Psychosis is a severe, chronic mental illness of unknown aetiology that typically begins in late adolescence or early adulthood. Onset before 18 years of age is termed early-onset psychosis, and is commonly characterised by atypical psychotic symptoms, highly implicit cognitive impairment, more severe disease forms and poor prognosis.Reference Remschmidt and Theisen1 Cognitive control deficits are one of the main symptoms of psychosis.Reference Fassbender, Scangos, Lesh and Carter2 Cognitive control is a goal-oriented, flexible and timely process of adjusting intentions and actions,Reference Folstein and Van Petten3,Reference Gratton4 including inhibitory control, working memory updating and cognitive flexibility. Cognitive control develops throughout childhood until near maturity in adolescence. Previous studies have shown that cognitive control deficits in psychosis are associated with decreased prefrontal cortex and frontal parietal network involvement.Reference Tu, Su, Lin, Chang WC and Li CT5 Understanding the neural mechanisms leading to cognitive control deficits is thus a key step for early intervention and the development of targeted therapies for psychosis.
Electroencephalogram changes in psychosis with cognitive control deficits
In recent years, electroencephalogram has been used to examine the neural mechanisms of cognitive control.Reference Jia, Qin, Cui, Shi, Ye and Yang6,Reference Viviani and Vallesi7 N2 is a negative event-related potential (ERP) component that appears approximately 200–300 ms after stimulation, usually in the central prefrontal region.Reference Folstein and Van Petten3 N2 is thought to reflect top-down cognitive processes, which are likely attributed to the perception of novel stimuli and the orientation of visual attention in cognitive control regulation.Reference Folstein and Van Petten3 Reduced N2 amplitude was found in patients with a variety of psychiatric disorders, including psychosis, which may reflect cognitive control dysfunction in patients with psychosis.Reference Vignapiano, Mucci, Merlotti, Giordano GM and Palumbo D8
In recent years, increasing studies have explored the local and distributed neural circuit mechanisms underlying ERP impairment through time-frequency analysis.Reference Javitt9 Large-scale neuronal integration is formed by synchronisation or de-synchronisation of neurons in different frequency bands, also termed event-related synchronisation (ERS)/event-related de-synchronisation and neural oscillation, which represent increases or decreases in the power of a specific frequency band when an event occurs. This process is related to the neural mechanisms of cognitive activity,Reference Lee, Lindquist and Nam10 and allows rapid and efficient communication between neurons in the brain. Theta band (4–7 Hz) oscillations are involved in cognitive control and top-down processes,Reference Jiang, Zhang and van Gaal11 such as the encoding of working memory.Reference Jacobs, Hwang, Curran and Kahana12 Alpha band (8–13 Hz) oscillations are associated with the distribution of attention,Reference Deng, Choi and Shinn-Cunningham13 and reflect processes related to inhibitory functions of the brain.Reference Schneider, Göddertz, Haase, Hickey and Wascher14
Abnormal theta oscillations have been reported in patients with psychosis. For example, one study used the Stroop word interference task and found that patients with psychosis have a significantly smaller increase in theta oscillations in the left frontal and parietal regions than controls.Reference Popov, Wienbruch, Meissner, Miller and Rockstroh15 Another study of event-related spectral perturbations of misprediction tasks found that theta oscillations of the prefrontal lobe were significantly reduced in patients with psychosis compared with controls.Reference Roa Romero, Keil, Balz, Gallinat and Senkowski16 Furthermore, patients with psychosis show abnormal alpha oscillations. For example, during a self-referential task, people with schizophrenia showed abnormally decreased alpha oscillations in the prefrontal, parietal and occipital regions 100–300 ms after stimulation.Reference Jia, Liu, Huang, Zhao Y and Go R17 Attention-related regulation of alpha activity is also significantly impaired during visual processing tasks in people with schizophrenia.Reference Martínez, Gaspar, Hillyard, Bickel S and Dias EC18
The current study
There is an emerging idea that the basic patterns based on neural oscillations associated with cognitive control are already present in early adolescence.Reference Buzzell, Barker, Troller-Renfree, Bernat EM and Morales S19 However, as previous studies on cognitive control in psychosis have focused on adults with a chronic disease course, it is unclear whether neurobiological impairments in cognitive control occur in children and adolescents with first-episode psychosis (FEP) or earlier stages of the disease, i.e. clinical high-risk (CHR) state for psychosis, which is characterised by attenuated symptoms of psychosis, impairment of social function and a higher risk of conversion to psychosis spectrum disorder (approximately 30% within 2 years).Reference Fusar-Poli, Bonoldi, Yung, Borgwardt S and Valmaggia L20 To address this research gap, we investigated N2 amplitude characteristics in unmedicated children and adolescents with FEP or CHR, specifically the characteristics of theta and alpha oscillations, to better understand the neuroelectrophysiological basis of cognitive control impairment in early-onset psychosis.
Method
Participants
From August 2018 to April 2021, 48 patients with FEP (aged 12–17 years) and 24 patients with CHR (aged 10–17 years) were recruited from the psychiatric clinic and ward of the Second Xiangya Hospital in Hunan Province, China. Patients were diagnosed according to the DSM-5Reference Gnanavel and Robert21 criteria for FEP and Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes (SIPS)Reference Miller, McGlashan, Rosen, Cadenhead K and Ventura J.22 criteria for CHR. SIPS has been used to identify different types of CHR, which classifies CHR into three types: attenuated positive symptom syndrome, transient intermittent psychotic syndrome and genetic risk and degenerative syndrome.Reference Miller, McGlashan, Rosen, Cadenhead K and Ventura J.22Additionally, 42 healthy controls aged 10–17 years were recruited by advertising in schools. Psychiatric symptoms in all patients were assessed by a trained psychiatrist with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler23 None of the patients received antipsychotic drugs or other therapy before participating in this study. All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected normal vision. The exclusion criteria for patients included a history of other mental diseases, any central nervous system disease, intellectual disability, serious physical diseases, alcohol or drug misuse, or inability to cooperate with the study protocol. For healthy controls, the exclusion criteria were any history of mental or neurological disorders (confirmed by an interview with a trained psychiatrist using the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Psychosis – Present and Lifetime Version),Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao U and Moreci P24 intellectual disability, alcohol or drug misuse, or family history of mental illness within three generations.
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, China (approval number: 2018030). All participants and their parents/guardians were voluntary and provided written consent.
Experimental paradigm
The experiment was conducted by a professionally trained psychiatrist in a quiet, soundproof electroencephalogram room, maintained at a constant temperature. Before the beginning of the experiment, the researchers ensured that the participants fully understood the experimental process. Participants were seated 60 cm away from the computer display and instructed to look directly at the centre of the screen. The visual oddball paradigm was designed and presented with E-prime 2.0 software for Windows (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA; see https://pstnet.com/products/e-prime/). Participants were instructed to press the ‘F’ or ‘J’ key in response to the random standard stimulus (letter ‘W’, probability of 80%) and target stimulus (letter ‘m’, probability of 20%), respectively. The paradigm was divided into three blocks: the first block comprised 15 practice trials, and the next two blocks comprised the formal experiment of 125 trials each. At the beginning of the experiment, a blank screen was presented for 500–1500 ms, followed by a black ‘+’ symbol for 500 ms to remind the participants to concentrate, and then 2000 ms of stimulation.
Electroencephalogram preprocessing and ERP recording
Electroencephalogram data were preprocessed using EEGLAB in the MATLAB platform for Windows (R2013b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA; see https://www.mathworks.com). The sampling rate of electroencephalogram data was first reduced to 500 Hz, and then filtered (high-pass filtering >0.1 Hz, low-pass filtering <30 Hz and notch filtering from 48 to 52 Hz). Segmentation was performed from 500 ms before the stimulus to 800 ms after the stimulus, and only corrected trials were kept. Next, we eliminated bad segments and interpolated bad leads, using the spherical interpolation method to interpolate abnormal leads. Independent component analysis was used to remove artifacts such as eye movements, electrical components and oscillation frequency interference. Electrodes at the bilateral mastoid (TP9/TP10) were used as reference electrodes. Finally, extreme values (voltage amplitude exceeding ±100 μV) were removed. All segments were superimposed and averaged to obtain the ERP waveform. The time window for N2 was determined from a previous studyReference Dayan-Riva and Berger25 and inspection of the ERP grand averages. In this way, the N2 amplitude was defined as the average amplitude between 200 and 350 ms after the stimulus. N2 was detected in the frontal region where the N2 amplitude is greatest. The following four regions of interest (ROIs) were selected for analysis: ROI1 (F1, Fz, F2), ROI2 (C1, Cz, C2), ROI3 (FC1, FC3, FC5) and ROI4 (FC2, FC4, FC6). These ROIs correspond to the frontal, central, left fronto-central and right fronto-central brain regions, respectively.
Time-frequency analysis
Theta band (4–7 Hz) and alpha band (8–13 Hz) oscillations were only analysed in correct trials. Theta and alpha oscillations were calculated by using MATLAB to apply the short-time Fourier transform to individual trials, and electroencephalogram data were converted into the time-frequency domain. Theta and alpha oscillations were measured for each participant, in ROI1, ROI2, ROI3 and ROI4. The frequency range was set to 0.1–30 Hz in 0.5 Hz steps. As the time-frequency transformation results were not perfectly accurate at the beginning and end of trials, 350–150 ms before the stimulus was used as the baseline range. The time-frequency plots after baseline correction were obtained by subtracting the average oscillations of the baseline period from the power of each time-frequency point after stimulation. We calculated the average theta and alpha oscillations in the time window of 200–350 ms after the start of stimulation. The time-frequency characteristics of electroencephalogram were visualised by time-frequency analysis and topographic maps.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS version 22.0 (IBM, Chicago, Illinois, USA). Quantitative variables are expressed as the mean ± s.d. and median (third quartile, first quartile), whereas categorical variables are expressed as the frequency (n) and composition ratio. Continuous data were compared between the three groups, using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Chi-squared test was used for comparison of categorical data. The Kruskal–Wallis rank-sum test was used to compare data that did not conform to normal distribution or homogeneity of variance. Three-factor repeated measure ANOVA was used to examine the effects group (FEP, CHR, healthy controls) × ROI (ROI1, ROI2, ROI3, ROI4) × stimulus type (standard, target) on N2 amplitude, theta oscillations and alpha oscillations. Greenhouse–Geisser correction for sphericity was applied. Bonferroni correction was applied for pairwise comparisons.
Results
Participant characteristics
Table 1 presents the general demographic data and clinical characteristics of the FEP, CHR and healthy control groups. For the FEP and CHR groups, the duration of illness and PANSS score were provided. For the CHR group, the Scale of Psychosis-Risk Syndromes Positive, Negative, Disorganized and General symptom total scores were also provided. Post hoc analyses revealed no significant differences between any two groups (P > 0.05), despite the Kruskal-Wallis rank-sum test indicating an age difference between the three groups (P = 0.030). Age was therefore excluded as a covariate in the analyses that followed. There was no significant difference in gender composition among the three groups (P = 0.477).
Data are represented as the mean ± s.d. or median (third quartile, first quartile). FEP, first-episode psychosis; CHR, clinical high risk; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; SOPS, Scale of Psychosis-Risk Syndromes.
a. Post hoc tests showed no statistically significant difference.
N2 amplitude
As shown in Table 2, the group × region × stimulus interaction effect was not statistically significant for N2 amplitude (F(6,220) = 1.393, P = 0.219). However, the main effect of stimulus type was significant (F(1,111) = 14.922, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the N2 amplitude evoked by the target stimulus was significantly greater than that evoked by the standard stimulus. The main effect of brain region (ROI1, ROI2, ROI3, ROI4) was also significant (F(3,109) = 40.500, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the N2 amplitude in the frontal region was the highest, being significantly higher than that in the central and other regions (P < 0.001). There was no significant main effect of group (P = 0.099).
FEP, first-episode psychosis; CHR, clinical high risk; ROI, region of interest.
*** P < 0.001.
Theta oscillation
Theta oscillations were increased relative to baseline in the frontal, central, left fronto-central and right fronto-central regions in all three groups (Fig. 1). The group × region × stimulus interaction on theta oscillation was not significant (F(6,220) = 0.864, P = 0.522; Table 3). The main effect of stimulus type was significant (F(1,111) = 44.116, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the theta oscillation induced by the target stimulus was significantly larger than that induced by the standard stimulus. The main effect of brain region (ROI1, ROI2, ROI3, ROI4) was also significant (F(3,109) = 78.246, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that theta oscillation in the frontal region was the significantly larger than that in the central region (P < 0.001), and that the theta oscillation in the left fronto-central region was significantly larger than that in the right fronto-central region (P < 0.001). The main effect of group was significant. Post hoc tests showed that theta oscillation in healthy controls was significantly larger than that in the FEP group (P < 0.001). However, theta oscillations in the CHR group were not significantly different from those in the healthy control and FEP groups (P > 0.05). Topographic maps of the theta oscillations in the three groups to the target and standard stimuli are shown in Fig. 2.
FEP, first-episode psychosis; CHR, clinical high risk; ROI, region of interest.
*** P < 0.001.
Alpha oscillation
Alpha oscillations were increased relative to baseline in the frontal, central, left fronto-central and right fronto-central areas in all three groups (Fig. 1). The group × region × stimulus interaction effect was not statistically significant (F(6,220) = 1.016, P = 0.416; as shown in Table 4). However, the main effect of brain region (ROI1, ROI2, ROI3, ROI4) was significant (F(3,109) = 20.582, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the alpha oscillation in the left fronto-central region was significantly larger than that in the right fronto-central region (P < 0.001), and that the alpha oscillation in the frontal region was significantly larger than that in central region (P < 0.001). The main effect of group was also significant. Post hoc tests showed that alpha oscillations in healthy controls were significantly higher than that in the FEP group (P < 0.01). Alpha oscillations in the CHR group were not significantly different from those in the healthy control and FEP groups (P > 0.05). Topographic maps of the alpha oscillations in the three groups to the target and standard stimuli are shown in Fig. 3.
FEP, first-episode psychosis; CHR, clinical high risk; ROI, region of interest.
*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
Discussion
In the present study, there was no difference in the amplitude of N2 in the frontal and parietal lobes between the patient and control groups. Previous studies of the characteristics of N2 amplitude in patients with psychosis have reported mixed results. For example, some studies suggest that deficits in the fronto-central N2 amplitude in patients with psychosis are related to specific deficits in neural functions related to recognising complex stimuli.Reference Doniger, Foxe, Murray, Higgins and Javitt26,Reference Klein, Shekels, McGuire and Sponheim27 In contrast, another study found that the N2 amplitude cannot be used to differentiate patients with FEP from controls.Reference Demiralp, Uçok, Devrim, Isoglu-Alkaç, Tecer and Polich28 However, as a decreased N2 amplitude was found in patients with a chronic course of psychosis,Reference Ogura, Nageishi, Matsubayashi, Omura, Kishimoto and Shimokochi29 it can be interpreted that a decreased N2 amplitude is related to the chronic course of the disease, and worsens with disease development. It is reasonable to assume that, during the early stages of psychosis, damage to N2-related neurons is minor, and the alterations in the time-domain amplitude reflected by ERP are insufficient to be observed. Our results also support the viewpoint that N2 amplitude is not a reliable biomarker of early-onset psychosis.Reference Kropotov, Ponomarev, Hollup and Mueller30
This study did find that the theta-ERS after stimulus in the frontal, central, left fronto-central and right fronto-central regions in the FEP group was significantly smaller than that in healthy controls, which is consistent with previous research on adults with psychosis.Reference Cooper and Hughes31 In one such study, when the cognitive abilities required by the task increased, adult patients with psychosis did not show any load-dependent increase in theta oscillations in the fronto-posterior region.Reference Berger, Minarik, Griesmayr, Stelzig-Schoeler, Aichhorn and Sauseng32 In a study using a working memory paradigm, healthy controls showed increased theta-ERS in the frontal central region as task difficulty increased, whereas adult patients with psychosis showed a decreased theta-ERS in the frontal lobe under all difficult task conditions.Reference Schmiedt, Brand, Hildebrandt and Basar-Eroglu33 In the present study, we found impaired theta-ERS in adolescents with FEP. During simple tasks, the decreased theta oscillations is attributable to the advantages of automatic processing and the lower need for intrusive executive control functions; in contrast, the increased theta oscillations is attributable to the greater impact of interference as the task load increases, and the need for stronger executive control functions to suppress automatic processing and preparation dependent on expected responses.Reference Berger, Minarik, Griesmayr, Stelzig-Schoeler, Aichhorn and Sauseng32 Thus, a possible explanation for decreased theta-ERS in adolescents with FEP is the absence of anticipatory guided action plans and the inability to monitor self-priming behaviours and predict upcoming events. However, theta oscillatory alterations in FEP are difficult to explain at present, because the neurobiological mechanisms behind low-frequency oscillations in psychosis are not as easily identified as those behind high-frequency oscillations, such as gamma oscillation.
Furthermore, alpha-ERS in the frontal, central, left fronto-central and right fronto-central regions in the FEP group were significantly smaller than that in the healthy controls. Alpha-ERS is thought to reflect active processing associated with memory maintenance or top-down inhibition functions.Reference Palva and Palva34 A significant reduction in alpha-ERS in the right parietal midline and bilateral occipital areas has been previously observed in patients with psychosis.Reference Fujimoto, Okumura, Takeuchi, Kodabashi, Tanaka and Otsubo35 Deficits in alpha oscillation in psychosis may be related to abnormal thalamic-cortical circuitry or cortical-cortical connectivity.Reference Sauseng, Klimesch, Gruber, Hanslmayr, Freunberger and Doppelmayr36 Alternatively, it has been suggested that deficits in alpha oscillation in patients with psychosis reflect deficits in attention allocation and visual information processing.Reference Martínez, Gaspar, Hillyard, Bickel S and Dias EC18,Reference Kustermann, Rockstroh, Kienle, Miller and Popov37 It should be noted that alpha-ERS in the left fronto-central region was significantly larger than that in the right fronto-central region in our study. A possible explanation for this result is the frontal alpha asymmetry. A recent study found that frontal alpha asymmetry was significantly lower in patients with psychosis than in healthy controls, suggesting relatively lower activation of left frontal electrodes.Reference Jang, Lee, Lee, Huh and Chae38 However, since all participants in this study were right-handed, it is unclear whether this reflects differences between the left and right hemispheres.
Although there were no significant differences in theta and alpha oscillations in the CHR group compared with healthy controls and the FEP group, there was a tendency for impaired neural oscillations in the CHR group compared with healthy controls, and impairment was not as significant as that in patients with FEP. The smaller sample size in the CHR group may explain the failure to detect significant nerve oscillation damage in this group. Nevertheless, this study provides the first evidence of potential alteration from low-frequency oscillations in CHR individuals. Since some individuals may show relevant structural and functional brain damage at the CHR stage of psychosis,Reference Sun, Fang, Shi, Wang, Peng and Tan39 follow-up of the prognosis of this population will be helpful for identifying biomarkers of the clinical stage of psychosis, clarifying the development of psychosis and developing appropriate treatment for those in need.
The following limitations of this study must be noted. First, we used a relatively simple oddball paradigm in this study to ensure that participants with severe psychotic symptoms could still complete the tasks. Second, as the sample size of this study was limited, the results should be interpreted with caution. Third, as this was a cross-sectional study, we cannot identify symptom development, disease outcomes or whether electroencephalogram results change with disease outcomes. Follow-up is needed to provide neurobiological evidence of whether neural oscillations can predict disease outcome.
In conclusion, having ruled out the effects of antipsychotic use and chronic disease duration, this study provides clear evidence of alterations in theta and alpha oscillations in frontal and central regions in children and adolescents with FEP, and a trend of related changes in patients with CHR. The early stages of psychosis is already characterised by functional abnormalities in the brain (abnormal neuroregulation mediated by N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors, for instance, is considered to be a key component in the pathophysiology of psychosisReference Friston, Brown, Siemerkus and Stephan40). This study further confirms that abnormal rhythmic activity of neurons may constitute the pathophysiological mechanism of cognitive dysfunction related to early-onset psychosis.
Data availability
The supporting data for this study are accessible upon request from the corresponding author, X.C. However, please note that the data cannot be made publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.
Acknowledgements
We thank all parents, children and adolescents for their continuous support and participation in our studies.
Author contributions
Y.Z. was responsible for data collection, data management, data analyses and manuscript writing, and was a key contributing author of the manuscript. T.Y. and Y.H. were responsible for data collection and assisted with manuscript revisions. F.M., K.Z. and X.J. assisted with patient recruitment and contributed to the authorship of the paper. X.C. and X.L. were responsible for management and oversight of the study, manuscript revisions and conducted clinical evaluations of symptom severity for this study. All authors contributed to and approved of the final manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by National Key R&D Program of China (grant number 2017YFC1309904), the Hunan Province Innovation Province Construction Project (grant number 2019SK2334) and the Hunan Provincial Innovation Foundation for Postgraduate (grant number CX20190159).
Declaration of interest
None.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.