No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Developmental research assessing bias would benefit from naturalistic observation data
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 13 May 2022
Abstract
Cesario's critiques and suggestions for redesigning social psychology experiments echo Dahl's (2017) call for developmental researchers to use experimental and naturalistic methods in a complementary manner for understanding children's development. We provide examples of how naturalistic observations can rectify Cesario's missing flaws for developmental studies investigating children's social biases and help researchers derive theories they can then experimentally test.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Pelletier, J., & Ferrell, J. M. (2009). Children's group nous: Understanding and applying peer exclusion within and between groups. Child Development, 80(1), 224–243. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01256.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braun, S. S., & Davidson, A. J. (2017). Gender (non) conformity in middle childhood: A mixed methods approach to understanding gender-typed behavior, friendship, and peer preference. Sex Roles, 77(1), 16–29. https://doi:10.1007/s11199-016-0693-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahl, A. (2017). Ecological commitments: Why developmental science needs naturalistic methods. Child Development Perspectives, 11(2), 79–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, Y., & Degner, J. (2013). From categories to exemplars (and back again). In Banaji, M. R. & Gelman, S.A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 275–280). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzroy, S., & Rutland, A. (2010). How social experience is related to children's intergroup attitudes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 679–693. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, P. A., & Walsh, P. V. (1991). Modification of children's gender-stereotyped behavior. Child Development, 62(2), 338–351. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01535.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, C. L., Kornienko, O., Schaefer, D. R., Hanish, L. D., Fabes, R. A., & Goble, P. (2013). The role of sex of peers and gender-typed activities in young children's peer affiliative networks: A longitudinal analysis of selection and influence. Child Development, 84(3), 921–937. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGlothlin, H., & Killen, M. (2010). How social experience is related to children's intergroup attitudes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 625–634. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rennels, J. L., & Langlois, J. H. (2014). Children's attractiveness, gender, and race biases: A comparison of their strength and generality. Child Development, 85(4), 1401–1418. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Milne, A., & McGeorge, P. (2005). Social norms and self-presentation: Children's implicit and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development, 76(2), 451–466. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00856.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
What can experimental studies of bias tell us about real-world group disparities?
Related commentaries (29)
A skeptical reflection: Contextualizing police shooting decisions with skin-tone
Accuracy in social judgment does not exclude the potential for bias
Beyond stereotypes: Prejudice as an important missing force explaining group disparities
Centering the relationship between structural racism and individual bias
Cesario's framework for understanding group disparities is radically incomplete
Controlled lab experiments are one of many useful scientific methods to investigate bias
Culturally fluent real-world disparities can blind us to bias: Experiments using a cultural lens can help
Developmental research assessing bias would benefit from naturalistic observation data
Experimental studies of bias: Imperfect but neither useless nor unique
Experiments make a good breakfast, but a poor supper
External validity of social psychological experiments is a concern, but these models are useful
Fighting over who dictates the nature of prejudice
How should we understand “bias” as a thick concept in recruitment discrimination studies?
Missing context from experimental studies amplifies, rather than negates, racial bias in the real world
Missing perspective: Marginalized groups in the social psychological study of social disparities
Practical consequences of flawed social psychological research on bias
Social bias insights concern judgments rather than real-world decisions
Surely not all experimental studies of bias need abandoning?
Taking social psychology out of context
The call for ecological validity is right but missing perceptual idiosyncrasies is wrong
The importance of ecological validity, ultimate causation, and natural categories
The internal validity obsession
The logic of challenging research into bias and social disparity
The missing consequences: A fourth flaw of experiments
The only thing that can stop bad causal inference is good causal inference
The unbearable limitations of solo science: Team science as a path for more rigorous and relevant research
Two thousand years after Archimedes, psychologist finds three topics that will simply not yield to the experimental method
Understanding causal mechanisms in the study of group bias
What can the implicit social cognition literature teach us about implicit social cognition?
Author response
Reply to the commentaries: A radical revision of experimental social psychology is still needed