they would engage in repeated assessments, evaluating the immediately preceding 2–3 minutes of interaction, and that their ratings would be private. They were also reminded that, during Tasks 2 and 3, the RA would stop the interaction briefly to allow for mid-task assessments. Specific task instructions were given before each task, always in the same manner. The speakers read the instructions, then summarized the instructions to the RA as a comprehension check, and finally asked clarification questions. The speakers were reminded that Task 1 would be stopped after 3 minutes and Tasks 2 and 3 after 7 minutes even if the discussion were ongoing, and that the RA would be using a timer to keep task duration and assessment intervals comparable.
DATA ANALYSIS
TARGET MEASURES AND COVARIATES
The criterion variable was the speakers’ ratings of their partner’s comprehensibility. As in Trofimovich et al. (Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020), these ratings were recorded per speaker at the seven rating episodes and expressed numerically (out of 100), by measuring the distance with a ruler (to the nearest millimeter) between the anchor point and the speaker’s mark (the intersection of the cross or angle point of the checkmark) on the 100-millimeter scale. The predictors were each speaker’s self- and partner-specific ratings of anxiety and collaborativeness, on the assumption that the speakers’ impressions of comprehensibility might be shaped not only by how they view their partner’s anxiety and collaborativeness (henceforth, partner-anxiety and partner-collaborativeness) but also by how the speakers perceived their own anxiety and collaborativeness (henceforth, self-anxiety and self-collaborativeness). These ratings were similarly derived per speaker at each rating episode and expressed numerically. Audio recordings of interaction were transcribed to determine each speaker’s lexical output during interaction so a content measure could be used as a covariate.
To control various potential influences on ratings of comprehensibility, anxiety, and collaborativeness over time, several covariates were retained from the original dataset. The first covariates were each speaker’s IELTS speaking and listening scores, included on the assumption that the speakers’ ratings might have reflected their own or their partners’ L2 skill level. The second covariate was a measure of type frequency, derived through lexical profiling, from each speaker’s output in each segment preceding the rating episode (i.e., before Time 1, between Time 1 and 2, and so on). This covariate was included to account for the speakers’ lexical contribution, assuming that the ratings might reflect the amount of content produced before each assessment. The final covariate was a time deviation variable, which captured each pair’s deviation from the intended rating time. Although all pairs engaged in each task for comparable amounts of time and performed repeated assessments at similar intervals (see Trofimovich et al., Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020), individual variations (ratings occurring earlier or later than intended) may have impacted them.
STATISTICAL MODELING
We used mixed-effects models to estimate relationships between anxiety and collaborativeness, the primary predictor variables of interest in this study, and comprehensibility. Mixed-effects models are especially appropriate for analyzing longitudinal data because they are robust in the face of missing data and make simpler statistical assumptions than other analyses such as ANOVA (for an overview, see Cunnings & Finlayson, Reference Cunnings, Finlayson and Plonsky2015; Linck & Cunnings, Reference Linck and Cunnings2015). Mixed-effects models are also well-suited to hierarchical data structures, where one unit is nested within a higher-order unit, such as students within classes or, in the current study, speakers within pairs. Mixed-effects models allow researchers to account for hierarchical data (i.e., to account for the fact that the students in one class or the speakers in one pair are more likely to be similar to one another than to the students in another class or the speakers in another pair) through random effects. Most importantly for our purposes, mixed-effects modeling is a more flexible statistical option that is conducive to time-varying independent variables, or independent variables that take on unique values at each point in time, such as the partner- and self-ratings of anxiety and collaborativeness that participants provided at each of the seven rating episodes.
We fit models using the lme4 package (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) in R Version 4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2020). In our previous work, we focused on the effect of time on comprehensibility ratings to determine the extent to which the speakers’ ratings of one another changed and potentially converged over time. Our final (piecewise) model included separate predictors involving a quadratic trend for time over Tasks 1–2 and a linear trend over Task 3, covariates to control for the speakers’ speaking and listening proficiency, lexical output (type frequency), and variability in the timing of repeated assessments (time deviation), as well as random intercepts for speakers and pairs. We adopted this model as the baseline model here, integrating anxiety and collaborativeness as predictors of comprehensibility while controlling for the effect of time and other covariates. To streamline the analyses, as in our earlier work, we split the data into two comparable datasets—one for Tasks 1–2 (four rating episodes) and another for Task 3 (three rating episodes)—that enabled us to explore whether the effect of anxiety and collaborativeness on comprehensibility varied across tasks, providing insight into task-induced variation between speakers’ affective state and engagement in relation to comprehensibility.
To limit model complexity, we adopted a conservative approach, integrating each of the partner- and self-ratings of anxiety and collaborativeness as predictors into the baseline model. Using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) in single-effect models, where a lower AIC indicates better fit, we then ranked the anxiety and collaborativeness predictors according to their informativeness. This ranking dictated order of entry as we evaluated more complex models. At each step, we compared model fit through likelihood ratio tests, retaining the corresponding anxiety or collaborativeness predictor only if it significantly improved fit. We opted for a simple random-effects structure consisting of random intercepts for speakers and pairs, with all fixed effects standardized.
For each final model, we computed variance inflation factors to check for multicollinearity among the predictors and plotted model residuals to confirm that they were distributed normally. All inflation values were below 2, indicating that multicollinearity was not a concern, but the residual plots showed that both models had a heavy lower tail. To correct for this excursion, we screened the datasets for residuals larger than 2.5 and refit the models to the pruned data. Following this procedure, we removed 5 of 160 observations for Tasks 1–2 and 6 of 120 observations for Task 3, which brought the distribution of residuals closer to normality (though some deviation was still observed at the tails). The final models were thus fit to 155 observations for Tasks 1–2 and 114 observations for Task 3.
RESULTS
GENERAL PATTERNS
As a first step, we plotted self and partner anxiety and collaborativeness ratings to examine change over time (with descriptive statistics summarized in online Supplementary Materials). As shown in Figure 1, anxiety and collaborativeness were near mirror images of each other; the more collaborative the speakers considered themselves and their partner to be, the less anxiety they perceived. The figure also underscores the importance of task characteristics, where dotted lines indicate a shift to the next task. During Task 2, where the speakers worked with separate images to narrate a story together, they gave low ratings for collaborativeness while indicating that they were relatively anxious. In contrast, Task 3, where the speakers discussed potential solutions to common challenges faced by international students, showed the opposite pattern, with high collaborativeness coinciding with low anxiety.
To gain insight into the relationship between these measures, we computed global correlation coefficients between the anxiety and collaborativeness ratings, pooling over data points. As shown in Table 1, anxiety and collaborativeness were negatively linked, insofar as higher collaborativeness was associated with lower anxiety. Self-self and partner-partner ratings showed moderate to large correlations, such that the speakers’ self-perceptions of anxiety and collaborativeness were strongly linked (r = −.70) as were the speakers’ judgments of their partner’s anxiety and collaborativeness (r = −.56). Although the relationships across self and partner ratings were predictably weaker, they revealed links between anxiety and collaborativeness that were codependent on the two partners.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
ANXIETY AND COLLABORATIVENESS IN TASKS 1–2
For Tasks 1–2, comparing AICs for single-predictor models revealed the following informativeness ranking for the target predictors: partner-collaborativeness, partner-anxiety, self-collaborativeness, and self-anxiety. The first three predictors improved model fit, whereas self-anxiety did not. Therefore, model building concentrated on integrating the significant factors in stepwise order of informativeness. The addition of these effects significantly improved model fit, resulting in the best-fitting model reported in Table 2 (for a summary of models fit and model comparisons, see online Supplementary Materials). The marginal R 2 (.590) for this model showed that the fixed effects alone explained 59% of the variance in comprehensibility ratings over the first two tasks. The complete model, including random effects, accounted for nearly 72% of the variance in comprehensibility (conditional R 2 = .717). According to Plonsky and Ghanbar’s (Reference Plonsky and Ghanbar2018) benchmarks for interpreting R 2 values for multiple regression, the explanatory power of this model would be considered moderate to large.
Note: The poly function was used to fit orthogonal polynomials for time. The lmerTest package was used to estimate p values. All predictors were standardized using the scale function.
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.
As shown in Table 2, the speakers’ perception of their partner’s collaborativeness emerged as the strongest predictor of comprehensibility, with the largest coefficient. The more collaborative the speakers perceived their partner to be, the higher they rated their partner’s comprehensibility. The speakers’ perception of their partner’s anxiety was also negatively linked to comprehensibility, although with a smaller coefficient indicative of a slightly weaker relationship. The more anxious the speakers perceived their partner to be, the lower they rated their partner’s comprehensibility. Finally, although the speakers’ self-rating of collaborativeness was significantly related to their partner’s comprehensibility, its contribution was much weaker, yet far from trivial, such that the speakers’ own degree of collaboration positively predicted how they viewed their partner’s comprehensibility. None of the covariates emerged as significant predictors of comprehensibility over the first two tasks.
ANXIETY AND COLLABORATIVENESS IN TASK 3
We followed the same procedure to model comprehensibility as a function of anxiety and collaborativeness in Task 3. However, for this task, single-predictor models showed that only partner-anxiety and partner-collaborativeness significantly improved the baseline model and that partner-anxiety, unlike partner-collaborativeness in Tasks 1–2, emerged as the more informative predictor. Thus, partner-anxiety was integrated into the baseline model first, followed by partner-collaborativeness. With each step, model fit improved, resulting in the best-fitting model shown in Table 3. In this model, the fixed effects explained 60% (marginal R 2 = .603) of the variance in comprehensibility and the full model with random effects approximately 65% (conditional R 2 = .647). This model would also be considered to have moderate to large explanatory power (Plonsky & Ghanbar, Reference Plonsky and Ghanbar2018).
Note: All predictors were standardized using the scale function.
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.
As shown in Table 3, the speakers’ perception of their partner’s anxiety and collaborativeness was associated with that partner’s comprehensibility, such that the less anxious and more collaborative the speakers perceived their partner to be, the higher they rated their partner’s comprehensibility. Compared to the model for Tasks 1–2, the effect of partner-anxiety remained similar (i.e., coefficients were comparable), but the effect of partner-collaborativeness decreased substantially, from 7.19 (Tasks 1–2) to 2.12 (Task 3). Most covariates remained nonsignificant, save type frequency, which was positively associated with comprehensibility, with greater type frequency in the partner’s speech in the segment immediately preceding the rating linked to a higher comprehensibility rating for that partner.
In summary, modeling demonstrated that speakers’ perception of their partner’s comprehensibility was associated with their perception of their partner’s collaborativeness and anxiety. In the first two tasks, collaborativeness was a stronger predictor than anxiety, whereas in the third task, anxiety was a stronger predictor than collaborativeness. Additionally, speakers’ perception of their partner’s comprehensibility was tied to their perception of their own collaborativeness, albeit to a lesser extent and only during the first two tasks.
DISCUSSION
As a metric of a person’s subjective experience of the ease or difficulty with which information is processed (Reber & Greifeneder, Reference Reber and Greifeneder2017), comprehensibility likely captures various influences that enhance or impair listener experience with speech. Some influences might derive from the linguistic attributes of speech, such as its lexical sophistication, grammatical complexity, or segmental and suprasegmental accuracy (Saito et al., Reference Saito, Trofimovich and Isaacs2017; Trofimovich & Isaacs, Reference Trofimovich and Isaacs2012). Other contributors to comprehensibility might stem from the clarity or coherence of the speech content, as the speaker creates discourse (Nagle et al., Reference Nagle, Trofimovich and Bergeron2019). Yet other influences on comprehensibility—some of which were explored here—might be related to interpersonal fluency (Ackerman & Bargh, Reference Ackerman, Bargh and Bruya2010), or people’s experience of effortlessness arising through social coordination. This coordination can involve behavior, such as people appropriating one another’s gestures and speech patterns (Paxton et al., Reference Paxton, Dale, Richardson, Passos, Davids and Chow2016), and affect, such as people becoming sensitive to one another’s emotional and affective states (Parkinson, Reference Parkinson2011).
Set against this backdrop, it is hardly surprising that collaborativeness and anxiety predicted comprehensibility. Conceptualized within the broader construct of engagement (Philp & Duchesne, Reference Philp and Duchesne2016), collaborativeness ratings likely reflected various behavioral dimensions of social coordination. For instance, collaborativeness may have encompassed attention to task instructions, orientation toward task completion, quality of task-relevant talk, and reciprocity of participation, in the sense that partners needed to work together to attain the task goal without surrendering or seizing full control of the interaction. Although unpacking the distinct facets of the collaborative behavior relevant to comprehensibility was not feasible in the present study, the collaborativeness-comprehensibility link is revealing, in that L2 speakers’ general perception of their partners’ task involvement has a bearing on the ease or difficulty with which they understand those partners. It is also worth noting that the role of collaborativeness in promoting comprehensibility was evident even after controlling for speakers’ lexical contribution to the conversation through the type frequency covariate, reinforcing the view that at least some aspects of collaborative behavior are distinct from and/or transcend linguistic output.
The association between anxiety and comprehensibility is a novel finding, linking comprehensibility to a socioaffective dimension of interaction. Anxiety ratings likely captured visual signs of anxious L2 speakers, such as restrained facial expressions, decreased eye contact, rigid postures, and hand movements focused on manipulating objects (e.g., clicking a pen) rather than on enhancing the meaning of speech (Gregersen, Reference Gregersen2005). Anxiety ratings may have also reflected linguistic and interactional behaviors shown by anxious speakers, including generic rather than detailed utterances, avoidance in claiming or volunteering a turn, and frequent single-syllable backchannels with nonverbal encouragement (e.g., nodding) for the interlocutor to continue talking (Ely, Reference Ely1986; Steinberg & Horwitz, Reference Steinberg and Horwitz1986). These cues, individually or combined, may have made processing the L2 speakers’ message more effortful for the interlocutor, leading to lower comprehensibility ratings.
It is important to acknowledge that the linguistic and behavioral cues of collaborativeness and anxiety that informed participants’ holistic ratings of each partner-oriented dimension likely overlap. For example, an absence of task-relevant content detail, general state of uneasiness, avoidance in claiming a turn, or lack of interest may be signs of both reduced collaborativeness and increased anxiety. It is little surprise, therefore, that the dynamic curves of anxiety and collaborativeness were mirror images of each other and that the two ratings shared up to 49% of their variance. Nonetheless, the two ratings remained sufficiently distinct, in that they predicted comprehensibility differently depending on the task. Most speakers felt that the picture narrative (Task 2) was the most difficult of the three tasks (Trofimovich et al., Reference Trofimovich, Nagle, O’Brien, Kennedy, Taylor Reid and Strachan2020). For the picture narrative task, speakers had to reconstruct a coherent, shared narrative from 14 scrambled images, which required close collaboration from both partners. Because collaboration was task-essential, it makes sense that collaborativeness was a stronger predictor of comprehensibility than anxiety. In contrast, for the discussion task focusing on the shared, lived experiences of international students adjusting to life in a new environment (Task 3), collaboration was less critical, insofar as every speaker had ample input to contribute, which could explain why anxiety emerged as a stronger predictor than collaborativeness. Additionally, in Task 3, speakers often discussed personally relevant emotional themes (e.g., culture shock), which likely heightened the task’s socioaffective load, resulting in stronger links between comprehensibility and anxiety than between comprehensibility and collaborativeness. At the same time, these task-related findings should be interpreted with caution given that all pairs completed the tasks in a fixed order, which means that we could not separate the effects of task and time in the current analysis. To arrive at a full understanding of how socioaffective variables change depending on the task and the amount of time spent with a particular interlocutor, it would be necessary to counterbalance the order of tasks across pairs.
Finally, L2 speakers’ judgments of their partner’s comprehensibility were predicted by the speakers’ own behavior, namely, their collaborativeness. Although this relationship emerged only for Tasks 1–2, it nonetheless implies that a speaker’s comprehensibility—as assessed in dialogue—is coconstructed by both interacting partners. Put differently, a speaker’s comprehensibility may reflect not only that speaker’s linguistic and nonlinguistic behaviors but may also encompass the interlocutor’s contributions to the dialogue. This relationship might reflect the halo effect, whereby speakers project a positive image of themselves on their partner, whose comprehensibility they are assessing. Alternatively, it might arise because people often misattribute their assessment of ease or difficulty to an irrelevant source (Greifeneder et al., Reference Greifeneder, Bless and Pham2011), which, in this case, amounts to speakers upgrading their partner’s comprehensibility based on their own participation in dialogue. Regardless of its source, this self-oriented influence on partner comprehensibility in interaction represents a novel contribution to existing work, which to date has chiefly targeted individual differences in raters’ cognitive and experiential profiles (e.g., Saito et al., Reference Saito, Tran, Suzukida, Sun, Magne and Ilkan2019).
CONCLUSION
To conclude, this exploratory study revealed links between interlocutor-rated comprehensibility and affective (anxiety) and behavioral (collaborativeness) dimensions of interaction. For anxiety, this study extended prior work, where anxiety is typically rated retrospectively while speakers view their recorded performances in monologic tasks (Gregersen et al., Reference Gregersen, MacIntyre and Meza2014), into an interactive domain, with both interlocutors evaluating their own and their partner’s anxiety. For collaborativeness, the study allowed for tracking speaker participation on a minute-by-minute timescale, which complements previous longitudinal work (Oga-Baldwin & Nakata, Reference Oga-Baldwin and Nakata2017). Despite their promise, these findings must be revisited in future work. First, because the residuals in this study’s mixed-effects models deviated from normality even after outlier cases had been removed, it would be important to replicate the present findings before making broader generalizations about the role of anxiety and collaborativeness in L2 comprehensibility. Similarly, in follow-up work, researchers could also target speakers of different proficiency levels engaged in other tasks (whose order must be rotated across speaker dyads) and employ other measures of collaborativeness (e.g., turn-taking frequency) and anxiety (e.g., galvanic skin response). Lastly, linking comprehensibility to various facets of task engagement and anxiety requires an understanding of whether speakers notice and use the cues that signal their partner’s collaborativeness and anxiety. Such insight can be gained through stimulated recall, (video) observation, or eye-tracking. Online teaching and research environments may be particularly conducive to designs similar to ours, where partners are asked to periodically evaluate one another as they work through a set of communicative tasks. Above all, researchers should intensify work exploring links between speech assessments and various social, affective, and behavioral measures to clarify the multidimensional nature of L2 communication in both face-to-face and virtual environments. This work should prioritize interactive approaches to L2 communication, given that the relationships between linguistic features and speech ratings that have been documented using monologic speaking tasks may not hold during interaction, when a wider range of time-varying affective and behavioral influences are at play.
Supplementary Materials
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263121000073.