Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T18:34:49.094Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A guide for planning triangulation studies to investigate complex causal questions in behavioural and psychiatric research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2024

Jorien L. Treur*
Affiliation:
Genetic Epidemiology, Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Eva Lukas
Affiliation:
Genetic Epidemiology, Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Hannah M. Sallis
Affiliation:
Centre for Academic Mental Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Department of Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Medical Research Council (MRC) Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Robyn E. Wootton
Affiliation:
Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Nic Waals Institute, Oslo, Norway MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK PsychGen Centre for Genetic Epidemiology and Mental Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
*
Corresponding author: Jorien L - Treur; Email: j.l.treur@amsterdamumc.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

At the basis of many important research questions is causality – does X causally impact Y? For behavioural and psychiatric traits, answering such questions can be particularly challenging, as they are highly complex and multifactorial. ‘Triangulation’ refers to prospectively choosing, conducting and integrating several methods to investigate a specific causal question. If different methods, with different sources of bias, all indicate a causal effect, the finding is much less likely to be spurious. While triangulation can be a powerful approach, its interpretation differs across (sub)fields and there are no formal guidelines. Here, we aim to provide clarity and guidance around the process of triangulation for behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology, so that results of existing triangulation studies can be better interpreted, and new triangulation studies better designed.

Methods

We first introduce the concept of triangulation and how it is applied in epidemiological investigations of behavioural and psychiatric traits. Next, we put forth a systematic step-by-step guide, that can be used to design a triangulation study (accompanied by a worked example). Finally, we provide important general recommendations for future studies.

Results

While the literature contains varying interpretations, triangulation generally refers to an investigation that assesses the robustness of a potential causal finding by explicitly combining different approaches. This may include multiple types of statistical methods, the same method applied in multiple samples, or multiple different measurements of the variable(s) of interest. In behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology, triangulation commonly includes prospective cohort studies, natural experiments and/or genetically informative designs (including the increasingly popular method of Mendelian randomization). The guide that we propose aids the planning and interpreting of triangulation by prompting crucial considerations. Broadly, its steps are as follows: determine your causal question, draw a directed acyclic graph, identify available resources and samples, identify suitable methodological approaches, further specify the causal question for each method, explicate the effects of potential biases and, pre-specify expected results. We illustrated the guide’s use by considering the question: ‘Does maternal tobacco smoking during pregnancy cause offspring depression?’.

Conclusions

In the current era of big data, and with increasing (public) availability of large-scale datasets, triangulation will become increasingly relevant in identifying robust risk factors for adverse mental health outcomes. Our hope is that this review and guide will provide clarity and direction, as well as stimulate more researchers to apply triangulation to causal questions around behavioural and psychiatric traits.

Type
Special Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Introduction

At the basis of many important research questions is causality – does X causally impact Y? Answering such questions is particularly challenging for behavioural and psychiatric traits, as they are highly complex and multifactorial in their aetiology. Most of the time, conducting a randomized trial, the ‘gold standard’ of causality testing (Schulz et al., Reference Schulz, Altman and Moher2010), is not feasible due to practical and/or ethical reasons. Instead, researchers try to answer complex causal questions by using other methods that may help determine if changes in one variable lead to changes in another. Examples of such methods are longitudinal cohort analyses corrected for relevant confounders or natural experiments that leverage the effects of a policy change or genetic variants (Gage et al., Reference Gage, Munafò and Davey Smith2016). Any such individual method relies on particular assumptions and has certain limitations. In order to overcome the weaknesses of individual methods, ‘triangulation’ of several methods can be applied. The idea here is that prospectively choosing and then conducting several methods with different (preferably uncorrelated) biases will lead to more reliable evidence and ultimately more robust conclusions (Lawlor et al., Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016). If different methods, with different sources of bias, all indicate a causal effect in the same direction, the finding is less likely to be spurious (depicted as different ‘points of view’ on a causal question in Fig. 1) (Lawlor et al., Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016). Triangulation is not a new concept, but its application has rapidly gained popularity in behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology in recent years (Fig. 2). While triangulation has been eloquently described by others (Hammerton and Munafò, Reference Hammerton and Munafò2021; Munafò et al., Reference Munafò, Higgins and Smith2021) a more ‘hands-on’ guide with clear steps and considerations that can be used by researchers who are planning to conduct a triangulation study is currently not available. Here, we would like to provide more clarity and guidance around the process of triangulation, so that new triangulation studies can be better designed and results can be better interpreted. Ultimately, this will help researchers to determine which associations are causal and, consequently, which variables can be used to improve prevention and treatment of complex mental health problems.

Figure 1. The concept of triangulation. When two variables of interest, X and Y, are associated, this may be due to a non-causal explanation (i.e., confounding or reverse causality) or a causal explanation (X causally affecting Y). Triangulation can be seen as a process that uses different ‘points of view’ to come to a more reliable answer to a causal question. It refers to the use of different approaches, with different underlying biases, strengths, and/or weaknesses, to assess the same causal question. It is important to clarify that the goal of triangulation is not solely to pinpoint a precise point estimate of the causal effect, but rather to gather more reliable evidence regarding the direction and nature of the effect. Common types of triangulation are the use of different analytical methods, applying the same method across different study samples, and/or using different types of measures. All of these are meant to obtain more robust evidence on causality, which can help determine whether or not an intervention or change in X could beneficially impact Y.

Figure 2. Accumulation of research papers mentioning or facilitating ‘triangulation’ The number of publications in mental health and substance abuse research has increasingly gained in popularity in recent years. We show the significant trend of an increased number of scientific outputs, resulting from our PubMed search strategy: Since 2005, 1294 “triangulation” studies have been published in the field. This upward trend underscores the need for a more systematic guide for triangulation efforts and harmonize diverse approaches.

Specifically, we aim to (1) explain the concept of triangulation and how it is applied in behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology, (2) put forth a step-by-step guide that can be used to design a triangulation study, including a worked example, and (3) provide important general recommendations for conducting a triangulation study.

The concept of triangulation

What is triangulation?

The term triangulation originates from an approach that determines the location of a point of interest by measuring angles to it from two or more (known) points and applying trigonometry (i.e., using the properties of triangles) (Lawlor et al., Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016). In scientific research, usually triangulation is interpreted as conducting two or more research methods to obtain the answer to a complex question, although it is used differently across different research fields (see Box 1). By synthesizing multiple sources of information, the overall evidence is assumed to be more reliable, and more robust conclusions can be derived. The idea, that if a finding replicates across different settings, it is more likely to be true, is also incorporated in the well-known Bradford-Hill guidelines for causality as the ‘consistency’ criterion (Fedak et al., Reference Fedak, Bernal, Capshaw and Gross2015). To some degree, it lies at the basis of scientific practice in general. Researchers usually compare findings of their empirical work to those from other studies in the discussion section of a paper or in a review, acknowledging strengths and weaknesses. Systematic reviews and particularly meta-analyses aim to retrospectively combine estimates from similar analyses to come to a more precise point estimate (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Shamseer, Clarke, Ghersi, Liberati, Petticrew, Shekelle and Stewart2015). If these estimates come from the same method, some error might be removed by combining across them, but consistent bias inherent to the method will remain.

Box 1. Triangulation across research fields

Triangulation has been used across various research fields, including epidemiology, but also education and sociology (Denzin, Reference Denzin2007; Flick, Reference Flick1992; Mathison, Reference Mathison1988; Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, McFarland, Spindler, White, Patel, Aberle-Grasse, Sabin, Smith, Taché, Calleja-Garcia and Stoneburner2010). In the 80s, sociologist Norman Denzin posed that a body of various methods represents different aspects of reality, each with their own weight and relevance to the observed phenomena; ‘If each method leads to different features of empirical reality, then no single method can ever completely capture all the relevant features of that reality; consequently, sociologists must learn to employ multiple methods in the analysis of the same empirical events’ (Denzin, Reference Denzin2017). In sociology in particular, triangulation is often interpreted as ‘mixed-method research’, i.e., the combination of quantitative and qualitative methods (Casey and Murphy, Reference Casey and Murphy2009; Denzin, Reference Denzin2007). There has been criticism on this specific application of triangulation. For instance, Erzberger and Prein (Reference Erzberger and Prein1997) state that measures of the same variable of interest with such different (quantitative versus qualitative) procedures may not reflect the same ‘shape of reality’ (Erzberger and Prein, Reference Erzberger and Prein1997). Within the realm of quantitative methods, similar points of criticism could be made, such that results from (very) different data sources may be incompatible and therefore challenging to interpret together (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Goodman-Williams, Feeney and Fehler-Cabral2020). As applied in more recent literature, quantitative triangulation is typically used to test the existence and direction of a potential causal effect, but not necessarily to estimate the precise effect size.

Triangulation in the field of behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology

Here, we focus on the use of triangulation to answer aetiological questions about behavioural and psychiatric traits. In this context, a precise definition of triangulation has been proposed by Lawlor et al. (Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016) as: ‘The practice of strengthening causal inferences by integrating results from several different approaches, where each approach has different (and assumed to be largely unrelated) key sources of potential bias’. Differing from retrospective synthesis of evidence (e.g. systematic reviews), the definition of triangulation is predominantly prospective (Hammerton and Munafò, Reference Hammerton and Munafò2021; Lawlor et al., Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016). It entails deliberately selecting several approaches to complement one another, conducting them in unison and then synthesizing and interpreting the results together. Approaches can vary in (1) the methods applied, (2) the population from which the sample is drawn and/or (3) the measurement of the variables.

Approach 1: Triangulation using different methods

It is recommended that two or more methods with different sources of potential bias should be used to address the same underlying causal question (Lawlor et al., Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016). Triangulation is most effective if the selected methods have different expected directions of bias. This means that where the bias of one method may lead to an overestimation of the true causal effect, the bias of another method may be towards the null (or even towards the opposite sign of the effect) (Lawlor et al., Reference Lawlor, Tilling and Davey Smith2016). In Supplementary Table 1, we provide a broad, but non-exhaustive overview of methods that may be used in triangulation studies, meant to serve as an inspiration for introducing breadth. Under methodological triangulation, we differentiate experimental from observational studies. Experimental studies, e.g. in the form of randomized trials, are ideal for causal inference, but usually unfeasible in behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology. This is largely due to practical and ethical reasons, but also the long timespan that it would take for a potential causal effect to arise. In some instances, however, experimental manipulation can be used to assess relevant short-term processes that may tell you something about longer-term effects. In observational research, there is a wide range of methods that try to determine whether one variable affects another (although this causal goal is not always acknowledged [Hernán, Reference Hernán2018]), the most common being analysis of longitudinal cohort data to test if changes in the exposure are followed by changes in the outcome. Certain observational methods can be considered ‘quasi-experimental’ because some type of (environmental) influence or change is leveraged to create quasi-experimental groups in a given population, e.g. a policy change (Leatherdale, Reference Leatherdale2019; Maciejewski, Reference Maciejewski2020). Current triangulation literature also incorporates many relatively novel causal methods from the field of genetic epidemiology, which has leveraged major technical advances in molecular genetics over the past two decades (Abdellaoui et al., Reference Abdellaoui, Yengo, Verweij and Visscher2023). The most notable is Mendelian randomization (MR), which uses genetic variants as instrumental variables to proxy for levels of the exposure (Davey Smith and Hemani, Reference Davey Smith and Hemani2014). Other genetically informative designs don’t use molecular genetic data, but instead rely on inferences based on familial relatedness, such as discordant twin and sibling-control studies (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, O’reilly, Schoeler, Ploubidis, Rijsdijk and Dudbridge2018).

An example of methodological triangulation by van de Weijer et al. (Reference van de Weijer, Demange, Pelt, Bartels and Nivard2024) explored whether spending more years in education causally increases later-life mental wellbeing (van de Weijer et al., Reference van de Weijer, Demange, Pelt, Bartels and Nivard2024). They compared results across four methodological designs: (1) a natural experiment using the raising of the school leaving age policy change (2) a sibling-control design which accounts for shared familial confounding, (3) MR and (4) within-family MR, which further accounts for assortative mating, population stratification and dynastic effects (Brumpton et al., Reference Brumpton, Sanderson, Heilbron, Hartwig, Harrison, Vie, Cho, Howe, Hughes, Boomsma and Havdahl2020). An additional example of methodological triangulation comes from Esen et al. (Reference Esen, Ehrenstein, Sørensen, Laugesen and Pedersen2022). Seeking to understand whether in utero exposure to antidepressant drugs causes offspring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Esen et al. (Reference Esen, Ehrenstein, Sørensen, Laugesen and Pedersen2022) compared results from four methodological designs: (1) regular analysis controlling for confounders, (2) a negative-control analysis comparing the association of paternal antidepressant use with that of pregnant mothers, (3) sibling-control analysis comparing siblings who were discordant for maternal antidepressant use during pregnancy and (4) a former-user analysis comparing results of design 1 with mothers who previously took antidepressants but not during pregnancy, to mitigate confounding via maternal depression status (Esen et al., Reference Esen, Ehrenstein, Sørensen, Laugesen and Pedersen2022). For both of the triangulation examples here, the authors purposefully selected designs that each help correct for different sources of bias, where converging evidence across the different designs would suggest a true causal effect.

Approach 2: Triangulation using samples from different populations

Second, let us consider triangulating across samples from different populations. This form of triangulation can help to mitigate bias from confounding and selection pressures. An overview of applications of this approach is given in Supplementary Table 1. A particularly illustrative example comes from Brion et al. (Reference Brion, lawlor, Matijasevich, Horta, Anselmi, Araújo, Menezes, Victora and Smith2011), who tested the (potentially causal) effect of breastfeeding on cognitive development in offspring. Breastfeeding is strongly associated with socio-economic position (SEP) which is also a major confounder for the breastfeeding-cognitive development relationship. Conveniently, the direction of the confounding effect of SEP is different in the United Kingdom, where breastfeeding is associated with higher SEP, than in Brazil, where breastfeeding is associated with lower SEP. This makes the underlying confounding structures of the samples different. By investigating the causal question (if breastfeeding positively affects cognitive development) in both samples, confounding by SEP as an explanation can be partly circumvented. Finding a causal effect in both samples would constitute more reliable evidence overall (Brion et al., Reference Brion, lawlor, Matijasevich, Horta, Anselmi, Araújo, Menezes, Victora and Smith2011)

As an additional example, Chartier et al. (Reference Chartier, Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2023) explored whether immigration increases risk for alcohol use disorder (AUD) through acculturation. Acculturation refers to the assimilation of the less dominant into the more dominant culture (Chartier et al., Reference Chartier, Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2023). Chartier et al. (Reference Chartier, Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2023) hypothesize that the rates of AUD in the immigrant populations will become more similar to the rates in the native population over subsequent generations. They compared immigrants in Sweden from seven different geographical regions, each with different starting levels of AUD. For example, 1st generation immigrants from Africa have lower rates of AUD than the Swedish population and Finnish 1st generation immigrants have higher rates. Over subsequent generations, rates of AUD in both populations became closer to the Swedish average. Change in different directions makes this less likely to be due to confounding and more likely due to acculturation.

Approach 3: Triangulation using different measures

Finally, triangulation could compare different types of measures for the variable of interest, for instance taken from different sources, raters or obtained with different measurement instruments. For example, while self-report can be affected by recall or social desirability bias, registry data may be measured with error due to missingness or incorrectly recorded disease codes whilst obtaining a correct diagnosis. An illustrative example comes from Haan et al. (Reference Haan, Sallis, Ystrom, Njølstad, Andreassen, Reichborn‐Kjennerud, Munafò, Havdahl and Zuccolo2021) who explored the association between foetal alcohol exposure and ADHD risk in offspring (Haan et al., Reference Haan, Sallis, Ystrom, Njølstad, Andreassen, Reichborn‐Kjennerud, Munafò, Havdahl and Zuccolo2021). They observed different patterns of association depending on whether ADHD symptoms were mother or teacher reported, and depending on the scale used. With no single measure being free from bias, triangulating across several available measures can help strengthen conclusions. However, careful consideration of the interpretation is needed when results differ. An overview of how this approach could be applied is given in Supplementary Table 1.

A step-by-step guide for designing a triangulation study

A successful triangulation study could take many forms, depending upon the causal question and available data, but should always follow the key principle of triangulation – that more powerful causal conclusions are drawn by combining multiple approaches with different sources of bias. In this section, we provide a step-by-step guide to aid the planning and interpretation of triangulation studies (Fig. 3). Our guide doesn’t aim to be prescriptive nor exhaustive, but rather to prompt the crucial considerations necessary for an effective triangulation protocol. We accompany this with a worked example, where we work through each step of the guide (see Supplementary Note).

Figure 3. Triangulation guide. This step-by-step guide is meant to take researchers along through the most important steps of designing a triangulation study, highlighting the most important considerations and current best practices.

Step 1: Determine the causal question

It is of upmost importance to begin with a clear causal research question. The best causal questions are informed by theory and existing literature and take into account whether or not a potential effect is restricted to a particular timing or length of exposure. For our worked example, we ask the causal question ‘Does maternal tobacco smoking during pregnancy cause offspring depression?’ (Haber et al., Reference Haber, Wood, Wieten and Breskin2022). It should be noted that the initial question that is posed at the start of a triangulation study, will translate to (slightly) different, but related, research questions across the different approaches that are subsequently chosen (see step 5 of this guide). Approaches which are testing more similar questions will be easier to integrate and interpret together. Ultimately, triangulation aims to come to the best-fitting explanation for an observed relationship, based on the totality of the data.

Step 2: Draw a directed acyclic graph

Directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) can be used as a tool for mapping out assumed relationships between relevant variables (Lipsky and Greenland, Reference Lipsky and Greenland2022). In essence, a DAG is a graph that consists of the variables of interest with arrows between the variables indicating the direction of hypothesized causal effects and a lack of an arrow between any two variables implying that there is no hypothesized causal effect. A DAG is ‘acyclic’, meaning that cycles (or feedback loops) are not allowed. In the context of triangulation, DAGs can help you clarify your assumptions, establish key sources of bias and identify appropriate covariates for statistical analysis. A DAG for our worked example is given in the Supplementary Note.

Step 3: Identify available resources and samples

With your causal question defined, the next step is to scope out the resources available. Here, the aim is to find the boundaries within which the triangulation will have to be conducted, which may represent financial, ethical or practical constraints. For many causal questions in aetiological epidemiology, there will not be sufficient resources or time to collect new data. Therefore, identification of suitable secondary data often presents the most effective way to conduct a triangulation study. When identifying possible datasets, consideration should be given to sample characteristics and selection mechanisms, so that these sources of bias can also be triangulated.

Let us take our worked example, does maternal tobacco smoking during pregnancy cause offspring depression? The timing of our question is specific to intrauterine effects. Therefore, suitable datasets are restricted to those with relevant smoking data during pregnancy and long enough follow-up to capture depression traits in offspring. As collecting new prospective longitudinal data to investigate this question would be too time-consuming and expensive (Esen et al., Reference Esen, Ehrenstein, Sørensen, Laugesen and Pedersen2022), we identify only relevant secondary data sources for this worked example (see Supplementary Note).

Step 4: Identify suitable methodological approaches

At this stage, we identify methods which are suitable to test the causal question and have different (preferably uncorrelated) main sources of bias. The aim here is to try and mitigate the most problematic sources of bias as identified in step 2. Here, we identify which methods are feasible given the available samples.

In the Supplementary Note, we expand upon four methodological approaches that would allow us to test whether there is evidence for causal effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy on offspring depression. From Supplementary Table 1, several methods were not suitable, for example, any type of randomisation to smoking during pregnancy would pose serious ethical concerns. However, there were still a multitude of study designs which could help investigate our causal question, within the scope of available resources. First, to address potential confounding by environmental factors (e.g. SEP) and by mother’s own depression, we propose a longitudinal regression model with adjustment for confounding in an existing prospective pregnancy cohort. This method can still be biased by residual confounding and doesn’t account for shared genetic liability between parents and offspring. In other words, if the mother is more likely to smoke, the offspring is more likely to smoke, and their own smoking could cause their depression. To address these biases, we would plan to conduct an intergenerational MR study. This approach uses genetic variants as instrumental variables to reduce bias from residual confounding, and adjusts for offspring genotype to isolate maternal effects (Reference Davies, Holmes and SmithDavies et al.,). Additionally, we could add a proxy GxE MR study, which tests whether maternal smoking during pregnancy is associated with offspring depression. If we observe an association between the genetic variants for maternal smoking and offspring depression, regardless of maternal smoking status, effects are likely non-specific to smoking during pregnancy. Finally, we would repeat the confounder adjustment and MR analyses with father’s smoking as a powerful negative control (since the father cannot have direct intrauterine effects on the offspring).

Statistical power is an important factor to consider here, because it will determine the likelihood for each method to pick up on a true causal effect. For new data collection, conducting an a priori power calculation is important and highly recommended. For secondary data analysis (where data collection has already occurred) there is no consensus about the usefulness of conducting power analyses (Dziak et al., Reference Dziak, Dierker and Abar2020). Where possible (i.e., if the data are already accessible), we consider a power analysis at this stage beneficial, as it can be taken into account when specifying your expectations of potential findings and how to interpret them. When key information to conduct a power analysis is not available, it may be computed post hoc to aid interpretation. While underpowered analyses should generally be avoided there are certainly cases where adding an analysis that could mitigate an important source of bias would be valuable.

Step 5: Further specify the causal question per method

After having posed the initial causal question under step 1, here, we aim to define the specific questions that can be answered with each of the approaches that we have chosen. To facilitate the integration of findings from different approaches, we recommend researchers formulate their causal research questions as specifically as possible in this step. Drawing specific DAGs for each approach may be useful during this refinement process. To illustrate, the specific causal question for the confounder adjusted analysis in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC)(see Supplementary Note) is as follows: ‘In a sample of offspring born in the early 90s in Avon (UK), is mother-reported maternal tobacco smoking during pregnancy (in any trimester) associated with offspring self-reported depressive symptoms up to age 30 years, after adjustment for measured confounders?’

Step 6: Explicate the effects of potential biases

Once the most suitable methods, samples and measures for triangulation have been chosen, and specific questions have been formulated, it is important to explicate the potential effects of biases. Of course, it is not possible for the effects of all potential biases to be known, by their very definition some are unknown (e.g., residual confounding). In Supplementary Table 1, we highlight the most notable sources of bias for a selection of approaches.

Step 7: Pre-specify expectations under causality

For triangulation to be most effective, it is important to specify a priori what results we would expect to see under true causality and what results we may expect if our association of interest is in fact not causal (Höfler, Reference Höfler2005). Most often, pre-specification of a true causal effect is straightforward – we would expect each of the methods to show statistical evidence for an effect in the same direction across the methods/cohorts/samples fulfilling broadly similar characteristics (such as age). When specifying expected causal effects, it is important to consider the power of each method. For example, van de Weijer et al. (Reference van de Weijer, Demange, Pelt, Bartels and Nivard2024) pre-specified that they would not expect the within-family MR results to pass the threshold for statistical significance given low power and that consequently, for that method they would be looking to see whether the direction of effect was consistent with the other methods (van de Weijer et al., Reference van de Weijer, Demange, Pelt, Bartels and Nivard2024). Another important consideration is the timing and duration of the exposure of interest across different methods. If for one method the influence of the exposure is relatively short (for instance in an experimental setting) while in another method it is relatively long (for instance in a MR study where the genetic variants express a lifetime influence), it is to be expected that the size of the effect would differ. This needs to be taken into consideration when pre-specifying your expectations, and later, when comparing the results. If one method finds evidence for a causal effect and another doesn’t, but this can convincingly be explained based on (pre-specified) differences in the timing, measurement, or duration of the exposure, it might not decrease the overall evidence for causality.

More difficult, but equally as important, is to pre-specify your expected results under a non-causal model. Where known (or deducible), it is useful to begin by pre-specifying the expected biases of each method, and their expected direction of effect. This can help to ensure that you have selected methods with uncorrelated biases. For example, van de Weijer et al. (Reference van de Weijer, Demange, Pelt, Bartels and Nivard2024) specified a priori that they would conclude that there was substantial confounding or bias from reverse causation if results were not significant and consistent (van de Weijer et al., Reference van de Weijer, Demange, Pelt, Bartels and Nivard2024). When defining such expectations, it is important to consider your limitations with regard to statistical power. Identifying true causal relationships requires the presence of robust outcomes with strong statistical evidence. If your analyses are underpowered then small effects might not be detected. Nonetheless, it is likely that utilizing diverse methodologies across potentially heterogeneous samples leads to outcomes of differing levels of (statistical) strength. To employ an analogy, envision a reservoir: with each piece of evidence stemming from a method or sample, water is added, its volume proportional to the method’s significance and weight. Conversely, opposing effects would deplete the reservoir’s reserves. The overall evidence for causality is reflected in the fullness of the reservoir.

General recommendations for future triangulation studies

In addition to our step-by-step guide, here we would like to highlight some important, more general recommendations for those planning to conduct a triangulation study.

Triangulation in the context of the broader literature

A well-designed triangulation study should be clearly informed by the existing literature and set up to address the most important limitations of prior studies. The results of a triangulation study should also be evaluated within the context of the body of evidence that already exists, taking into account the inherent biases of each line of evidence and their capacity to reflect reality. If we want to come to more reliable scientific evidence to answer complex causal questions in behavioural and psychiatric epidemiology, it is crucial to acknowledge which exact biases are relevant for which methods, samples and measures, both when we compare the results of analyses within one (triangulation) study as well as when we compare results across studies.

Falsification

One approach that can be particularly powerful in a triangulation framework but which is not applied widely enough is the negative-control analysis. In the context of the worked example in this review, we chose a negative-control design where we compared the effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on offspring depression to the effect of paternal smoking during pregnancy. Father’s smoking cannot have direct causal intrauterine effects on the offspring, therefore if paternal effects have a similar magnitude of association as maternal effects, this is likely due to bias from confounding. We recommend researchers to adapt an ethos of attempting to falsify a potentially causal finding, for which negative-control analysis is very suitable. Note that either a negative-control outcome variable or a negative-control exposure variable can be tested. It is important to select valid negative controls, where it is clear or well established that there is no true causal effect but the same confounding structure. Besides detecting whether or not there is residual bias, there are also statistical methodologies that can be applied to then correct for this bias (Shi et al., Reference Shi, Miao and Tchetgen2020).

The importance of diversity of thought

While the explicit combination of different types of analytical methods, data sources and/or samples is the major strength of triangulation, it may also pose certain difficulties. These may include lack of financial resources and time to collect new data, constraints in obtaining access to the required secondary data, insufficient methodological expertise and/or insufficient cross-cultural knowledge in your research team. Some of these constraints can be counteracted by the inclusion of collaborators with relevant expertise or datasets. We recommend including diversity of thought and an interdisciplinary team approach at the idea generation stage. Incorporating broad study designs across different fields and comparing across cultural contexts are effective ways to achieve triangulation (Forscher et al., Reference Forscher, Wagenmakers, Coles, Silan, Dutra, Basnight-Brown and IJzerman2023).

Pre-registration

Finally, we highly encourage that an analysis plan is pre-registered prior to conducting analyses. Pre-registration is crucial for the validity of any study, including studies that use secondary data analysis (Baldwin et al., Reference Baldwin, Pingault, Schoeler, Sallis and Munafò2022), because it will reduce questionable research practices and the effects of processes such as ‘confirmation bias’ and ‘hindsight bias’ (Munafò et al., Reference Munafò, Nosek, Bishop, Button, Chambers, Percie du Sert, Simonsohn, Wagenmakers, Ware and Ioannidis2017). For a triangulation study, the most crucial components to formulate and pre-register are the initial causal question, the more refined causal questions per method, the key sources of bias for each method and their expected (direction of) effect, and finally, the expected results under a true causal effect and under no causal effect. Pre-registrations can be posted on platforms such as the Open Science Framework, or researchers could consider submitting to a journal which invites registered reports (Henderson and Chambers, Reference Henderson and Chambers2022).

Conclusion

In the current era of big data, triangulation will become increasingly important to be able to draw definitive conclusions about causality. With more and more large-scale (publicly) available datasets and biobanks, as well as novel sophisticated (causal) methods, there will likely be a massive influx of ‘single studies’ that make use of one dataset and apply one particular research method. If there are many published studies based on the same dataset, with similar (but not the same) applied methodologies, and these are not integrated as in a triangulation framework, then the collective knowledge base may become ‘cluttered’, taking longer to achieve a scientific consensus. Planning and conducting a triangulation study inevitably requires more time and effort than a paper comprising a single approach, resulting in a trade-off between the benefits for the individual researcher versus the benefits for the scientific field. However, with epidemiology experiencing a move towards more collaborative team science, and large amounts of available secondary data, we argue that triangulation is becoming more feasible (and necessary) than ever before. Furthermore, the resulting publication from a triangulation study is likely more impactful, making a more important contribution to the literature. That is why we would like to advocate for triangulation as the preferred approach for complex causal questions in epidemiological psychiatry. With this review, we hope to encourage more people to conduct prospective triangulation studies, but also to be more aware and explicit when comparing findings from different, already conducted studies to one another (for a good example of this see Shahab et al., Reference Shahab, Brown, Boelen, Beard, West and Munafò2022). Triangulation stands as a robust and encompassing approach to attain causal conclusions that draw validation from a multifaceted array of perspectives.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796024000623.

Availability of data and materials

No empirical data or materials were employed for this review paper.

Financial support

JLT and EL are supported by a Senior Scientist Dekker Grant from the Dutch Heart Foundation (project number 03-004-2022-0055). JLT is funded by the European Union (ERC, UNRAVEL-CAUSALITY, 101076686). Views and opinions expressed are however those of the author(s) only and do not necessarily reflect those of the European Union or the European Research Council. Neither the European Union nor the granting authority can be held responsible for them. REW is funded by the Norwegian South-Eastern Regional Health Authority (2020024). HMS and REW are members of the MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol which is supported by the Medical Research Council and the University of Bristol (MC_UU_00011/7)

Competing interests

None

References

Abdellaoui, A, Yengo, L, Verweij, K. J and Visscher, PM (2023) 15 years of GWAS discovery: Realizing the promise. The American Journal of Human Genetics 110, 179194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, JR, Pingault, J-B, Schoeler, T, Sallis, HM and Munafò, MR (2022) Protecting against researcher bias in secondary data analysis: Challenges and potential solutions. European Journal of Epidemiology 37, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brion, M. J, lawlor, DA, Matijasevich, A, Horta, B, Anselmi, L, Araújo, CL, Menezes, AM, Victora, CG, Smith, GD. (2011) What are the causal effects of breastfeeding on IQ, obesity and blood pressure? Evidence from comparing high-income with middle-income cohorts. International Journal of Epidemiology 40, 670680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brumpton, B, Sanderson, E, Heilbron, K, Hartwig, FP, Harrison, S, Vie, , Cho, Y, Howe, LD, Hughes, A, Boomsma, DI and Havdahl, A (2020) Avoiding dynastic, assortative mating, and population stratification biases in Mendelian randomization through within-family analyses. Nature Communications 11, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, R, Goodman-Williams, R, Feeney, H and Fehler-Cabral, G (2020) Assessing triangulation across methodologies, methods, and stakeholder groups: The joys, woes, and politics of interpreting convergent and divergent data. American Journal of Evaluation 41, 125144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, D and Murphy, K (2009) Issues in using methodological triangulation in research. Nurse researcher 16, 4055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chartier, KG, Kendler, KS, Ohlsson, H, Sundquist, K and Sundquist, J (2023) Triangulation of evidence on immigration and rates of alcohol use disorder in Sweden: Evidence of acculturation effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 47, 104115.Google ScholarPubMed
Davey Smith, G and Hemani, G (2014) Mendelian randomization: Genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Human Molecular Genetics 23, R89R98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, NM, Holmes, MV, and Smith, GD (2018) Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: A guide, glossary, and checklist for clinicians. BMJ 362 362, . doi:10.1136/bmj.k601.Google ScholarPubMed
Denzin, NK (2007) Triangulation. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Denzin, NK The research act: A theoretical introduction to sociological methods. (Transaction publishers, 2017).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dziak, JJ, Dierker, LC and Abar, B (2020) The interpretation of statistical power after the data have been gathered. Current Psychology 39, 870877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erzberger, C and Prein, G (1997) Triangulation: Validity and empirically-based hypothesis construction. Quality and Quantity 31, 141154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esen, , Ehrenstein, V, Sørensen, HT, Laugesen, K and Pedersen, L (2022) Prenatal antidepressant exposure and the risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in childhood: A cohort study with triangulation. Epidemiology 33, 581592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fedak, KM, Bernal, A, Capshaw, ZA and Gross, S (2015) Applying the Bradford Hill criteria in the 21st century: How data integration has changed causal inference in molecular epidemiology. Emerging Themes in Epidemiology 12(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flick, U (1992) Triangulation revisited: Strategy of validation or alternative? Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 22, 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forscher, PS, Wagenmakers, EJ, Coles, NA, Silan, MA, Dutra, N, Basnight-Brown, D, IJzerman, H (2023) The benefits, barriers, and risks of big-team science. Perspectives on Psychological Science 18, 607623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, SH, Munafò, MR and Davey Smith, G (2016) Causal inference in developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) research. Annual Review of Psychology 67, 567585.Google ScholarPubMed
Haan, E, Sallis, HM, Ystrom, E, Njølstad, PR, Andreassen, OA, Reichborn‐Kjennerud, T, Munafò, MR, Havdahl, A and Zuccolo, L (2021) Maternal and offspring genetic risk score analyses of fetal alcohol exposure and attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder risk in offspring. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 45, 20902102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, NA, Wood, ME, Wieten, S and Breskin, A (2022) DAG with omitted objects displayed (DAGWOOD): A framework for revealing causal assumptions in DAGs. Annals of Epidemiology 68, 6471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammerton, G and Munafò, MR (2021) Causal inference with observational data: The need for triangulation of evidence. Psychological Medicine 51, 563578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, EL and Chambers, CD (2022) Ten simple rules for writing a Registered Report. PLoS Computational Biology 18, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernán, MA (2018) The C-word: Scientific euphemisms do not improve causal inference from observational data. American Journal of Public Health 108, 616619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Höfler, M (2005) Causal inference based on counterfactuals. BMC Medical Research Methodology 5, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawlor, DA, Tilling, K and Davey Smith, G (2016) Triangulation in aetiological epidemiology. Internal Journal of Epidemiology 45, 18661886.Google ScholarPubMed
Leatherdale, ST (2019) Natural experiment methodology for research: A review of how different methods can support real-world research. International Journal of Social Research Methodology 22, 1935.Google Scholar
Lipsky, AM and Greenland, S (2022) Causal directed acyclic graphs. Jama 327, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maciejewski, ML (2020) Quasi-experimental design. Biostatistics & Epidemiology 4, 3847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathison, S (1988) Why triangulate? Educational Researcher 17, 1317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moher, D, Shamseer, L, Clarke, M, Ghersi, D, Liberati, A, Petticrew, M, Shekelle, P, and Stewart, LA, Prisma-P Group (2015) Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews 4(1), .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munafò, MR, Higgins, JPT and Smith, GD (2021). Triangulating evidence through the inclusion of genetically informed designs Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine 11, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munafò, MR, Nosek, BA, Bishop, DV, Button, KS, Chambers, CD, Percie du Sert, N, Simonsohn, U, Wagenmakers, EJ, Ware, JJ, Ioannidis, J (2017) A manifesto for reproducible science. Nature Human Behaviour 1, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, JB, O’reilly, PF, Schoeler, T, Ploubidis, GB, Rijsdijk, F, Dudbridge, F (2018) Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nature Reviews Genetics 19, 566580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, GW, McFarland, W, Spindler, H, White, K, Patel, SV, Aberle-Grasse, J, Sabin, K, Smith, N, Taché, S, Calleja-Garcia, JM, and Stoneburner, RL (2010) Public health triangulation: Approach and application to synthesizing data to understand national and local HIV epidemics. BMC Public Health 10, .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, KF, Altman, DG and Moher, D (2010) CONSORT 2010 statement: Updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Journal of Pharmacology & Pharmacotherapeutics 1, 100107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shahab, L, Brown, J, Boelen, L, Beard, E, West, R, Munafò, MR (2022) Unpacking the gateway hypothesis of e-cigarette use: The need for triangulation of individual-and population-level data. Nicotine & Tobacco Research 24, 13151318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, X, Miao, W and Tchetgen, ET (2020) A selective review of negative control methods in epidemiology. Current Epidemiology Reports 7, 190202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Weijer, MP, Demange, PA, Pelt, DHM, Bartels, M, and Nivard, MG (2024) Disentangling potential causal effects of educational duration on well-being, and mental and physical health outcomes. Psychological Medicine 54(7), 14031418. doi:10.1017/S003329172300329X.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. The concept of triangulation. When two variables of interest, X and Y, are associated, this may be due to a non-causal explanation (i.e., confounding or reverse causality) or a causal explanation (X causally affecting Y). Triangulation can be seen as a process that uses different ‘points of view’ to come to a more reliable answer to a causal question. It refers to the use of different approaches, with different underlying biases, strengths, and/or weaknesses, to assess the same causal question. It is important to clarify that the goal of triangulation is not solely to pinpoint a precise point estimate of the causal effect, but rather to gather more reliable evidence regarding the direction and nature of the effect. Common types of triangulation are the use of different analytical methods, applying the same method across different study samples, and/or using different types of measures. All of these are meant to obtain more robust evidence on causality, which can help determine whether or not an intervention or change in X could beneficially impact Y.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Accumulation of research papers mentioning or facilitating ‘triangulation’ The number of publications in mental health and substance abuse research has increasingly gained in popularity in recent years. We show the significant trend of an increased number of scientific outputs, resulting from our PubMed search strategy: Since 2005, 1294 “triangulation” studies have been published in the field. This upward trend underscores the need for a more systematic guide for triangulation efforts and harmonize diverse approaches.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Triangulation guide. This step-by-step guide is meant to take researchers along through the most important steps of designing a triangulation study, highlighting the most important considerations and current best practices.

Supplementary material: File

Treur et al. supplementary material

Treur et al. supplementary material
Download Treur et al. supplementary material(File)
File 77.8 KB