Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T15:18:31.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The psychology of intergroup relations was grounded in intragroup processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 January 2024

R. Matthew Montoya*
Affiliation:
Murdoch University, Murdoch, WA, Australia matthew.montoya@murdoch.edu.au
Brad Pinter
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University, Altoona, PA, USA tbp1@psu.edu
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

Although Glowacki proposed that peace developed from the relatively recent advent of intergroup norms and tolerance for out-group members, we submit that (a) positive intergroup relations developed from a psychology grounded in the regulation of intragroup relations, (b) the “default” intergroup orientation is uncertainty, and (c) positive intergroup relations likely existed early in our evolutionary history.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Glowacki provides an interesting account regarding the conditions necessary for peaceful intergroup relations. On a behavioral level, the account makes sense, but it falls short when describing the underlying psychological processes. Glowacki submits that the key to peace is the ability to predict the behaviors of both in-group and out-group members, a process that is facilitated by the presence of group-level norms. However, this explanation omits processes critical to understanding the origin of peaceful intergroup relations.

One account for the development of human sociality is that positive intergroup relations developed from the processes that governed intragroup processes. Indeed, numerous theorists, beginning with Darwin (Reference Darwin1859, Reference Darwin1871), have proposed that the challenges associated with regulating interactions with other persons were preeminent in guiding the development of the hominid psyche (e.g., Alexander & Noonan, Reference Alexander, Noonan, Chagnon and Irons1979; Bigelow, Reference Bigelow1969; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton and Fox1975; Humphrey, Reference Humphrey, Bareson and Hinde1976; Wilson, Reference Wilson1973). The origins of the psychology of intragroup relations likely had their roots in simpler interindividual relationships. Starting with the first facilitative bipeds and the ability to gather, transport, and accrue resources (e.g., last common ancestor; Ardipithecus ramidus; 5.8–5.2 million years ago; WoldeGabriel et al., Reference WoldeGabriel, Ambrose, Barboni, Bonnefille, Bremond, Currie and White2009), the social structure changed to include sharing and cofeeding (Belisle & Chapais, Reference Belisle and Chapais2001). These changes fueled the growth of males' monopolization of access to females, and the physical proximity led males to spend more time with them (and her offspring) to adopt a stabilizing reproductive strategy (Grueter, Chapais, & Zinner, Reference Grueter, Chapais and Zinner2012). Parent–offspring and offspring–parent recognition and investment stemmed from this intimate and sustained parental care (Chapais, Reference Chapais2008; Lieberman, Tooby, & Cosmides, Reference Lieberman, Tooby and Cosmides2007). The psychology that governs intragroup relations – which at its most basic form suggests that kin should consider first the interests of fellow kin – was borne from the relatively basic processes related to kin-formation, including motherhood, fatherhood, siblingship, incest avoidance, and in-law recognition (e.g., Hill & Hurtado, Reference Hill and Hurtado2009; Korchmaros & Kenny, Reference Korchmaros and Kenny2001; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2009).

Early hominids extended these pair-bond-like relationships beyond breeding couples to develop relationships in wider social networks (Aureli et al., Reference Aureli, Schaffner, Boesch, Bearder, Call, Chapman and van Schaik2008). Thus, the intergroup norms that Glowacki describes as critical are important, but they are of secondary importance relative to the underlying psychological process bearing on intragroup processes.

A critical implication of an intergroup psychology derived from intragroup processes is that the “default” intergroup dynamic is uncertainty rather than the aggression/conflict. Laboratory research into intergroup relations reveals that conflict is strongly rooted in uncertainty or fear of the out-group (Wildschut, Pinter, Vevea, Insko, & Schopler, Reference Wildschut, Pinter, Vevea, Insko and Schopler2003). In the context of mixed motive game (i.e., the prisoner's dilemma; Luce & Raiffa, Reference Luce and Raiffa1957), interactions that include a safe, “withdrawal” option – in addition to the usual cooperative and competitive choices – groups prefer to withdraw to competing (Schopler et al., Reference Schopler, Insko, Graetz, Drigotas, Smith and Dahl1993, Reference Schopler, Insko, Drigotas, Wieselquist, Pemberton and Cox1995). Indeed, when there is uncertainty about what the out-group will do, in-groups cooperate less with them (Insko, Kirchner, Pinter, Efaw, & Wildschut, Reference Insko, Kirchner, Pinter, Efaw and Wildschut2005).

Groups avoid conflict – and even seek peace – when given the opportunity. Laboratory variations using “minimal” groups (Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, Reference Tajfel, Billig, Bundy and Flament1971) show that when group members allocate resources to the in-group and out-group, they divide money equally between them (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Crum, Wittenbraker, Harring, Insko and Thibaut1983; Gaertner & Insko, Reference Gaertner and Insko2001). And when in-group and out-group evaluations are made separately, participants prefer bolstering the in-group over harming the out-group (e.g., Brewer, Reference Brewer1999; Brewer & Campbell, Reference Brewer and Campbell1976). When participants are tasked with allocating painful noise, they favor equal distributions to ones that predominantly hurt the out-group (e.g., Mummendey et al., Reference Mummendey, Simon, Dietze, Grünert, Haeger, Kessler and Schäferhoff1992).

Such intergroup interactions reflect motivations grounded in an intragroup psychology. Many theorists, including those focusing on social identity (Tajfel, Reference Tajfel1970; Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986) and on in-group favoring norms (Montoya & Pinter, Reference Montoya and Pinter2016; Montoya & Pittinsky, Reference Montoya and Pittinsky2013) emphasize that intergroup motivations are fueled by normative pressure to favor the in-group. For instance, group members behave cooperatively or competitively with an out-group according to the group norm that was emphasized to them (Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, Reference Jetten, Spears and Manstead1996; Montoya & Pittinsky, Reference Montoya and Pittinsky2013). As noted by Glowacki, there are a number of pressures that can push intergroup relations to be hostile. For instance, conflict is more likely when in-group members overtly support hostilities (Wildschut, Insko, & Gaertner, Reference Wildschut, Insko and Gaertner2002) and when individual group member's behaviors are concealed from the out-group (Schopler et al., Reference Schopler, Insko, Drigotas, Wieselquist, Pemberton and Cox1995), to name but two.

Uncertainty permits a range of potential intergroup interactions, including ones that are positive. But as we have noted, the potential for intergroup peace is dependent on the concerns of the in-group.

The possibility of peace – and the advantages of positive intergroup relations – likely began early in human evolutionary history. The growing complexity of human's social relationships is reflected in a dramatic increase in encephalization that began approximately 4–6 million years ago (Grabowski, Reference Grabowski2016; Van Schaik, Triki, Bshary, & Heldstab, Reference Van Schaik, Triki, Bshary and Heldstab2021). The primary factor for the growth in brain size in hominids (controlling for body mass) has been attributed to the problems associated with the complexity of social relationships (including alloparenting, coalition formation, tactical deception; Dunbar & Shultz, Reference Dunbar and Shultz2007; Isler & Van Schaik, Reference Isler and Van Schaik2006). Indeed, cooperative intergroup relations were essential for survival, and their benefits that started earlier than 300,000 years ago. The degree of outbreeding identified by studies of genetic diversity (e.g., Workman & Niswander, Reference Workman and Niswander1970) cannot be explained solely by competitive strategies (e.g., taking slaves after victory). Cooperative intergroup relations (which include group merging or emigration) were necessary not only to avoid inbreeding, but also to create groups that served needs relative to the food supply and mating (Knauft et al., Reference Knauft, Abler, Betzig, Boehm, Dentan, Kiefer and Rodseth1991; Loehle, Reference Loehle1995).

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

None.

References

Alexander, R. D., & Noonan, K. M. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, paternal care, and human social evolution. In Chagnon, N. A. & Irons, W. (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 436453). Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., Boesch, C., Bearder, S. K., Call, J., Chapman, C. A., … van Schaik, C. P. (2008). Fission–fusion dynamics: New research frameworks. Current Anthropology, 49(4), 627641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belisle, P., & Chapais, B. (2001). Tolerated co-feeding in relation to degree of kinship in Japanese macaques. Behaviour, 138, 487509. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853901750382124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigelow, R. S. (1969). The Dawn warriors: Man's evolution toward peace. Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Bornstein, G., Crum, L., Wittenbraker, J., Harring, K., Insko, C. A., & Thibaut, J. (1983). On the measurement of social orientations in the minimal group paradigm. European Journal of Social Psychology, 13(4), 321350. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420130402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429444. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Campbell, D. T. (1976). Ethnocentrism and intergroup attitudes: East African evidence. Sage.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2008). Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Appleton.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science (New York, N.Y.), 317(5843), 13441347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaertner, L., & Insko, C. A. (2001). On the measurement of social orientations in the minimal group paradigm: Norms as moderators of the expression of intergroup bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(2), 143154. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grabowski, M. (2016). Bigger brains led to bigger bodies?: The correlated evolution of human brain and body size. Current Anthropology, 57(2), 174196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grueter, C. C., Chapais, B., & Zinner, D. (2012). Evolution of multilevel social systems in nonhuman primates and humans. International Journal of Primatology, 33(5), 10021037. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9618-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, W. D. (1975). Innate social aptitudes of man: An approach from evolutionary genetics. In Fox, R. (Ed.), Biosocial anthropology (pp. 133153). Malaby Press.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in south American hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 38633870. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Bareson, P. P. O. & Hinde, R. A. (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303317). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Insko, C. A., Kirchner, J. L., Pinter, B., Efaw, J., & Wildschut, T. (2005). Interindividual-intergroup discontinuity as a function of trust and categorization: The paradox of expected cooperation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 365385. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isler, K., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2006). Metabolic costs of brain size evolution. Biology Letters, 2(4), 557560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1996). Intergroup norms and intergroup discrimination: Distinctive self-categorization and social identity effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 12221233. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knauft, B. M., Abler, T. S., Betzig, L., Boehm, C., Dentan, R. K., Kiefer, T. M., … Rodseth, L. (1991). Violence and sociality in human evolution. Current Anthropology, 32(4), 391428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2001). Emotional closeness as a mediator of the effect of genetic relatedness on altruism. Psychological Science, 12(3), 262265. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. (2009). Rethinking the Taiwanese minor marriage data: Evidence the mind uses multiple kinship cues to regulate inbreeding avoidance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(3), 153160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2007). The architecture of human kin detection. Nature, 445(7129), 727731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loehle, C. (1995). Social barriers to pathogen transmission in wild animal populations. Ecology, 76(2), 326335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D., & Raiffa, H. (1957). Games and decisions: Introduction and critical survey. Wiley.Google Scholar
Montoya, R. M., & Pinter, B. (2016). A model for understanding positive intergroup relations using the in-group-favoring norm. Journal of Social Issues, 72(3), 584600. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montoya, R. M., & Pittinsky, T. (2013). Individual variability in adherence to the norm of group interest predicts outgroup bias. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16, 173191. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430212450523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mummendey, A., Simon, B., Dietze, C., Grünert, M., Haeger, G., Kessler, S., … Schäferhoff, S. (1992). Categorization is not enough: Intergroup discrimination in negative outcome allocation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28(2), 125144. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(92)90035-ICrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schopler, J., Insko, C. A., Drigotas, S. M., Wieselquist, J., Pemberton, M. B., & Cox, C. (1995). The role of identifiability in the reduction of interindividual-intergroup discontinuity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31(6), 553574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schopler, J., Insko, C. A., Graetz, K., Drigotas, S. M., Smith, V., & Dahl, K. (1993). Individual-group discontinuity: Further evidence for mediation by fear and greed. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 419431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167293194007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223(5), 96103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149178. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420010202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). NelsonHall.Google Scholar
Van Schaik, C. P., Triki, Z., Bshary, R., & Heldstab, S. A. (2021). A farewell to the encephalization quotient: A new brain size measure for comparative primate cognition. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 96, 112. doi:10.1159/000517013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wildschut, T., Insko, C. A., & Gaertner, L. (2002). Intragroup social influence and intergroup competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 975992. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.975CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wildschut, T., Pinter, B., Vevea, J. L., Insko, C. A., & Schopler, J. (2003). Beyond the group mind: A quantitative review of the interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 698722. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.698CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1973). Group selection and its significance for ecology. Bioscience, 23(11), 631638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WoldeGabriel, G., Ambrose, S. H., Barboni, D., Bonnefille, R., Bremond, L., Currie, B., … White, T. D. (2009). The geological, isotopic, botanical, invertebrate, and lower vertebrate surroundings of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science (New York, N.Y.), 326(5949), 65e1-5. doi:10.1126/science.1175817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Workman, P. L., & Niswander, J. D. (1970). Population studies on southwestern Indian tribes. II. Local genetic differentiation in the Papago. American Journal of Human Genetics, 22(1), 2449.Google ScholarPubMed