No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Affluence boosted intelligence? How the interaction between cognition and environment may have produced an eighteenth-century Flynn effect during the Industrial Revolution
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 20 November 2019
Abstract
Cognition played a pivotal role in the acceleration of technological innovation during the Industrial Revolution. Growing affluence may have provided favourable environmental conditions for a boost in cognition, enabling individuals to tackle more complex (industrial) problems. Dynamical systems thinking may provide useful tools to describe sudden transitions like the Industrial Revolution, by modelling the recursive feedback between psychology and environment.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019
References
Dickens, W. T. & Flynn, J. R. (2001) Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review 108:346–69.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1987) Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin 101:171–91.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2007) What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J. & Meaney, M. J. (2010) Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(9):651–59.Google Scholar
Haushofer, J. & Fehr, E. (2014) On the psychology of poverty. Science 344(6186):862–67. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1232491.Google Scholar
Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G. & Gage, F. H. (1997) More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 386(6624):493–95.Google Scholar
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E. & Zhao, J. (2013) Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science 341(6149):976–80. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1238041.Google Scholar
Scheffer, M. (2009) Critical transitions in nature and society. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Scheffer, M., Bascompte, J., Brock, W. A., Brovkin, V., Carpenter, S. R., Dakos, V., Held, H., van Nes, E. H., Rietkerk, M. & Sugihara, G. (2009) Early-warning signals for critical transitions. Nature 461:53–59.Google Scholar
Van Der Maas, H. L., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P., Wicherts, J. M., Huizenga, H. M. & Raijmakers, M. E. (2006) A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review 113:842–61.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Borsboom, D. & Dolan, C. V. (2009) Why national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences 48: 91–96.Google Scholar
Target article
Psychological origins of the Industrial Revolution
Related commentaries (24)
A claim for cognitive history
A needed amendment that explains too much and resolves little
Affluence boosted intelligence? How the interaction between cognition and environment may have produced an eighteenth-century Flynn effect during the Industrial Revolution
Are both necessity and opportunity the mothers of innovations?
Cultural interconnectedness and in-group cooperation as sources of innovation
Energy, transport, and consumption in the Industrial Revolution
England first, America second: The ecological predictors of life history and innovation
Environmental unpredictability, economic inequality, and dynamic nature of life history before, during, and after the Industrial Revolution
Explaining historical change in terms of LHT: A pluralistic causal framework is needed
Interrelationships of factors of social development are more complex than Life History Theory predicts
Life History Theory and economic modernity
Life History Theory and the Industrial Revolution
Many causes, not one
Psychological origins of the Industrial Revolution: Why we need causal methods and historians
Psychology and the economics of invention
Slowing life history (K) can account for increasing micro-innovation rates and GDP growth, but not macro-innovation rates, which declined following the end of the Industrial Revolution
The affective origins of the Industrial Revolution
The other angle of Maslow's pyramid: How scarce environments trigger low-opportunity-cost innovations
The wealth→life history→innovation account of the Industrial Revolution is largely inconsistent with empirical time series data
There is little evidence that the Industrial Revolution was caused by a preference shift
Timing is everything: Evaluating behavioural causal theories of Britain's industrialisation
Using big data to map the relationship between time perspectives and economic outputs
What came first, the chicken or the egg?
What motivated the Industrial Revolution: England's libertarian culture or affluence per se?
Author response
Psychological origins of the Industrial Revolution: More work is needed!