No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Better late than Now-or-Never: The case of interactive repair phenomena
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 02 June 2016
Abstract
Empirical evidence from dialogue, both corpus and experimental, highlights the importance of interaction in language use – and this raises some questions for Christiansen & Chater's (C&C's) proposals. We endorse C&C's call for an integrated framework but argue that their emphasis on local, individual production and comprehension makes it difficult to accommodate the ubiquitous, interactive, and defeasible processes of clarification and repair in conversation.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016
References
Bavelas, J. B. & Gerwing, J. (2007) Conversational hand gestures and facial displays in face-to-face dialogue. In: Social communication, ed. Fiedler, K., pp. 283–308. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Brennan, S. E. & Schober, M. F. (2001) How listeners compensate for disfluencies in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language
44(2):274–96.Google Scholar
Colman, M. & Healey, P. G. T. (2011) The distribution of repair in dialogue. In: Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, MA, July 2011, ed. Carlson, L. & Shipley, T. F., pp. 1563–68.Google Scholar
Eshghi, A., Hough, J. & Purver, M. (2013) Incremental grammar induction from child-directed dialogue utterances. In: Proceedings of the 4th Annual ACL Workshop on Cognitive Modeling and Computational Linguistics (CMCL), Sofia, Bulgaria, August 8 2013, pp. 94–103, ed. Demberg, V. & Levy, R.. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Eshghi, A., Howes, C., Gregoromichelaki, E., Hough, J. & Purver, M. (2015) Feedback in conversation as incremental semantic update. In: Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Computational Semantics (IWCS), London, UK, April 15–17 2015, pp. 261–71, ed. Purver, M., Sadrzadeh, M. & Stone, M.. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (1979) The interactive construction of a sentence in natural conversation. In: Everyday language: Studies in ethnomethodology, ed. Psathas, G., pp. 97–121. Irvington Publishers.Google Scholar
Healey, P. G. T., Eshghi, A., Howes, C. & Purver, M. (2011) Making a contribution: Processing clarification requests in dialogue. In: Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting of the Society for Text and Discourse. Poitiers, France, July 11–13 2011,Google Scholar
Healey, P. G. T., Purver, M. & Howes, C. (2014) Divergence in dialogue. PLOS ONE
9(6):e98598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hough, J. & Purver, M. (2012) Processing self-repairs in an incremental type-theoretic dialogue system. In: Proceedings of the 16th SemDial Workshop on the Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue (SeineDial), Paris, France, September 19–21, 2012, pp. 136–44, ed. Brown-Schmidt, S., Ginzburg, J. & Larsson, S.. SemDialGoogle Scholar
Hough, J. & Purver, M. (2013) Modelling expectation in the self-repair processing of annotat-, um, listeners. In: Proceedings of the 17th SemDial Workshop on the Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue (DialDam), Amsterdam, Netherlands, December 16–18, 2013, pp. 92–101, ed. Fernández, R. & Isard, A.. SemDial.Google Scholar
Howes, C., Purver, M., McCabe, R., Healey, P. G. & Lavelle, M. (2012) Helping the medicine go down: Repair and adherence in patient-clinician dialogues. In: Proceedings of the 16th SemDial Workshop on the Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue (SeineDial), Paris, France, September 19–21 2012, pp. 155–56, ed. Brown-Schmidt, S., Ginzburg, J. & Larsson, S.. SemDial.Google Scholar
Lake, J. K., Humphreys, K. R. & Cardy, S. (2011) Listener vs. speaker-oriented aspects of speech: Studying the disfluencies of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
18(1):135–40.Google Scholar
Purver, M., Cann, R. & Kempson, R. (2006) Grammars as parsers: Meeting the dialogue challenge. Research in Language and Computation
4(2–3):289–326.Google Scholar
Purver, M., Eshghi, A. & Hough, J. (2011) Incremental semantic construction in a dialogue system. Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Computational Semantics (IWCS), Oxford, UK, January 12–14, 2011, pp. 365–69, ed. Box, J. & Pulman, S.. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G. & Sacks, H. (1977) The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language
53(2):361–82.Google Scholar
Target article
The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language
Related commentaries (28)
Better late than Now-or-Never: The case of interactive repair phenomena
Conceptual short-term memory (CSTM) supports core claims of Christiansen and Chater
Consequences of the Now-or-Never bottleneck for signed versus spoken languages
Exploring some edges: Chunk-and-Pass processing at the very beginning, across representations, and on to action
Gestalt-like representations hijack Chunk-and-Pass processing
How long is now? The multiple timescales of language processing
Is Now-or-Never language processing good enough?
Language acquisition is model-based rather than model-free
Language processing is not a race against time
Linguistic representations and memory architectures: The devil is in the details
Linguistic structure emerges through the interaction of memory constraints and communicative pressures
Linguistics, cognitive psychology, and the Now-or-Never bottleneck
Many important language universals are not reducible to processing or cognition
Mechanisms for interaction: Syntax as procedures for online interactive meaning building
Memory limitations and chunking are variable and cannot explain language structure
Natural language processing and the Now-or-Never bottleneck
Neural constraints and flexibility in language processing
Now or … later: Perceptual data are not immediately forgotten during language processing
On the generalizability of the Chunk-and-Pass processing approach: Perspectives from language acquisition and music
Pro and con: Internal speech and the evolution of complex language
Processing cost and its consequences
Realizing the Now-or-Never bottleneck and Chunk-and-Pass processing with Item-Order-Rank working memories and masking field chunking networks
Reservoir computing and the Sooner-is-Better bottleneck
Socio-demographic influences on language structure and change: Not all learners are the same
The bottleneck may be the solution, not the problem
The ideomotor recycling theory for language
What gets passed in “Chunk-and-Pass” processing? A predictive processing solution to the Now-or-Never bottleneck
“Process and perish” or multiple buffers with push-down stacks?
Author response
Squeezing through the Now-or-Never bottleneck: Reconnecting language processing, acquisition, change, and structure