No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Not just a hijack: Imaginary worlds can enhance individual and group-level fitness
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 18 November 2022
Abstract
Why has fiction been so successful over time? We make the case that fiction may have properties that enhance both individual and group-level fitness by (a) allowing risk-free simulation of important scenarios, (b) effectively transmitting solutions to common problems, and (c) enhancing group cohesion through shared consumption of fictive worlds.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Boyd, B. (2018). The evolution of stories: From mimesis to language, from fact to fiction. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 9(1), e1444.Google ScholarPubMed
Jiménez, Á. V., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Prestige-biased social learning: Current evidence and outstanding questions. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. R. (2012). Transportation into a story increases empathy, prosocial behavior, and perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 150–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. R. (2013). Transportation into literary fiction reduces prejudice against and increases empathy for Arab-Muslims. Scientific Study of Literature, 3(1), 77–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. R., Cushman, G. K., Borden, L. A., & McCune, M. S. (2013). Potentiating empathic growth: Generating imagery while reading fiction increases empathy and prosocial behavior. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. A., Carroll, J., Gottschall, J., & Kruger, D. (2011). Portrayal of personality in Victorian novels reflects modern research findings but amplifies the significance of agreeableness. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 50–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A., Li, J., Nguyen, A. T., & Ta, C. P. (2021). Memory and comprehension of narrative versus expository texts: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(3), 732–749.Google ScholarPubMed
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(3), 173–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97(3), 405–423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin, O., Acerbi, A., & Sobchuk, O. (2019). Why people die in novels: Testing the ordeal simulation hypothesis. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scrivner, C, Johnson, J. A., Kjeldgaard-Christiansen, J, & Clasen, M. (2021). Pandemic practice: Horror fans and morbidly curious individuals are more psychologically resilient during the COVID-19 pandemic. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silveira, A. S. Jr (2021). The power of repetition. In Building and managing high-performance distributed teams (pp. 155–167). Apress.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, D., Schlaepfer, P., Major, K., Dyble, M., Page, A. E., Thompson, J., … Migliano, A. B. (2017). Cooperation and the evolution of hunter-gatherer storytelling. Nature Communications, 8(1), 1–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Why imaginary worlds? The psychological foundations and cultural evolution of fictions with imaginary worlds
Related commentaries (32)
All non-real worlds provide exploration: Evidence from developmental psychology
Am I present in imaginary worlds? Intentions, actions, and flow in mediated experiences and fiction
Autism and the preference for imaginary worlds
Brave new world: Imaginative fictions offer simulated safety and actual benefits
Cognitive exploration drives engagement and re-engagement with imaginary worlds, but not spatial exploration as predicted by evolutionary theory
Determining the markers of a preference for imaginary worlds fiction calls for comparisons across kinds of fiction readers and forms of exploration
Does progressive aphantasia exist? The hypothetical role of aphantasia in the diagnosis of neurodegenerative diseases
Exploration and arrangement in physical and social worlds
How granular should our explanations of fantastic fiction be?
Imaginary worlds are awesome: Awe provides a key to understanding the individual and social functions of imaginary worlds
Imaginary worlds are attractive because they simulate multiple adaptive problems and encode real-world information
Imaginary worlds pervade forager oral tradition
Imaginative processes in children are not particularly imaginative
Imagining our moral values in the present and future
Influences on the development of imaginary worlds
It's not fiction if you believe it: How imaginary worlds are derived from imaginary realities
Middle-earth wasn't built in a day: How do we explain the costs of creating a world?
Not just a hijack: Imaginary worlds can enhance individual and group-level fitness
Simulation, stories, and fictional worlds
Socioecology and fiction
The allure of the unknown in a tamed, mapped, and homogenized world
The call of the final frontier?
The deep history of imaginary worlds
The development of the imagination and imaginary worlds
The evolution of imagination and the adaptive value of imaginary worlds
The familiar appeal of imaginary worlds
Using imaginary worlds for real social benefits
Why do people create imaginary worlds? The case of Fanfiction
Why frightening imaginary worlds? Morbid curiosity and the learning potential of horror
Why imaginary worlds? The role of self-exploration within online gaming worlds
Young children are not driven to explore imaginary worlds
“Never Land”: Where do imaginary worlds come from?
Author response
Imaginary worlds through the evolutionary lens: Ultimate functions, proximate mechanisms, cultural distribution