No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Metarepresentation, trust, and “unleashed expression”
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 17 February 2023
Abstract
Heintz & Scott-Phillips's account of human expression leaves a number of central issues unclear – not least, whether the lack of expression in nonhuman species is attributable to their lack of the relevant metarepresentational abilities, an absence of trust, or a consequence of other factors. In place of their view, we propose a gradualistic account of the origins of human expression.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Andrews, K., & Monsó, S. (2021). Animal cognition. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2021 Edition).Google Scholar
Bar-On, D., & Moore, R. (2017). Pragmatic interpretation and signaler-receiver asymmetries in animal communication. In K. Andrews, & J. Beck, (Eds.), Routledge handbook of philosophy of animal minds (pp. 291–300). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berio, L. (2021a). Culturally embedded schemata for false belief reasoning. Synthese, 199(1), 285–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berio, L. (2021b). Talking about thinking: Language, thought, and mentalizing, Epistemic Studies (Vol. 49). De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Berio, L., & Moore, R. (Unpublished manuscript). Great ape enculturation studies: a neglected resource in cognitive development research.Google Scholar
Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A., Singer, T., & Steinbeis, N. (2017). Implicit and explicit false belief development in preschool children. Developmental Science, 20(5), e12445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2014). The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology, 24(14), 1596–1600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurley, S. (2003). Animal action in the space of reasons. Mind & Language, 18(3), 231–257.Google Scholar
Laland, K., & Seed, A. (2021). Understanding human cognitive uniqueness. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 689–716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Low, J. (2010). Preschoolers’ implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: Relations with complex syntactical mastery. Child Development, 81(2), 597–615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyn, H., Russell, J. L., & Hopkins, W. D. (2010). The impact of environment on the comprehension of declarative communication in apes. Psychological Science, 21(3), 360–365.Google ScholarPubMed
Moore, R. (2014). Ape gestures: Interpreting chimpanzee and bonobo minds. Current Biology, 24(14), R645–R647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, R. (2016). Meaning and ostension in great ape gestural communication. Animal Cognition, 19(1), 223–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, R. (2017a). Gricean communication and cognitive development. The Philosophical Quarterly, 67(267), 303–326.Google Scholar
Moore, R. (2017b). Convergent minds: Ostension, inference and Grice's third clause. Interface Focus, 7(3), 20160107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, R. (2017c). Social cognition, stag hunts, and the evolution of language. Biology & Philosophy, 32(6), 797–818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, R. (2021). The cultural evolution of mind-modelling. Synthese, 199(1), 1751–1776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006). How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task. Animal Cognition, 9(3), 193–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newen, A., & Starzak, T. (2020). How to ascribe beliefs to animals. Mind & Language, 37(1), 3–21.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Shanker, S., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. (2014). Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C., & Fischer, J. (2012). Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21(5), 195–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Expression unleashed: The evolutionary and cognitive foundations of human communication
Related commentaries (18)
Cognitive pragmatics: Insights from homesign conversations
Expression unleashed in artificial intelligence
From the pragmatics of charades to the creation of language
Illustrating continuity between linguistic and non-linguistic human communication and expression
Loosening the leash: The unique emotional canvas of human screams
Metarepresentation, trust, and “unleashed expression”
No unleashed expression without language
On the murky dissociation between expression and communication
Ostensive communication, market exchange, mindshaping, and elephants
Primates unleashed
Putting the cart before the horse? The origin of information donation
Structuring unleashed expression: Developmental foundations of human communication
Teaching unleashes expression
The central problem is still evolutionary stability
The co-evolution of cooperation and communication: Alternative accounts
The evolutionary roots of goal-directed mechanisms: A communication account
The scaffolded evolution of human communication
What semantic dementia tells us about the ability to infer others' communicative intentions
Author response
Being ostensive (reply to commentaries on “Expression unleashed”)