Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:27:28.277Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“Negative emotions” live in stories, not in the hearts of readers who enjoy them

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2017

Vladimir J. Konečni*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109. vkonecni@ucsd.eduhttps://psychology.ucsd.edu/people/profiles/vkonecni.html

Abstract

The commendably ambitious project by Menninghaus et al. fails because its main connective tissue – “negative emotions” – is beyond the grasp of the authors' largely literary approach. The critique focuses on their treatment of the Paradox of Fiction, the neglect of the biological, adaptive nature of emotions, and the absence of convincing empirical support for key aspects of the proposed model.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arieti, J. A. & Crossett, J. M., trans. (1985) Longinus on the sublime. Mellen.Google Scholar
Burke, E. (1759/1971) A philosophical enquiry into the origin of our ideas of the sublime and beautiful, 2nd edition. Garland.Google Scholar
Gerger, G., Leder, H. & Kremer, A. (2014) Context effects on emotional and aesthetic evaluations of artworks and IAPS pictures. Acta Psychologica 151(1):174–83. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.008.Google Scholar
Konečni, V. J. (1991) Psychological aspects of the expression of anger and violence on the stage. Comparative Drama 25:215–41.Google Scholar
Konečni, V. J. (2008) Does music induce emotion? A theoretical and methodological analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 2:115–29. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konečni, V. J. (2013) Music, affect, method, data: Reflections on the Carroll versus Kivy debate. American Journal of Psychology 126:179–95. Available at: https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.2.0179.Google Scholar
Konečni, V. J. (2015) Emotion in painting and art installations. American Journal of Psychology 128:305–22. Available at: https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.3.0305.Google Scholar
Konečni, V. J., Brown, A. & Wanic, R. A. (2008) Comparative effects of music and recalled life-events on emotional state. Psychology of Music 36:289308. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607082621.Google Scholar
Lundqvist, L.-O., Carlsson, F., Hilmersson, P. & Juslin, P. N. (2009) Emotional responses to music: Experience, expression, and physiology. Psychology of Music 37:6190. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607086048.Google Scholar
Radford, C. & Weston, M. (1975) How can we be moved by the fate of Anna Karenina? Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 49(Suppl):6793.Google Scholar
Wagner, V., Klein, J., Hanich, J., Shah, M., Menninghaus, W. & Jacobsen, T. (2016) Anger framed: A field study on emotion, pleasure, and art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 10(2):134–46. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000029.Google Scholar