Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T16:36:35.999Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association of self-reported mother–infant relationship with child and adolescent mental health

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2023

Ida Scheel Rasmussen*
Affiliation:
The Research Unit for General Practice and Section of General Practice, Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Philip Wilson
Affiliation:
The Research Unit for General Practice and Section of General Practice, Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark; and Centre for Rural Health, Institute of Applied Health Sciences, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, UK
Gritt Overbeck
Affiliation:
The Research Unit for General Practice and Section of General Practice, Department of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Katrine Strandberg-Larsen
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Section of Epidemiology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
*
Correspondence: Ida Scheel Rasmussen. Email: idra@sund.ku.dk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The quality of the relationship between mother and infant may have profound implications for the development of a child. Early indicators of psychological vulnerability may allow targeting of support for the child's cognitive, emotional and social development. A challenging mother–infant relationship could be one indicator of risk.

Aims

This study examined variations in psychological well-being and psychopathology among boys and girls according to early maternal perception of the mother–infant relationship.

Method

This study is based on 64 663 mother–infant pairs from the Danish National Birth Cohort, for which data on the mother–infant relationship were collected at 6 months postpartum. Behavioural problems were assessed with the Danish version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) at child ages 7, 11 and 18 years, and we retrieved information on diagnosed childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders and prescriptions of psychotropic drugs from Danish registries.

Results

Children in the challenging mother–infant relationship group had higher odds of behavioural problems at age 7 among both boys and girls. The same pattern of elevated estimates was identified for boys across all SDQ domains and for girls in three of five SDQ domains. All associations were attenuated at age 18, but increased odds of behavioural problems still existed. A challenging early mother–infant relationship increased the offspring's risk of being diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder or being prescribed a psychotropic drug before the age of 18.

Conclusion

A challenging self-reported mother–infant relationship was associated with later psychopathological difficulties. Routine clinical enquiry may be useful in identification of future vulnerability.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

The relationship between parents and their infants has profound implications for a child's development. A healthy and positive relationship creates an environment that nurtures the infant's physical, cognitive, emotional and social development.Reference Apter, Devouche and Gratier1 Secure attachment occurs when the child's needs for security, calm and understanding are met and allows for optimal brain development in most circumstances.Reference Bowlby2 Secure attachment is strongly related to numerous positive child outcomes,Reference Sroufe, Carlson, Levy and Egeland3 whereas insecure attachment often leaves infants less able to handle stress and more vulnerable to other adverse behavioural and emotional outcomes.Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn and Juffer4 Equally importantly, impairments in the infant's ability to self-regulate – evidenced by, for example, prolonged crying, sleep disorders and feeding problems – have been associated with behavioural problems, particularly externalising problems, in childhood and adolescence.Reference Hemmi, Wolke and Schneider5 The causal mechanisms and the association between the parent–child relationship and later behavioural problems are neither simple nor direct. Although it is indisputable that the infant's ability to self-regulate affects both the well-being of parents and the parent–infant relationship, it has been demonstrated that parental characteristics including levels of distress and overall psychological functioning are also important factors.Reference Smarius, Strieder, Loomans, Doreleijers, Vrijkotte and Gemke6 Numerous longitudinal studies have examined the relationship between different dimensions of the parent-child relationship and either behavioural problems or psychiatric disorders in childhood. Results show in general that poor parent–infant attachment and dysfunctional parent–infant relationship are associated with behavioural problems and psychopathology in the offspring.Reference Allely, Johnson, Marwick, Lidstone, Kočovská and Puckering7,Reference Puckering, Allely, Doolin, Purves, McConnachie and Johnson8 Very few studies have examined the association between the parent–infant relationship and the offspring's behavioural problems and/or psychiatric diagnoses through to adolescence.Reference Schmid, Blomeyer, Buchmann, Trautmann-Villalba, Zimmermann and Schmidt9

Objective

The primary objective of this study was to explore the association between the maternally perceived mother–infant relationship and the offspring's behavioural problems at different times during childhood and adolescence. The secondary objective was to examine the association between the self-reported mother–infant relationship and the offspring's risk of diagnosed psychopathology before the age of 18. All analyses were carried out separately for boys and girls, as there is a substantial literature describing differences in maternal perceptions of and behaviour towards boys and girls.Reference Raikes and Thompson10 There is a significant interaction between gender and maternal behaviour, such that maternal behaviour was more strongly related to attachment security for girls than for boys. At a population level, there are also substantial differences in behavioural measures between boys and girls.Reference Barry, Marryat, Thompson, Ellaway, White and McClung11

Method

We used data from the Danish National Birth Cohort (DNBC).Reference Olsen, Melbye, Olsen, Sørensen, Aaby and Andersen12 The DNBC is a longitudinal nationwide cohort and includes 96 824 children enrolled during pregnancy, corresponding to around 30% of children born in Denmark between 1996 and 2003. For more information and data available, visit dnbc.dk. The protocol for this study is registered at The Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/pjkuq/).

Participants

Pregnant women who had their first antenatal visit in general practice between 1996 and 2002 were offered participation in the cohort. After inclusion, the women were interviewed by telephone twice during pregnancy and twice postpartum. The participants were invited to answer follow-up questionnaires when the children were 7, 11 and 18 years of age. All codebooks are available at DNBC.DK. All mother–infant pairs, in which the pregnancy resulted in a live, full-term (gestational age ≥37 completed weeks) singleton birth were eligible for this study (n = 88 932). We excluded mother–child pairs without information from 6 months postpartum (n = 22 421). In addition, we excluded children with congenital anomalies (n = 1078) and those diagnosed with conditions affecting the central nervous system (n = 179) or global developmental delay (n = 545). We excluded these children because these relatively rare conditions could cause behaviour changes independent of the mother–child interaction. Finally, mothers responding ‘don't know’ or ‘don't want to answer’ to the question regarding their relationship with their child were omitted from the analyses (n = 46), leaving a total of 64 663 mother–infant pairs (Supplementary material 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2023.4).

Ethics

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Ethical approval was granted by the Committee on Biomedical Research Ethics under case no. (KF) 01-471/94, and the cohort was approved under ref. no 2008-54-0431 by Danish Data Protection Agency. The pregnant women who participated in the study provided informed written consent for themselves and their children to be followed to their legal age of consent. At 18 years of age, the participants born into the DNBC were contacted about their participation and their rights and given information about withdrawal from the study. This study was approved by the DNBC steering committee (ref. 2020–06) and by the University of Copenhagen with case no. 514-0488/20-3000.

Measures

Mother–infant relationship

Assessment of the maternally perceived mother–infant relationship was based on responses to the question: ‘How have you experienced the task of taking care of your child’ at 6 months postpartum. We generated an outcome with two possible values: ‘easy mother–infant relationship’, combining the response options ‘very easy’ and ‘fairly easy’, versus ‘challenging mother–infant relationship’, combining the response options ‘difficult’ and ‘very difficult’ (described in detail elsewhere: https://osf.io/pjkuq/). We chose the terms ‘challenging’ and ‘easy’ in our categorisation, as our measure was self-reported and our question reflects the mother's perception.

Behavioural problems: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

Validated Danish versions of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) were completed at 7, 11 and 18 years of age.Reference Goodman13 The SDQ is a screening tool designed to assess five areas of social–behavioural development in children and adolescents: emotional symptoms, hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, peer relationship problems and prosocial behaviour. It can be completed by parents, caregivers, teachers, or children and/or adolescents themselves.Reference Goodman13 The SDQ comprises 25 questions, each of which is rated on a three-point Likert scale. We analysed behavioural difficulties in the five subscales and the total difficulties scale (summed from the four problem subscales) with two possible values: raised or average. The raised group was defined by cut-off points identifying the 10% with the highest problem score in each follow-up. At age 11, the behavioural problem scores were based on a multi-respondent measure compositing the parent-, teacher- and self-reported responses, whereas the 7 and 18 year follow-ups were based on single-respondent data from a parent and the adolescent, respectively; for more detailed information, see Supplementary material 2.

Child psychiatric disorders

We retrieved any records of diagnosed child or adolescent psychiatric disorder from the Danish health registries. All live-born children in Denmark are assigned a unique civil registration number that is linked to the National Patient Register.Reference Lynge, Sandegaard and Rebolj14 We obtained data on psychiatric disorders from both in-patient admissions and out-patient contacts. We were interested first in main diagnoses in any of the following diagnostic groups: any psychiatric disorder (ICD-10: F00-F99); schizophrenia, schizotypal and delusional disorders (ICD-10: F20 – F29); mood disorders, anxiety, dissociative, stress-related, somatoform and other nonpsychotic mental disorders (ICD-10: F30-F45, F48, F93); autism spectrum disorders (ICD-10: F84 not: F84.2-F84.4); personality disorders (F60-F69); hyperkinetic disorders (ICD-10: F90, F98.9); and conduct disorders (ICD-10: F91, F90.1).15 Thus, the disorder-specific diagnostic groups were not mutually exclusive. We formed a group of children and adolescents with, presumably, milder mental disordersReference Mors, Perto and Mortensen16 who had redeemed two or more prescriptions of psychotropic drugs without being registered with a psychiatric diagnosis (ACT groups: N05, N06A, N06B, N06C).Reference Wallach Kildemoes, Toft Sørensen and Hallas17

Covariates

Potential confounders were chosen a priori based on empirical evidence and on a directed acyclic graph (Supplementary material 3). We included the following variables as potential confounders: parity, maternal psychiatric diagnosis, paternal psychiatric diagnosis, maternal educational level, prenatal alcohol consumption, low weight for gestational ageReference Maršál, Persson, Larsen, Lilja, Selbing and Sultan18 and colic, in accordance with previous studies.Reference Wessel, Cobb, Jackson, Harris and Detwiler19 Information on the potential confounders was obtained by self-report in the interviews or from national registries (described in detail elsewhere: https://osf.io/pjkuq/).

Statistical analyses

We used logistic regression to estimate the odds of the behavioural problem outcomes in children in each subscale of the SDQ according to the reported mother–infant relationship. All three follow-ups were analysed separately. Selective attrition was adjusted for using inverse probability weighting, where the propensity score was predicted by maternal psychiatric diagnosis, paternal psychiatric diagnosis, parity, marital status, maternal age, alcohol consumption during pregnancy and maternal educational level.

Cox proportional hazards regression was used to estimate hazard ratios for the reported mother–child relationship and subsequent offspring psychiatric diagnoses for each of the diagnostic groups. Person-time follow-up was started at the child's first birthday and ended at first psychiatric diagnosis within each diagnostic group, or alternatively at death, migration or end of follow-up. End of follow-up was the adolescent's 18th birthday or the last date of record linkage, 31 December 2018. Children who immigrated before their first birthday were not included in the analysis. We estimated both crude and adjusted ratios and 95% confidence intervals for all analyses. All analyses were conducted separately for boys and girls.

Sensitivity and subgroup analyses

We performed sensitivity analyses to examine differences attributable to respondent type at the 11 year follow-up. Information on alcohol consumption in the main analyses was taken in prioritised order from the interview at 12 weeks’ gestation, the 30th week of gestation or postpartum. We conducted an analysis restricted to mothers with information on alcohol consumption reported at 12 weeks of gestation owing to risk of recall bias. We computed estimates separately for primiparous and multiparous mothers with thoughts on potential differences in anxiety or stress levels, leisure, parental confidence, etc.

Results

Ninety-five per cent of the included mothers reported an easy mother–infant relationship. The 5% of the mothers who reported a challenging mother–infant relationship were younger, less educated, more likely to be nulliparous, more likely to be single or unmarried, and more often had a psychiatric diagnosis compared with mothers reporting an easy mother–infant relationship. Infants in the challenging mother–infant relationships were more likely to be boys, to have had symptoms of colic and to have been born small for gestational age (Table 1).

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of study population according to maternally reported mother–infant relationship

a. Psychiatric diagnoses before birth of DNBC child.

b. χ²-test.

We observed a higher prevalence of behavioural problems at 7 and/or at 11 years of age in the group where the mothers reported a challenging mother–infant relationship (Table 2). Behavioural problems at 18 years of age were more evenly divided between the two mother–infant relationship groups. However, the prevalence of at least one psychiatric disorder before the age of 18 or redeemed psychotropic drugs was higher among those with a challenging mother–infant relationship (Table 2).

Table 2 Prevalence of increased behavioural problems defined by the total difficulties scale score of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and child and adolescent psychiatric disorders according to maternally perceived mother–infant relationship

a. Combined measure – z-scores.

b. Children/adolescents with redemptions of two or more prescriptions of psychotropic drugs without having a registered psychiatric diagnosis.

Behavioural difficulties

A challenging mother–infant relationship was associated with behavioural problems across all five subdomains and for both sexes at age 7 years. Odds of behavioural problems were 1.7-fold higher among both boys and girls when the mother perceived the mother–infant relationship as challenging compared with mothers reporting an easy mother–infant relationship. We found the same pattern of elevated estimates for boys at age 11. However, for girls at 11 years of age we found an elevated risk of emotional problems, hyperactivity and peer problems, but prosocial behaviour and conduct problems were not associated with a challenging mother–infant relation. At 18 years of age, all associations were clearly attenuated, and among boys we only observed indications for increased odds of emotional problems (Fig. 1 and Supplementary material 4). Among girls, all domains had adjusted point estimates above 1 (Fig. 1 and Supplementary material 4). Analyses adjusting for selective attrition based on inverse probability weights (Supplementary material 5) did not change our results notably.

Fig. 1 Association between mother–infant relationship at 6 months postpartum and behavioural problems at ages 7, 11 and 18 years, as estimated by logistic regression models. Asterisks indicate values adjusted for parity, maternal psychiatric diagnosis at birth, paternal psychiatric diagnosis at birth, maternal educational level at birth, alcohol consumption, small for gestational age and colic.

Psychiatric disorder during childhood and/or adolescence

For both sexes, a challenging early mother–infant relationship was associated with an approximately 30% increased rate of psychiatric diagnoses during childhood and adolescence (Table 3). Among males, the increased rate of any psychiatric disorder was primarily attributable to an increased rate of hyperkinetic disorders, although we found elevated point estimates in schizophrenia, schizotypal and delusional disorders, personality disorders and conduct disorders, all estimated on relatively few cases. Girls in a challenging relationship had increased rates for all diagnostic subgroups of psychiatric disorders compared with those with an easy mother–infant-relationship. The rate was either close to double or greater for hyperkinetic disorders, autism and conduct disorders among the girls, but the latter was based on rare events. For psychiatric problems identified by psychotropic drugs alone, we did not find elevated point estimates for boys in the challenging mother–infant group. We found more than a doubled rate for girls in the challenging mother–infant group.

Table 3 Hazard ratios for psychiatric diagnosis according to maternally perceived mother–infant relationship

HR, hazard ratio.

a. Adjusted for parity, maternal psychiatric diagnosis at birth, paternal psychiatric diagnosis at birth, maternal educational level at birth, alcohol consumption, small for gestational age and colic.

b. Mean age in years at first diagnosis/initial treatment.

c. Children/adolescents with redemptions of two or more prescriptions of psychotropic drugs without having a registered psychiatric diagnosis.

*P < 0.05.

Adjustments for the selected covariates had a minor impact on the results and most often slightly attenuated the estimated associations.

Sensitivity and subgroup analyses

When restricting the outcome at age 11 years to parent-reported SDQ scores, we observed a stronger and more significant association between the mother–infant relationship and behavioural problems across all domains except for conduct problems, compared with restricting to either teacher-reported or self-reported SDQ scores for the boys. The results were mixed for the girls; restricting the outcome to self-reported SDQ scores gave a stronger association between the hyperactivity scale and total behavioural problems than restricting the outcome to parent-reported SDQ scores. Likewise, we observed higher odds of emotional and peer problems for parent-reported SDQ scores than for teacher- and self-reported scores (Supplementary material 6). Restricting the analyses to participants with alcohol consumption reported in the earliest pregnancy interview had no impact on our findings. We found no effect of excluding mothers with information on alcohol consumption only reported at 12 weeks of gestation. In the analyses where we examined the children of primiparous and multiparous mothers separately, we observed that the association was marginally stronger for multiparous mothers compared with primipara except for boys aged 7 and girls aged 18 years. We found a stronger association for multiparous mothers compared with primipara in a few of the diagnostic groups, but this finding was not consistent (Supplementary material 7 and 8).

Discussion

Our findings showed that a challenging mother–infant relationship was associated with increased odds of total behavioural difficulties in childhood at ages 7 and 11 years for both boys and girls in a general population. We found an increased risk of behavioural problems in all subdomains for both sexes at age 7, and for boys at age 11, but only emotional, hyperactivity and peer problems among girls at age 11. The association between both behavioural problems and a challenging mother–infant relationship were attenuated with age and the association became less clear at age 18, although we could not rule out an association for girls. Increased rates of psychiatric diagnoses were observed for the offspring of mothers who had reported a challenging mother–infant relationship. This applied to all of the diagnostic groups for girls but was mainly present for hyperkinetic disorders and conduct problems in boys. A doubling of the receipt of psychotropic drugs was found for girls in the challenging mother–infant relationship group.

The obvious strengths of this study were its prospective design, the large sample size, and the repeated and long-term follow-up data on behavioural problems together with the multi-respondent reporting at 11 years of age. It is also important to highlight that the unique linked administrative data-set allowed us to conduct weighted analyses to compensate for the attrition that exists in all cohorts. Finally, we were able to report both symptoms of behavioural problems and psychiatric diagnoses.

Some limitations are important to consider when interpreting our results. Our biggest concern was our single subjective source of information about the mother–infant relationship causing potential informant bias. This measure is a non-validated source of information about the mother–infant relationship, and it is therefore important to note that this is an overall perception of the relationship between a mother and her infant. We did not consider the background (e.g. feeding problems) of her perception or the actual situation. However, previous studies have provided compelling evidence for an association between mothers’ self-reported relationship with their infant and many aspects of parenting.Reference Jones, Cassidy and Shaver20 The generalisability of the findings is somewhat limited by the underrepresentation of families with low socioeconomic position in the DNBC compared with the Danish population.Reference Jacobsen, Nohr and Frydenberg21 We perceived the loss to follow-up as a minor limitation owing to our weighted analyses and previous studies’ conclusions about the low impact of selective drop-out in predictive models of behavioural problems.Reference Wolke, Waylen, Samara, Steer, Goodman and Ford22

This study joins a strong body of research that aims to clarify the link between early mother–infant interaction and/or attachment and the child's risk for psychopathology later in life. In general, our findings are consistent with those of others in suggesting that the mother–infant relationship can be linked to behaviour problems in childhood.Reference Allely, Johnson, Marwick, Lidstone, Kočovská and Puckering7,Reference Puckering, Allely, Doolin, Purves, McConnachie and Johnson8,Reference Mäntymaa, Puura, Luoma, Salmelin and Tamminen23 Other studies have also found an increased risk of receiving psychiatric diagnoses during childhood and adolescence if the mother–infant relationship was challenging.Reference Schmid, Blomeyer, Buchmann, Trautmann-Villalba, Zimmermann and Schmidt9 We have not been able to identify any studies with the same follow-up duration between assessment of mother–infant relationship and the offspring's behavioural problems as that of the present study. We believe that this study is unique as it is the first longitudinal examination indicating some type of long-term association, at least for girls. Previous studies have either focused on behavioural problems during early childhood or have examined diagnosed psychiatric disorders alone.Reference O'Connor, Collins and Supplee24Reference Nofech-Mozes, Pereira, Gonzalez and Atkinson31

Our findings yield as many questions as answers regarding the maternal role in the development of behavioural problems and psychiatric diagnoses, and there are multiple potential explanations for our findings. Interpretation of our findings is complicated by the mothers’ direct involvement in both the mother–infant relationship assessment at 6 months postpartum and the measurement of behavioural problems at ages 7 and 11 years, which potentially introduced common method bias. Attenuation of the association at 11 years when excluding mother reports, and at age 18 years solely based on self-report, indicate that the prevalence of children's behavioural problems may be overrated among mothers who perceived a challenging mother–infant relationship. We cannot exclude the possibility that the estimates in the main analyses reflect maternal personality to some extent. Gartstein et al (2009) referred to this phenomenon as the depression-distortion effect,Reference Gartstein, Bridgett, Dishion and Kaufman32 whereas Van der Toorn et al (2010) showed that maternal depression does not bias maternal reporting of children's behavioural problems.Reference van der Toorn, Huizink, Utens, Verhulst, Ormel and Ferdinand33 In addition, we could not isolate the extent to which the mother's mental traits, e.g. distress, were internalised by the offspring, thereby influencing the child's self-concept.

The discrepancy between the 7 and 11 year follow-ups in contrast to the 18 year follow-up is likely to be attributable to the lack of direct maternal input to measurement at 18 years of age, but other potential explanations exist. First, our three follow-up assessments covered given periods of life, and the social and educational environment may become more or less well tailored to the individual's needs later in the school system and where cognitive development proceeds apace. Adolescents experiencing behavioural problems may choose circumstances where their behavioural problems are either less important or can be tolerated to a higher degree, which would affect self-reported SDQ scores because comparison is likely to be in relation to peers. Second, adolescents with behavioural problems may join peer groups with similar behavioural difficulties, potentially influencing self-reporting.

Some studies have suggested that emotional regulation problems tend to persist and convert into other regulatory and/or behavioural problems later,Reference Kim34 whereas others have discounted this relationship.Reference Bell, Hiscock, Tobin, Cook and Sung35 With this in mind, we cannot completely reject the possibility that our findings reflect and catch the association between colic, for example, and later psychopathology. Colic was included in our analyses as a potential confounder, and it generally abates before the age of 3 months, so we believe that colic is unlikely to have been the main driver of our findings.

All analyses were conducted separately for girls and boys. Sex-based differences exist in psychopathologyReference Goodman, Ford, Corbin and Meltzer36,Reference Frank37 and SDQ-scores,Reference Niclasen, Teasdale, Andersen, Skovgaard, Elberling and Obel38,Reference Madsen, Ravn, Arnfred, Olsen, Rask and Obel39 and a previous study conducted by Thomson et al demonstrated that male children were more likely to be negatively parented than girls.Reference Thomson, Allely, Purves, Puckering, McConnachie and Johnson40 Our findings did not reveal large consistent differences between boys and girls when comparing results across the five SDQ domains. However, we found that a challenging mother–infant relationship predicted psychiatric diagnoses in several of the diagnostic groups among girls, whereas a challenging mother–infant relationship mainly predicted hyperactivity among boys. We found slight indications of maternal parity as a moderator, suggesting that multiparous mothers reporting challenging relationships were slightly more likely to have children with later behavioural problems than primiparous mothers.

In summary, our study provides evidence for an association between the early mother–infant relationship as reported by the mother and the child's psychopathology. Despite the limitations, our findings should be considered from a clinical perspective: a dysfunctional self-reported mother–infant relationship, reported as early as infancy, may be an early indicator of potential future social, emotional or behavioural problems, or diagnosed psychopathology. Regardless of the reason for experiencing challenges in early infancy, this may be of importance when targeting resources towards mother–infant dyads in the general population.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2023.4.

Data availability

Researchers can apply for use of the data at http://www.dnbc.dk.

Acknowledgments

The DNBC was established with a significant grant from the Danish National Research Foundation. Additional support was obtained from the Danish Regional Committees, the Pharmacy Foundation, the Egmont Foundation, the March of Dimes Birth Defects Foundation, the Health Foundation and other minor grants. The DNBC Biobank has been supported by the Novo Nordisk Foundation and the Lundbeck Foundation. Follow-up of mothers and children have been supported by the Danish Medical Research Council (SSVF 0646, 271-08-0839/06-066023, O602-01042B, 0602-02738B), the Lundbeck Foundation (195/04, R100-A9193), The Innovation Fund Denmark 0603-00294B (09-067124), the Nordea Foundation (02-2013-2014), Aarhus Ideas (AU R9-A959-13-S804), University of Copenhagen Strategic Grant (IFSV 2012), and the Danish Council for Independent Research (DFF – 4183-00594 and DFF – 4183-00152).

Authors contributions

P.W., K.S.-L. and G.O. were involved in planning and supervised the work, and I.S.R. handled the data, performed the analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors interpreted the results and worked and commented on the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by TRYGfonden (grant number 125227).

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Apter, G, Devouche, E, Gratier, M. Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology: Volume I: Infancy. Springer International Publishing, 2019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. Behav Brain Sci 1979; 2(4): 637–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, LA, Carlson, EA, Levy, AK, Egeland, B. Implications of attachment theory for developmentalpsychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 1999; 11(1): 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, Van IJzendoorn, MH, Juffer, F. Disorganized infant attachment and preventive interventions: a review and meta-analysis. Infant Ment Health J 2005; 26(3): 191216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemmi, MH, Wolke, D, Schneider, S. Associations between problems with crying, sleeping and/or feeding in infancy and long-term behavioural outcomes in childhood: a meta-analysis. Arch Dis Child 2011; 96(7): 622–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smarius, LJCA, Strieder, TGA, Loomans, EM, Doreleijers, TAH, Vrijkotte, TGM, Gemke, RJ, et al. Excessive infant crying doubles the risk of mood and behavioral problems at age 5: evidence for mediation by maternal characteristics. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2017; 26(3): 293302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allely, CS, Johnson, PC, Marwick, H, Lidstone, E, Kočovská, E, Puckering, C, et al. Prediction of 7-year psychopathology from mother-infant joint attention behaviours: a nested case–control study. BMC Pediatr 2013; 13(1): 147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puckering, C, Allely, CS, Doolin, O, Purves, D, McConnachie, A, Johnson, PC, et al. Association between parent-infant interactions in infancy and disruptive behaviour disorders at age seven: a nested, case–control ALSPAC study. BMC Pediatr 2014; 14(1): 223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, B, Blomeyer, D, Buchmann, AF, Trautmann-Villalba, P, Zimmermann, US, Schmidt, MH, et al. Quality of early mother–child interaction associated with depressive psychopathology in the offspring: a prospective study from infancy to adulthood. J Psychiatr Res 2011; 45(10): 1387–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raikes, HA, Thompson, RA. Links between risk and attachment security: models of influence. J Appl Dev Psychol 2005; 26(4): 440–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barry, SJE, Marryat, L, Thompson, L, Ellaway, A, White, J, McClung, M, et al. Mapping area variability in social and behavioural difficulties among Glasgow pre-schoolers: linkage of a survey of pre-school staff with routine monitoring data: mapping area variability. Child Care Health Dev 2015; 41(6): 853–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen, J, Melbye, M, Olsen, SF, Sørensen, TI, Aaby, P, Andersen, AM, et al. The Danish national birth cohort–its background, structure and aim. Scand J Public Health 2001; 29(4): 300–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40(11): 1337–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynge, E, Sandegaard, JL, Rebolj, M. The Danish national patient register. Scand J Public Health 2011; 39(7_suppl): 30–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. WHO, 1993.Google Scholar
Mors, O, Perto, GP, Mortensen, PB. The Danish psychiatric central research register. Scand J Public Health 2011; 39(7_suppl): 54–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallach Kildemoes, H, Toft Sørensen, H, Hallas, J. The Danish national prescription registry. Scand J Public Health 2011; 39(7_suppl): 3841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maršál, K, Persson, PH, Larsen, T, Lilja, H, Selbing, A, Sultan, B. Intrauterine growth curves based on ultrasonically estimated foetal weights. Acta Paediatr 1996; 85(7): 843–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wessel, MA, Cobb, JC, Jackson, EB, Harris, GS, Detwiler, AC. Paroxysmal fussing in infancy, sometimes called colic. Pediatrics 1954; 14(5): 421–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, JD, Cassidy, J, Shaver, PR. Parents’ self-reported attachment styles: a review of links with parenting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 2015; 19(1): 4476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobsen, TN, Nohr, EA, Frydenberg, M. Selection by socioeconomic factors into the Danish national birth cohort. Eur J Epidemiol 2010; 25(5): 349–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolke, D, Waylen, A, Samara, M, Steer, C, Goodman, R, Ford, T, et al. Selective drop-out in longitudinal studies and non-biased prediction of behaviour disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195(3): 249–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäntymaa, M, Puura, K, Luoma, I, Salmelin, RK, Tamminen, T. Mother's early perception of her infant's difficult temperament, parenting stress and early mother–infant interaction. Nord J Psychiatry 2006; 60(5): 379–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, EE, Collins, BA, Supplee, L. Behavior problems in late childhood: the roles of early maternal attachment and teacher–child relationship trajectories. Attach Hum Dev 2012; 14(3): 265–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasco Fearon, RM, Belsky, J. Infant-mother attachment and the growth of externalizing problems across the primary-school years: attachment and externalizing problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52(7): 782–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, KB, Marshall, PJ, Rubin, KH, Fox, NA. Infant attachment and temperament as predictors of subsequent externalizing problems and cardiac physiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003; 44(6): 819–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vondra, JI, Shaw, DS, Swearingen, L, Cohen, M, Owens, EB. Attachment stability and emotional and behavioral regulation from infancy to preschool age. Dev Psychopathol 2001; 13(1): 1333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrell, J, Murray, L. Parenting and the development of conduct disorder and hyperactive symptoms in childhood: a prospective longitudinal study from 2 months to 8 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003; 44(4): 489508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhold, M, Laucht, M, Texdorf, C, Schmidt, MH, Esser, G. Early mother–infant interaction as a precursor to childhood social withdrawal. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2002; 32(4): 277–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skovgaard, AM, Olsen, EM, Christiansen, E, Houmann, T, Landorph, SL, Jørgensen, T, et al. Predictors (0–10 months) of psychopathology at age 1½ years – a general population study in the Copenhagen Child Cohort CCC 2000. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49(5): 553–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nofech-Mozes, J, Pereira, J, Gonzalez, A, Atkinson, L. Cortisol secretion moderates the association between mother-infant attachment at 17 months and child behavior at age 5 years. Dev Psychobiol 2019; 61(2): 239–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, MA, Bridgett, DJ, Dishion, TJ, Kaufman, NK. Depressed mood and maternal report of child behavior problems: another look at the depression–distortion hypothesis. J Appl Dev Psychol 2009; 30(2): 149–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Toorn, SLM, Huizink, AC, Utens, EMWJ, Verhulst, FC, Ormel, J, Ferdinand, RF. Maternal depressive symptoms, and not anxiety symptoms, are associated with positive mother–child reporting discrepancies of internalizing problems in children: a report on the TRAILS Study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 19(4): 379–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, JS. Excessive crying: behavioral and emotional regulation disorder in infancy. Korean J Pediatr 2011; 54(6): 229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, G, Hiscock, H, Tobin, S, Cook, F, Sung, V. Behavioral outcomes of infant colic in toddlerhood: a longitudinal study. J Pediatr 2018; 201: 154–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R, Ford, T, Corbin, T, Meltzer, H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) multi-informant algorithm to screen looked-after children for psychiatric disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004; 13(S2): ii25–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, E. Gender and its Effects on Psychopathology. American Psychiatric Press, 2000.Google Scholar
Niclasen, J, Teasdale, TW, Andersen, AMN, Skovgaard, AM, Elberling, H, Obel, C. Psychometric properties of the Danish Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire: the SDQ assessed for more than 70,000 raters in four different cohorts. PLoS ONE 2012; 7(2): e32025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madsen, KB, Ravn, MH, Arnfred, J, Olsen, J, Rask, CU, Obel, C. Characteristics of undiagnosed children with parent-reported ADHD behaviour. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2018; 27(2): 149–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, RM, Allely, CS, Purves, D, Puckering, C, McConnachie, A, Johnson, PC, et al. Predictors of positive and negative parenting behaviours: evidence from the ALSPAC cohort. BMC Pediatr 2014; 14(1): 247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of study population according to maternally reported mother–infant relationship

Figure 1

Table 2 Prevalence of increased behavioural problems defined by the total difficulties scale score of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and child and adolescent psychiatric disorders according to maternally perceived mother–infant relationship

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Association between mother–infant relationship at 6 months postpartum and behavioural problems at ages 7, 11 and 18 years, as estimated by logistic regression models. Asterisks indicate values adjusted for parity, maternal psychiatric diagnosis at birth, paternal psychiatric diagnosis at birth, maternal educational level at birth, alcohol consumption, small for gestational age and colic.

Figure 3

Table 3 Hazard ratios for psychiatric diagnosis according to maternally perceived mother–infant relationship

Supplementary material: File

Rasmussen et al. supplementary material

Rasmussen et al. supplementary material

Download Rasmussen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 437.2 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.