Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T14:53:23.468Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grounded procedures in mind and society

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2021

Spike W. S. Lee
Affiliation:
Rotman School of Management and Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, ON M5S 3E6, Canadaspike.lee@utoronto.ca; https://mindandbodylab.wixsite.com/mindandbodylab
Norbert Schwarz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Mind & Society Center, and Marshall School of Business, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA90089. norbert.schwarz@usc.edu; https://dornsife.usc.edu/norbert-schwarz

Abstract

Our commentators explore the operation of grounded procedures across all levels of analysis in the behavioral sciences, from mental to social, developmental, and evolutionary/functional. Building on them, we offer two integrative principles for systematic effects of grounded procedures to occur. We discuss theoretical topics at each level of analysis, address methodological recommendations, and highlight further extensions of grounded procedures.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altmann, G. T. M., & Ekves, Z. (2019). Events as intersecting object histories: A new theory of event representation. Psychological Review, 126(6), 817840. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atherton, O. E., Grijalva, E., Roberts, B. W., & Robins, R. W. (2020). Stability and change in personality traits and major life goals from college to midlife. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 014616722094936. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220949362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1975). The structure of magic: A book about language and therapy. Science and Behavior Books. https://books.google.ca/books?id=hvvaAAAAMAAJ.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Basic processes; applications (Vols. 1–2, 2nd ed., pp. 140). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Bassok, M., Pedigo, S. F., & Oskarsson, A. T. (2008). Priming addition facts with semantic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 343352. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.343.Google ScholarPubMed
Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Mental construal and the emergence of assimilation and contrast effects: The inclusion/exclusion model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 319373.Google Scholar
Boyce, J. M., & Pittet, D. (2002). Guideline for hand hygiene in health-care settings: Recommendations of the Healthcare Infection Control Practices Advisory Committee and the HICPAC/SHEA/APIC/IDSA Hand Hygiene Task Force. Infection Control & Hospital Epidemiology, 23(Suppl. 12), S3S40. https://doi.org/10.1086/503164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (Eds.) (2012). Social metacognition. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Stavraki, M., Lamprinakos, G., Wagner, B. C., & Díaz, D. (2018). Affective and cognitive validation of thoughts: An appraisal perspective on anger, disgust, surprise, and awe. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 693718. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Burke, B. L., Martens, A., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(2), 155195. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309352321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56(1), 453484. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Things rank and gross in nature: A review and synthesis of moral disgust. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 300327. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chatard, A., Hirschberger, G., & Pyszczynski, T. (2020). A Word of Caution about Many Labs 4: If You Fail to Follow Your Preregistered Plan, You May Fail to Find a Real Effect [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ejubn.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtis, V., & Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease. Is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44(1), 1731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danovitch, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Children's extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 9(1), 107112. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Los Reyes, A., Aldao, A., Kundey, S. M. A., Lee, B. G., & Molina, S. (2012). Compromised decision making and the effects of manipulating physical states on human judgments. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Olimpio, F., & Mancini, F. (2014). Role of deontological guilt in obsessive-compulsive disorder-like checking and washing behaviors. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(6), 727739. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614529549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and danger: An analysis of the concept of pollution and taboo. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. M., & Radomsky, A. S. (2009). Analyses of mental contamination: Part I, experimental manipulations of morality. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(12), 9951003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.03.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, C. M., & Radomsky, A. S. (2012). Mental contamination: The effects of imagined physical dirt and immoral behaviour. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(6), 422427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.03.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairbrother, N., Newth, S. J., & Rachman, S. (2005). Mental pollution: Feelings of dirtiness without physical contact. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(1), 121130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2003.12.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golec de Zavala, A., Waldzus, S., & Cypryanska, M. (2014). Prejudice towards gay men and a need for physical cleansing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.04.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366385. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61139). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60016-7.Google Scholar
Herba, J. K., & Rachman, S. (2007). Vulnerability to mental contamination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11), 28042812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133168). Guilford.Google Scholar
IncenseChinese Customs. (n.d.). Retrieved September 9, 2020, from https://www.nationsonline.org/oneworld/Chinese_Customs/Incense.htm.Google Scholar
Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Nelson, T. O. (1998). Social metacognition: An expansionist review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(2), 137154. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kampf, G., & Kramer, A. (2004). Epidemiologic background of hand hygiene and evaluation of the most important agents for scrubs and rubs. Clinical Microbiology Reviews, 17(4), 863893. https://doi.org/10.1128/CMR.17.4.863-893.2004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. S., & Wohl, M. J. A. (2015). The bright side of self-discontinuity: Feeling disconnected with the past self increases readiness to change addictive behaviors (via nostalgia). Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(2), 229237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614549482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H. S., Wohl, M. J. A., Salmon, M., & Santesso, D. (2017). When do gamblers help themselves? Self-discontinuity increases self-directed change over time. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 148153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.08.037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, T. W., Duhachek, A., Briñol, P., Lee, S. W. S., & Petty, R. E. (2020). The meaning of physical actions moderates the impact of cleansing on guilt [Manuscript in preparation].Google Scholar
Klein, R. A., Cook, C. L., Ebersole, C. R., Vitiello, C. A., Nosek, B. A., Chartier, C. R., … … Ratliff, K. A. (2019). Many Labs 4: Failure to Replicate Mortality Salience Effect with and without Original Author Involvement [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vef2c.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koole, S. L., Tjew, A, Sin, M., & Schneider, I. K. (2014). Embodied terror management: Interpersonal touch alleviates existential concerns among individuals with low self-esteem. Psychological Science, 25(1), 3037. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613483478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Körner, A., & Strack, F. (2019). Conditions for the clean slate effect after success or failure. The Journal of Social Psychology, 159(1), 92105. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2018.1454881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being (Vol. 6, 1st ed.). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Landau, M. J. (2017). Conceptual metaphor in social psychology: The poetics of everyday life (1st ed.). Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., & Cecutti, L. (in press). Three mechanisms of mind-body influence: Feelings, concepts, and procedures. In L. R. Kahle, T. M. Lowrey, & J. Huber (Eds.), APA handbook of consumer psychology. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., Chen, K., Ma, C., & Hoang, J. (2020a). Psychological antecedents and consequences of physical cleansing: A meta-analytic review [manuscript in preparation].Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., Millet, K., Grinstein, A., Pauwels, K., Johnston, P. R., Volkov, A. E., & van der Wal, A. (2020b). Threat and cleaning [Manuscript in preparation].Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010a). Washing away postdecisional dissonance. Science (New York, N.Y.), 328, 709. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1186799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2011). Wiping the slate clean: Psychological consequences of physical cleansing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 307311. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411422694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2016). Clean-moral effects and clean-slate effects: Physical cleansing as an embodied procedure of psychological separation. In Duschinsky, R., Schnall, S., & Weiss, D. H. (Eds.), Purity and danger now: New perspectives (pp. 136161). Routledge.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., Tang, H., Wan, J., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2015). A cultural look at moral purity: Wiping the face clean. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 577. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr (1997). Conceptions and correlates of openness to experience. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 825847). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012134645-4/50032-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McShane, B. B., Böckenholt, U., & Hansen, K. T. (2016). Adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis: An evaluation of selection methods and some cautionary notes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(5), 730749. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616662243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mead, N., & Levav, J. (2016). A change is as good as a rest: Changing contexts restores self-control. ACR North American Advances, NA-44, 139144.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., Barrett, L. F., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.). (2010). The mind in context. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Newman, D. B., Sachs, M. E., Stone, A. A., & Schwarz, N. (2020). Nostalgia and well-being in daily life: An ecological validity perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(2), 325347. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 311342. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D., Sorensen, N., Reber, R., & Chen, S. X. (2009). Connecting and separating mind-sets: Culture as situated cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 217235. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paredes, B., Guyer, J. J., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2019). Subtle priming of subtraction versus addition: A spill-over effect of math/La inducción sutil de sumar vs. restar: un efecto indirecto de las matemáticas. Revista de Psicología Social, 34(3), 590622. https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2019.1649890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinto, A., Steinglass, J. E., Greene, A. L., Weber, E. U., & Simpson, H. B. (2014). Capacity to delay reward differentiates obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 75(8), 653659. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pittet, D., Allegranzi, B., & Boyce, J., & World Health Organization World Alliance for Patient Safety First Global Patient Safety Challenge Core Group of Experts. (2009). The World Health Organization guidelines on hand hygiene in health care and their consensus recommendations. Infection Control & Hospital Epidemiology, 30(7), 611622. https://doi.org/10.1086/600379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radvansky, G. A., & Copeland, D. E. (2006). Walking through doorways causes forgetting: Situation models and experienced space. Memory & Cognition, 34(5), 11501156. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuven, O., Liberman, N., & Dar, R. (2014). The effect of physical cleaning on threatened morality in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 224229. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613485565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology (pp. xvi, 286). McGraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 2341. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaller, M. (2015). The behavioral immune system. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 119). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych107.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(2), 99103. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411402596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildberg-Hörisch, H. (2018). Are risk preferences stable? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(2), 135154. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.32.2.135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in context. Social Cognition, 25(5), 638656. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. (2010). Meaning in context: Metacognitive experiences. In Mesquita, B., Barrett, L. F., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.), The mind in context (pp. 105125). Guilford.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (2012). Feelings-as-information theory. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. W., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 289308). Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Kruglanski, A. W. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 385407). Guilford.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., Sanna, L. J., Skurnik, I., Yoon, C. (2007). Metacognitive experiences and the intricacies of setting people straight: Implications for debiasing and public information campaigns. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 39, pp. 127161). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)39003-X.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J. (2015). Nostalgia counteracts self-discontinuity and restores self-continuity: Self-discontinuity, nostalgia, continuity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(1), 5261. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2014b). p-curve: A key to the file-drawer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 534547. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonsohn, U., Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (2015). Better p-curves: Making p-curve analysis more robust to errors, fraud, and ambitious p-hacking, a reply to Ulrich and Miller (2015). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(6), 11461152. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, N., & Heyd-Metzuyanim, E. (2014). Learning number with TouchCounts: The role of emotions and the body in mathematical communication. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 19(1–2), 8199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-014-9212-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2004). Socially situated cognition: Cognition in its social context. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 53117). Elsevier. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0065260104360028.Google Scholar
Stanley, T. D., & Doucouliagos, C. H. (2014). Meta-regression approximations to reduce publication selection bias. Research Synthesis Methods, 5(1), 6078. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talhelm, T., & English, A. S. (2020). Historically rice-farming societies have tighter social norms in China and worldwide. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(33), 1981619824. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1909909117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., & Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science (New York, N.Y.), 344(6184), 603608. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1246850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tasimi, A., & Gross, J. J. (2020). The dilemma of dirty money. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 4146. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E. (1998). The social being in social psychology. In Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1–2, pp. 5895). McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Tesser, A. (2000). On the confluence of self-esteem maintenance mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(4), 290299. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0404_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (2010). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20(4), 410433. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x (original work published in 1963).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullrich, S., Farrington, D. P., & Coid, J. W. (2007). Dimensions of DSM-IV personality disorders and life-success. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21(6), 657663. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2007.21.6.657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uskul, A. K., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(25), 85528556. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0803874105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Aert, R. C. M., & van Assen, M. A. L. M. (2018). p-uniform* [Preprint]. MetaArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31222/osf.io/zqjr9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 12571267. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, H., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Washing the guilt away: Effects of personal versus vicarious cleansing on guilty feelings and prosocial behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 97. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Q., Wu, X., Zhou, X., Mead, N. L., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2013). Diverging effects of clean versus dirty money on attitudes, values, and interpersonal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(3), 473489. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, L., Zheng, Y., Compton, B. J., Qin, W., Zheng, J., Fu, G., … Heyman, G. D. (2020). The moral barrier effect: Real and imagined barriers can reduce cheating. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(32), 1910119107. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2002249117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed