Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T08:07:41.343Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A review of factors affecting the food choices of disadvantaged women

Workshop on ‘Changing nutrition behaviour to improve maternal and fetal health’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2009

W. Lawrence*
Affiliation:
Food Choice Group, University of Southampton, Southampton General Hospital, Southampton S016 6YD, UK
M. Barker
Affiliation:
Food Choice Group, University of Southampton, Southampton General Hospital, Southampton S016 6YD, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Ms Wendy Lawrence, fax +44 23 8070 4021, email wtl@mrc.soton.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The diets of young women are important not just for their own health but also for the long-term health of their offspring. Unbalanced unvaried diets are more common amongst poor and disadvantaged women. If the diets of these women are to be improved, it is first necessary to understand why they make the food choices they do. Influences on women's food choices range from the global to the individual: environmental factors, such as difficulty in acquiring and affording good-quality healthy foods; social support and social relationships, such as those with parents, spouses and children; life transitions, such as leaving home, living with a partner or having children; individual factors, such as having low perceived control or self-efficacy in making food choices and placing a low value on health in general and on their own health in particular. These interrelated factors all influence food choice, suggesting that if the diets of disadvantaged women are to be improved, it will be necessary to do more than simply educate about the link between diet and health.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2009

The present paper aims to review the range of factors that affect the food-choice decisions made by disadvantaged women as a step towards meeting the challenge of improving the health and nutrition of disadvantaged populations. The diets of young women are important not just for their own health, but also for the long-term health of their offspring(Reference Barker1, Reference Barker2). Unbalanced and unvaried diets are known to be detrimental to the growth of babies(Reference Godfrey, Barker and Robinson3Reference Haugen, Hanson and Kiserud5) and are more common in disadvantaged women(6, Reference Dowler and Calvert7). Women of lower educational attainment are much more likely to eat a poor-quality diet than women of higher educational attainment(Reference Robinson, Crozier and Borland8). Improving the diets of disadvantaged women before and during pregnancy may hold the key to breaking the cycle of disadvantage and ill health suffered by the poor in Britain today.

Understanding food choice

Influences on women's food choices range from the global to the individual. Global influences are shaped by the productionist paradigm of food provision, which in today's developed world means extensive food choice is available to individuals who have the necessary resources(Reference Lang and Heasman9). These resources include money, access to shops, time and the knowledge required to choose and prepare the variety of food products on offer. The negative side to the extensive range of foods available is that many of the cheapest easiest-to-prepare ‘convenience’ foods are high in saturated fats and sugars, which are undesirable in a healthy diet. The choices an individual makes are going to be determined partly by what retailers offer.

Macro-environmental influences will vary according to local community and social circumstances shared by individuals living in similar situations. Micro-environmental influences such as family dynamics will affect the choices made by the household. Individual influences, such as preferences, will determine who eats what within that household. To make food choices individuals need to consider what, how, when, where and with whom they will eat, as well as selecting and consuming foods. These choices express preferences, identities and cultural meanings and are based on an individual's lifetime experience of food and expectations for the future(Reference Shepherd and Raats10). The importance of these factors for the food choices of disadvantaged women will be presented under four broad headings, i.e. environmental, social, historical and psychological influences on food choice.

Environmental factors

Cost

Large socio-economic differences in patterns of diet suggest that a woman's environment has an important impact on her food-choice decisions. One of the biggest factors in this relationship will undoubtedly be income, together with the availability and affordability of good-quality healthy foods. Having lower socio-economic status and being poor are strongly related to consuming an inadequate diet(Reference Shelton11), particularly where food insecurity exists, and women make trade-offs between their own health and that of their children(Reference Olson12). Women will ensure their families are fed before they feed themselves, even if it means they themselves go hungry. More specifically, poverty is associated with a reduced variety of foods eaten and with low fruit and vegetable consumption(Reference Dowler and Calvert7). A study looking at children's diets has found that eating less healthily and snacking are associated with greater deprivation and lower maternal education(Reference Sweeting and West13).

Income may also be one of the reasons for the failure of initiatives to increase the consumption of fruit and vegetables amongst groups of lower socio-economic status. It has been found that whilst low income does not appear to be a barrier to buying the habitual amount of fruit and vegetables, participants claim that they cannot afford to buy larger amounts(Reference Dibsdall, Lambert and Bobbin14). It is viewed as an additional expense, rather than an exchange of some food items for healthier options. The authors conclude that motivational, psycho-social and lifestyle factors present a bigger problem in increasing fruit and vegetable consumption than access and affordability. Focus-group data from Southampton, UK suggest that women of lower educational attainment balance cost with potential for waste when deciding whether to buy fruit and vegetables(Reference Barker, Lawrence and Skinner15), some of them feeling that it is more cost effective to buy more expensive frozen vegetables rather than fresh, because less is thrown away.

Access

Linked to the affordability of food is the issue of access. There is a substantial literature debating the existence of ‘food deserts’, i.e. populated urban areas in which residents do not have access to affordable healthy foods(Reference Cummins and Macintyre16). These areas are said to be the result of the development of edge-of-city superstores leading to the closure of smaller inner-city and suburban food stores, which has disadvantaged consumers who do not have access to a car(Reference Wrigley17Reference Clarke, Eyre and Guy19). Smaller corner stores have less fresh food and are more expensive, implying that consumers without cars may struggle to eat a healthy diet. However, it is not clear that food deserts are quite as big an issue as some investigators have suggested(Reference Cummins and Macintyre16). Big ‘multiple’ stores are able to stock a wide variety of reasonably-priced products, and are moving back into city centres and local sites. Many shops located in or near deprived areas stock a range of basic food items either similar in price to or cheaper than those in more-affluent areas(Reference Cummins and Macintyre16). However, one major study has reported that a new superstore built in an area of poor food retail provision did lead to a sudden and marked improvement in access to healthier food items and a corresponding increase in fruit and vegetable consumption(Reference Wrigley, Warm and Margetts20).

There may be an issue of access to healthy affordable food in some areas and for some individuals. Evidence from Southampton women is that access as such is not an issue. Those women who did not have access to a car did mention the difficulties of fruit and vegetable shopping, but these difficulties were about carrying heavy weights on a pushchair rather than not having access(Reference Barker, Lawrence and Skinner15). This finding suggests that the whole shopping experience should be considered when developing initiatives to encourage this population to eat more healthily.

Social factors

Food consumers exist within the contexts formed by other individuals and groups, such as family and friends, food industries and governments(Reference Worsley21), and much still needs to be understood about how individuals manage food choices in social relationships(Reference Bove, Sobal and Rauschenbach22). Social support from family, friends and co-workers has been found to predict 12-month increases in fruit and vegetable intake, independently of demographic factors(Reference Steptoe, Perkins-Porras and Rink23). Social relationships, such as those with parents, spouses and children, are important influences on personal food systems and rarely remain stable over time. They are affected by life transitions, such as leaving home, living with a partner or having children. Families and households provide one of the most important sets of interpersonal relationships influencing food choice.

It has been reported that when women move in with a partner their food choices become constrained by the man's preferences(Reference Brown and Miller24). Thus, eating has the potential to create conflict and to influence the health of both partners. Negotiations ensue and are important to understand because of the high proportion of food eaten with, or under the influence of, a partner(Reference Bove, Sobal and Rauschenbach22). Whilst women tend to do the majority of food shopping and preparation, the preferences of men and children in the home are primary influences on the food choices made(Reference Murcott, Garmarnikow, Morgan, Purvis and Taylorson25). In a recent study women cite objections from male partners as the greatest barrier to healthier eating(Reference Beagan and Chapman26). It was found that some of the women who had modified their diets to make themselves feel better, gave up these changes under the influence of a new partner. Children were often described as picky eaters, and whilst some women refused to accommodate children's preferences, others only cooked meals they knew the family would eat, thus impacting on the quality of the whole-family's diet.

Others researchers suggest that a woman's perception of her role within the household influences her food choices for the family(Reference Beagan and Chapman26). It has been reported that participants see healthier eating as being more common for married couples, and one participant talked of it being part of her spousal role to encourage her husband to eat more fruit and vegetables(Reference Devine, Connors and Bisogni27). A woman's need to manage social relationships or minimise cost can come into conflict with her desire to provide healthy food choices(Reference Devine, Connors and Bisogni27). Introducing healthier foods is often met with resistance from family members who refuse to eat them, so women have a dilemma(Reference Connors, Bisogni and Sobal28). Unless faced with an important health crisis, women tend to place a higher priority on maintaining social relationships through meeting others' food desires rather than persisting in efforts to provide healthy food choices(Reference Beagan and Chapman26). In a study to investigate barriers to healthy eating within the family context it was found that women with children see increasing fruit and vegetable consumption as more feasible than do other women(Reference Ball, Crawford and Warren29). It could be that an increased sense of responsibility towards ensuring healthy food is available in the household for children has a positive effect on a woman's perceived ability and motivation to change her family's eating behaviour. Again, data from Southampton women suggest that women from all socio-economic status groups take the health of their children very seriously and believe in the importance of feeding them a healthy diet, even if they do not always feel able to provide healthy foods(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30). Making healthy food-choice decisions for their children and meeting with persistent opposition demands skilled and confident parenting. Food and eating are central to domestic harmony. The way social relationships and household food provision are managed by women is likely to have been learned through early-life experiences and life events.

Historical factors

It has been suggested that food-management processes rely heavily on tradition, with the study finding that over their lifetime the participants had developed strong beliefs and feelings about the way they should be eating and providing food for others(Reference Bisogni, Jastran and Shen31). Memories from childhood provide images that stay throughout adulthood; thus, homemade or mother's cooking are used as reference points for how food should be prepared and taste(Reference Bisogni, Jastran and Shen31). Each time they choose a food individuals bring their past food choices, events and experiences to bear; they are thus strong influences on personal systems for food choices(Reference Devine, Connors and Bisogni27, Reference Devine32). The thoughts and feelings associated with those choices, and the temporal, social and historical contexts that have helped to shape them, make up individuals' life-course trajectories of food choice. Trajectories are cumulative, developing over the lifetime and incorporating meaningful experiences with food and eating. Thus, food-choice trajectories lead to habitual food selections that can affect how individuals adjust to transitions such as ageing and health changes(Reference Devine32). It has been found that participants' fruit and vegetable trajectories are shaped by seven major types of experiences and events over the life course: food upbringing; roles and role transitions; health; ethnic traditions; resources; location; the food system i.e. ever-changing diet and health information related to nutrition across the lifespan(Reference Devine, Connors and Bisogni27).

Food experiences early in life are a prominent factor in shaping current food choices, and provide lasting ‘food roots’. The role of tradition and the women's past experiences may be key influences on how she considers food within her family; what she is prepared to buy, prepare and cook being restricted if her experiences are limited or negative. Positive childhood experiences lead to more positive trajectories of lifelong healthier food consumption. Foods disliked or not featuring in childhood tend not to be incorporated into personal food systems and remain uneaten(Reference Devine32). Food preferences develop at a very early age, and repeated exposure to a variety of foods enhances the probability that this taste for a wide range of foods will be maintained throughout life. Infants sometimes only need one exposure before developing a preference for a particular food(Reference Birch, Gunder and Grimm-Thomas33), with older children it takes approximately eight to ten exposures(Reference Sullivan and Birch34Reference Birch, Mcphee and Shoba36) and in adulthood it takes many more exposures to develop a preference for a flavour previously disliked(Reference Pliner37). Thus, the tastes experienced by individuals in early life will largely determine the foods they eat as an adult, and habits, once acquired, are hard to break. It has been found that negative experiences of food and eating in childhood are common to disadvantaged women in Southampton, and these women describe the ways in which this experience has left them with a dislike for certain foods and a limited knowledge of many others(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30). Some women react against their own experience and, realising how far-reaching its effects are, want to provide their own children with more positive experiences of food(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30). The life-course transitions into marriage or partnership and into parenthood are often times of dietary change and have been proposed as windows of opportunity for improving food choices.

Psychological factors

Ultimately, the influence of all these factors operates through food choices made by the individual. Psychological concepts that have been identified as determinants of food choice include perceptions of control and self-efficacy, attitudes and beliefs, mood and self-esteem and the value given to eating a healthy diet.

Control

It is argued that control is a key concept in the psychology of health(Reference Walker38). Health locus of control is a specific measure of control beliefs about health(Reference Wallston, Wallston and Kaplan39). Research shows that chance health locus of control (belief that good health is a result of chance) is consistently related to being of lower socio-economic status and the higher likelihood of health-compromising behaviours, such as smoking, sedentary lifestyles and poorer diet(Reference Wardle and Steptoe40). Women of lower educational attainment and low socio-economic status are more likely to feel that their future health is an inevitable consequence of chance rather than under their control(Reference Wallston, Wallston and Kaplan39, Reference Steptoe, Perkins-Porras and McKay41). Other research has shown that women with higher levels of education eat fruit and vegetables more frequently and believe less in chance and more in eating healthily as a way of maintaining their health(Reference Davies42, Reference Leganger and Kraft43).

A strong theme in focus-group discussions with women of lower educational attainment is their sense that they lack control over food choices for themselves and their families(Reference Barker, Lawrence and Skinner15). Partners and children give them little support for making healthy food choices and exert a high extent of control over foods bought and prepared for the family. Perceptions of the high cost of healthy food, the need to avoid waste and being trapped at home surrounded by opportunities to snack all constrain these women's freedom to make healthy food choices. Having limited skill and experience with food further undermines their ability to feed their families in the way they feel they should. All these factors contribute to women's loss of control over their food choices(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30).

Self-efficacy

Self-efficacy refers to an individual's perceived ability to achieve a desired outcome, which affects every phase of personal change, i.e. whether they consider changing their health habits, find the motivation and perseverance needed to succeed and how well they maintain new health habits once achieved(Reference Bandura44). Individuals' beliefs that they can motivate themselves and regulate their own behaviour play a crucial role in whether they change their behaviour. Thus, measures of perceived self-efficacy are important in predicting health behaviour and behaviour change. Women with higher educational attainment believe more in their own competence and ability to control their behaviour and its outcome(Reference Leganger and Kraft43).

Certain processes have to occur for the successful provision of food to a household, involving food acquisition, storage, preparation, cooking, service and disposal, as well as organisation and coordination of time, tasks and household eating schedules. Women carrying out these tasks clearly have to have an extent of self-efficacy in their food choices and decisions about the family diet. In studies of individuals living in low-income neighbourhoods higher perceived self-efficacy has been found to be related to greater self-reported consumption of fruit and vegetables(Reference Steptoe, Perkins-Porras and McKay41). Furthermore, short-term increases in the self-efficacy of individuals of low income have been found to predict long-term changes in fruit and vegetable intake(Reference Steptoe, Perkins-Porras and Rink23).

Food-choice values

Values are the enduring beliefs that guide and motivate behaviour and are important in food choices. Research has shown that the most-frequently considered food-related values are health, taste, cost, convenience and managing relationships(Reference Connors, Bisogni and Sobal45). These values are often in conflict. Any value has the potential to be the deciding factor in a given situation, but when conflicts among values occur one typically emerges as dominant. In the case of women of lower educational attainment household harmony is often preserved at the expense of healthy food choices(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30). Values have to be prioritised and compromises made over food choices. Women appear to be more accommodating in this context than men; apparently because they place higher value on maintaining social relationships(Reference Connors, Bisogni and Sobal45).

Health value is a complex multidimensional concept, and work with disadvantaged women suggests that it is not necessarily the dominant value when making food choices(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30). It is clearly a more important consideration in food choices women are making for their children than in the food choices they are making for themselves, which suggests that reinforcing the importance of healthy food choices for her children might have more impact on a woman's quality of diet than stressing its importance for herself.

Mood

An individual's mood may well influence their priorities and values. Psychological studies of mood often concentrate on positive or negative affect and self-esteem. These factors are measurable aspects of psychological well-being and are related to behaviour. It is suggested that individuals rely partly on their affective state in judging their capabilities, so mood can affect judgements of personal efficacy(Reference Bandura46). Thus, a negative affect is likely to reduce an individual's perceived self-efficacy and personal control and thereby reduce the likelihood that they will attempt to undertake an action.

It is suggested that having good food-management skills provides individuals with self-esteem and a feeling of empowerment within the household(Reference Bisogni, Jastran and Shen31), which is likely to have a positive influence on their affective state. Food-management skills appear to be durable resources that help individuals meet personal food-related goals and adapt to changing circumstances, thus providing self-esteem. Other research has shown that dietary habits are related to nutritional attitudes and emotional distress(Reference Hollis, Carmody and Connor47). Using the nutrition attitude scale, which measures attitudes towards the adoption of a low-fat low-cholesterol diet(Reference Hollis, Carmody and Connor47), it was found that participants scoring highly on the ‘helpless and unhealthy’ factor eat more meat, are overweight, report more symptoms of emotional distress and have a history of more medical symptoms. It appears that a cluster of negative food attitudes is related to psychological and nutritional status as well as weight and physiological measures of coronary risk.

Some of the disadvantaged women in focus groups in Southampton have expressed concern with their weight and appearance. They describe not valuing themselves enough to be bothered to make healthy choices for themselves or to go to the trouble of cooking for themselves(Reference Lawrence, Skinner and Haslam30).

Improving diets

The evidence described here suggests that to improve the diets of disadvantaged women it will be necessary to do more than simply educate about the link between diet and health. There is a range of environmental, social, historical and psychological factors that may hinder individuals from using their knowledge about healthy eating to improve their diets. These factors include food preferences stemming from childhood, life skills such as cooking, perceived social support, attitudes and beliefs about health and healthy-eating messages and the ability to put them into practice, as well as cost and access to food(Reference Kelly and Stanner48).

Individuals interact in a variety of micro-environments, such as schools, workplaces, homes and restaurants, which are influenced by broader macro-environments such as the food industry, government and societal attitudes. In the UK most food is still eaten within the home. As family dynamics appear to have an important influence on food choices, there is an argument for using a family-based intervention to encourage change. If no account is taken of the context of food choice and eating events, interventions are unlikely to be successful(Reference Adamson and Mathers49).

Healthy-eating interventions can have an impact on awareness, knowledge and intention to change. However, behaviour is not usually influenced, particularly in those with lower socio-economic status and lower educational attainment(Reference Swinburn, Caterson and Seidell50, Reference Turrell51). The most-vulnerable groups, who are the most in need of change, are the hardest to reach and engage in behaviour change initiatives(Reference Kelly and Stanner48). They are often under-represented in interventions or have higher drop-out rates, so different strategies are required to target these groups. The challenge is to interest them in change if long-term health is not their top priority. Tailored approaches may be more successful, with different approaches for disadvantaged and hard-to-reach groups and for different aspects of diet(Reference Buttriss, Stanner and McKevith52).

A recent study has asked participants ‘What is the single most important thing that you could do, or that could be done, to make it easier for you to eat a healthy diet?’ The most popular responses, in decreasing order, were reported to be: having more time to prepare healthy food; having more fresh or healthy food in the house; having tasty or healthier food alternatives available; greater motivation and self-control; being able to limit sugary snacks and eat more fruit and vegetables(Reference Ball, Crawford and Warren29). These findings clearly demonstrate the need to develop interventions that can address multiple influences on food choice rather than concentrating on changing one factor alone.

Acknowledgements

There are no conflicts of interest. Funding is gratefully acknowledged from the Medical Research Council and Danone Institute International, Palaiseau, France. Both authors have contributed to the literature review, writing and editing of this article.

References

1. Barker, DJP (1998) Mothers, Babies and Health in Later Life, 2nd ed. London: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
2. Barker, DJP (2003) The Best Start in Life, 1st ed. London: Random House.Google Scholar
3. Godfrey, KM, Barker, DJP, Robinson, S et al. (1997) Maternal birthweight and diet in pregnancy in relation to the infant's thinness at birth. Br J Obstet Gynaecol 104, 663667.Google Scholar
4. Rao, S, Yajnik, CS, Kanade, A et al. (2001) Intake of micro-nutrient rich foods in rural Indian mothers is associated with the size of their babies at birth: the Pune Maternal Nutrition Study. J Nutr 131, 12171224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5. Haugen, G, Hanson, M, Kiserud, T et al. (2005) Fetal liver-sparing cardiovascular adaptations linked to mother's slimness and diet. Circulation Res 96, 1214.Google Scholar
6. National Children's Home (1996) Poverty and Nutrition Survey: The Difficulties of Providing an Adequate Diet for Families on Benefits. London: NCH.Google Scholar
7. Dowler, E & Calvert, C (1995) Nutrition and Diet in Lone-parent Families in London. London: Family Policy Studies Centre.Google Scholar
8. Robinson, SM, Crozier, SR, Borland, SE et al. (2004) Impact of educational attainment on the quality of young women's diets. Eur J Clin Nutr 58, 11741180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Lang, T & Heasman, M (2004) Food Wars: The Global Battle for Mouths, Minds and Markets. London: Earthscan.Google Scholar
10. Shepherd, R & Raats, MM (2006) The Psychology of Food Choice. Wallingford, Oxon.: CABI.Google Scholar
11. Shelton, NJ (2005) What not to eat: inequalities in healthy eating behaviour, evidence from the 1998 Scottish Health Survey. J Public Health 27, 3644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Olson, CM (2005) Food insecurity in women: a recipe for unhealthy trade-offs. Top Clin Nutr 20, 321328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13. Sweeting, H & West, P (2005) Dietary habits and children's family lives. J Hum Nutr Diet 18, 9397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Dibsdall, LA, Lambert, N, Bobbin, RF et al. (2003) Low-income consumers' attitudes and behaviour towards access, availability and motivation to eat fruit and vegetables. Public Health Nutr 6, 159168.Google Scholar
15. Barker, M, Lawrence, W, Skinner, TC et al. (2008) Constraints on the food choices of women with lower educational attainment. Public Health Nutr 26, 19.Google Scholar
16. Cummins, S & Macintyre, S (2002) ‘Food deserts’ – evidence and assumption in health policy making. Br Med J 325, 436438.Google Scholar
17. Wrigley, N (2002) ‘Food deserts’ in British cities: policy context, and research priorities. Urban Stud 39, 20292040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18. Whelan, A, Wrigley, N, Warm, D et al. (2002) Life in a ‘food desert’. Urban Stud 39, 20832100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19. Clarke, G, Eyre, H & Guy, C (2002) Deriving indicators of access to food retail provision in British cities: studies of Cardiff, Leeds and Bradford. Urban Stud 39, 20412060.Google Scholar
20. Wrigley, N, Warm, D, Margetts, B et al. (2002) Assessing the impact of improved retail access on diet in a ‘food desert’: a preliminary report. Urban Stud 39, 20612082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21. Worsley, A (2000) Food and consumers: Where are we heading? Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 9, S103S107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Bove, CF, Sobal, J & Rauschenbach, BS (2003) Food choices among newly married couples: convergence, conflict, individualism, and projects. Appetite 40, 2541.Google Scholar
23. Steptoe, A, Perkins-Porras, L, Rink, E et al. (2004) Psychological and social predictors of changes in fruit and vegetable consumption over 12 months following behavioural and nutrition education counseling. Health Psychol 23, 574581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Brown, JL & Miller, D (2002) Couples' gender role preferences and management of family food preferences. J Nutr Educ Behav 34, 215223.Google Scholar
25. Murcott, A (1983) ‘It's a pleasure to cook for him’: food, mealtimes, and gender in some South Wales Households. In The Public and the Private, pp. 7890 [Garmarnikow, E, Morgan, D, Purvis, J and Taylorson, Deditors]. London: Heinmann.Google Scholar
26. Beagan, BL & Chapman, GE (2004) Family influences on food choice: context of surviving breast cancer. J Nutr Educ Behav 36, 320326.Google Scholar
27. Devine, CM, Connors, M, Bisogni, CA et al. (1998) Life-course influences on fruit and vegetable trajectories: qualitative analysis of food choices. J Nut Educ 30, 361370.Google Scholar
28. Connors, M, Bisogni, CA, Sobal, J et al. (2001) Managing values in personal food systems. Appetite 363, 189200.Google Scholar
29. Ball, K, Crawford, D & Warren, N (2004) How feasible are healthy eating and physical activity for young women? Public Health Nutr 7, 433441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30. Lawrence, W, Skinner, TC, Haslam, C et al. (2008) Why women of lower educational attainment struggle to make healthier food choices: the importance of psychological and social factors. Psychol Health (Epublication ahead of print version).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
31. Bisogni, CA, Jastran, M, Shen, L et al. (2005) A biographical study of food choice capacity: standards, circumstances, and food management skills. J Nutr Educ Behav 37, 284291.Google Scholar
32. Devine, CM (2005) A life course perspective: understanding food choices in time, social location, and history. J Nutr Educ Behav 37, 121128.Google Scholar
33. Birch, LL, Gunder, L, Grimm-Thomas, K et al. (1998) Infants' consumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods. Appetite 30, 283295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Sullivan, SA & Birch, LL (1990) Pass the sugar, pass the salt: Experience dictates preference. Dev Psychol 26, 546551.Google Scholar
35. Birch, LL & Marlin, DW (1982) I don't like it; I never tried it: Effects of exposure on 2-year-old children's food preferences. Appetite 3, 353360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36. Birch, LL, Mcphee, L, Shoba, BC et al. (1987) What kind of exposure reduces children's food neophobia? Looking vs tasting. Appetite 9, 171178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Pliner, P (1982) The effects of mere exposure on liking for edible substances. Appetite 3, 283290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. Walker, J (2001) Control and the Psychology of Health: Theory, Measurement and Applications. Maidenhead, Berks.: Open University Press.Google Scholar
39. Wallston, BS, Wallston, KA, Kaplan, GD et al. (1976) Development and validation of the health locus of control (HLC) scale. J Consult Clin Psychol 44, 580585.Google Scholar
40. Wardle, J & Steptoe, A (2003) Socioeconomic differences in attitudes and beliefs about healthy lifestyles. J Epidemiol Community Health 57, 440443.Google Scholar
41. Steptoe, A, Perkins-Porras, L, McKay, C et al. (2003) Psychological factors associated with fruit and vegetable intake and with biomarkers in adults from a low-income neighborhood. Health Psychol 22, 148155.Google Scholar
42. Davies, M (2000) The role of commonsense understandings in social inequalities in health: an investigation in the context of dental health. PhD Thesis, University of Adelaide, Australia.Google Scholar
43. Leganger, A & Kraft, P (2003) Control constructs: do they mediate the relation between educational attainment and health behaviour? J Health Psychol 8, 361372.Google Scholar
44. Bandura, A (1995) Self-efficacy in Changing Societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
45. Connors, M, Bisogni, CA, Sobal, J et al. (2001) Managing values in personal food systems. Appetite 36, 189200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46. Bandura, A (1997) Health promotion from the perspective of social cognition theory. Psychol Health 13, 623649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47. Hollis, JF, Carmody, TP, Connor, SL et al. (1986) The nutrition attitude survey: Associations with dietary habits, psychological and physical well-being, and coronary risk factors. Health Psychol 5, 359374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. Kelly, CNM & Stanner, SA (2003) Diet and cardiovascular disease in the UK: are the messages getting across? Proc Nutr Soc 62, 583589.Google Scholar
49. Adamson, AJ & Mathers, JC (2004) Effecting dietary change. Proc Nutr Soc 63, 537547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50. Swinburn, BA, Caterson, I, Seidell, JC et al. (2004) Diet, nutrition and the prevention of excess weight gain and obesity. Public Health Nutr 7, 123146.Google Scholar
51. Turrell, G (1998) Socioeconomic differences in food preference and their influence on healthy food purchasing choices. J Hum Nutr Diet 11, 135149.Google Scholar
52. Buttriss, J, Stanner, S, McKevith, B et al. (2004) Successful ways to modify food choice: Lessons from the literature. Nutr Bull 29, 333343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar