No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
An extension of the novelty-seeking model: Considering the plurality of novelty types and their differential interactions with memory
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 21 May 2024
Abstract
The novelty-seeking model suggests that curiosity and creativity originate from novelty processes. However, different types of novelty exist, each with distinctive relationships with memory, which potentially influence curiosity and creativity in distinct ways. We thus propose expanding the NSM model to consider these different novelty types and their specific involvement in creativity.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Bastin, C., Delhaye, E., Moulin, C., & Barbeau, E. J. (2019). Novelty processing and memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 237–249. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Chen, Q., Christensen, A. P., Kenett, Y. N., Silvia, P. J., Benedek, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2020) Default network contributions to episodic and semantic processing during divergent creative thinking: A representational similarity analysis. NeuroImage, 209, 116499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brod, G., Lindenberger, U., Werkle-Bergner, M., & Shing, Y. L. (2015). Differences in the neural signature of remembering schema-congruent and schema-incongruent events. NeuroImage, 117, 358–366. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, M. C., Kurczek, J., Rubin, R., Cohen, N. J., & Tranel, D. (2013). Hippocampal amnesia disrupts creative thinking. Hippocampus, 23(12), 1143–1149. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duszkiewicz, A. J., McNamara, C. G., Takeuchi, T., & Genzel, L. (2019). Novelty and dopaminergic modulation of memory persistence: A tale of two systems. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(2), 102–114. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, D., & Kafkas, A. (2021). Expectation-driven novelty effects in episodic memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 183, 107466. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107466CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kafkas, A., & Montaldi, D. (2018). How do memory systems detect and respond to novelty? Neuroscience Letters, 680, 60–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.01.053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quent, J. A., Greve, A., & Henson, R. N. (2022). Shape of U: The nonmonotonic relationship between object–location memory and expectedness. Psychological Science, 33(12), 2084–2097. doi: 10.1177/09567976221109134CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quent, J. A., Henson, R. N., & Greve, A. (2021). A predictive account of how novelty influences declarative memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 179, 107382. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramey, M. M., & Zabelina, D. (2021). Creative people use memory differently: Divergent thinking modulates how episodic memory is combined with semantic knowledge. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/fgkwqCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Kesteren, M. T., Ruiter, D. J., Fernández, G., & Henson, R. N. (2012). How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends in Neurosciences, 35(4), 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
You have
Access
The innovative novelty-seeking model (NSM) by Ivancovsky et al. states that novelty-seeking processes are at the root of curiosity and creativity. Ivancovsky et al. distinguish four phases in their NSM model. Firstly, affinity, and therefore attention to new internal or external stimuli, generates new combinations within the existing semantic knowledge network. Secondly, this new associative representation is activated, and thirdly, when the representation exceeds the salience threshold, the evaluation of its relevance takes place. Fourthly, the new relevant representations are consolidated in the semantic memory network. In this commentary, we would like to comment on the role of memory, a significant component of the NSM, since the authors stress its importance by stating that their model originates and ends in memory.
By stating that the NSM starts in memory, the authors refer to the activation of prior knowledge. However, they also mention a role for episodic memory, which appears to be unclear. By definition, detecting novelty relies on distinguishing the new from the familiar. Novelty processing is thus closely tied to prior knowledge, which is of great interest because prior knowledge forms the groundwork for determining the types of new information that need to be best consolidated in memory. Recent studies (Quent, Greve, & Henson, Reference Quent, Greve and Henson2022) have shown that the link between memory consolidation of new information and their congruency with prior knowledge is a non-linear U-shape function: Highly congruent and highly incongruent information will be remembered better than less (in)congruent. Note that, for a long time, the two ends of this U-shape have been studied as two distinct fields of research: The congruency effect in memory on the one hand and the surprise effect on memory on the other.
Interestingly, information at the two extremities of the U-shape activates different brain regions at encoding (van Kesteren, Ruiter, Fernández, & Henson, Reference van Kesteren, Ruiter, Fernández and Henson2012): While processing information, the medial prefrontal cortex either triggers rapid learning of schema-congruent information into the neocortex or activates the hippocampus in the medial temporal lobe to encode schema-incongruent events. However, differences in memories are also suspected at retrieval as remembering incongruent events engages the network for source memory (Brod, Lindenberger, Werkle-Bergner, & Shing, Reference Brod, Lindenberger, Werkle-Bergner and Shing2015). We, therefore, hypothesize that the two ends of the U-shape are linked to different memory systems: While congruent information is supposed to be stored as semantic representations, incongruent information is thought to induce episodic memories – that are memories of unique personal past experiences with their spatiotemporal context. Even more interestingly, the two tales of the U-shape are likely to rely on different novelty types. As the current literature on novelty tends to revise terminology and identify different subtypes of what is generally understood under the term “novelty” (e.g., Bastin, Delhaye, Moulin, & Barbeau, Reference Bastin, Delhaye, Moulin and Barbeau2019; Kafkas & Montaldi, Reference Kafkas and Montaldi2018), this is another prominent point to which we would like to draw attention. Since prior knowledge determines novelty type and further modulates how novelty impacts memory (Frank & Kafkas, Reference Frank and Kafkas2021), we believe that considering different subtypes of novelty and memory systems is relevant for the NSM.
By citing the work of Duszkiewicz, McNamara, Takeuchi, and Genzel (Reference Duszkiewicz, McNamara, Takeuchi and Genzel2019), Ivancovsky et al. briefly evoke the existence of different types of novelty and the potential role of episodic memory. Specifically, Duszkiewicz et al. (Reference Duszkiewicz, McNamara, Takeuchi and Genzel2019) proposed that novel experiences classified as common novelty, which share similarities with congruent past experiences, facilitate the formation of semantic memories in contrast to experiences classified as distinct novelty, which have minimal connections with past experiences and result in the creation of contextualized specific episodic memories. However, we regret that these aspects have yet to be integrated further into the NSM, which primarily focuses on the semantic network and does not differentiate novelty types. Indeed, different types of novelty modulate the environment's uncertainty and memory encoding and consolidation differently (Quent, Henson, & Greve, Reference Quent, Henson and Greve2021) – the processes pointed out by the authors as cognitive underpinnings of creativity and curiosity.
Although we agree with Ivancovsky et al.'s central claim that novelty-seeking processes mediate curiosity and creativity, we call for a distinction between different types of novelty that could interact differently with curiosity or creativity and lead to memory representations of different natures. Specifically, if both common and distinct novelty are known to attract attention and thus lead to a curiosity state, their respective link with creativity may seem less straightforward. On the one hand, common novelty fits the typical situation described by the authors: The new information congruent with prior knowledge will be combined with prior semantic knowledge to fill an existing gap within the associative semantic network. On the other hand, distinct novelty, a good candidate for creativity as its high incongruence naturally provides novel original associations, is thought to induce episodic memories. Although most of the research on creativity has focused on semantic memory, researchers have recently demonstrated the role of episodic memory in creative processes (Beaty et al., Reference Beaty, Chen, Christensen, Kenett, Silvia, Benedek and Schacter2020) as well as its interaction with semantic memory during divergent thinking (Ramey & Zabelina, Reference Ramey and Zabelina2021).
To conclude, given that semantic and episodic memory systems may be linked to different types of novelty, and given that novelty is at the heart of the NSM model, we would suggest adding the impact of episodic memory in the NSM. This addition will help increase the completeness of the model to make it generalizable to as many novelty-seeking situations as possible. This would also have implications for the consideration of clinical perspectives. Beyond psychopathology, the NSM could also address the case of brain-damaged patients with memory problems and the fact that they present decreased creativity (Duff, Kurczek, Rubin, Cohen, & Tranel, Reference Duff, Kurczek, Rubin, Cohen and Tranel2013).
Financial support
This work was supported by the F.R.S.-FNRS (C. B. is a senior research associate at F.R.S.-FNRS).
Competing interest
None.