No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Constructing rationals through conjoint measurement of numerator and denominator as approximate integer magnitudes in tradeoff relations
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 15 December 2021
Abstract
To investigate mechanisms of rational representation, I consider (1) construction of an ordered continuum of psychophysical scale of magnitude of sensation; (2) counting mechanism leading to an approximate numerosity scale for integers; and (3) conjoint measurement structure pitting the denominator against the numerator in tradeoff positions. Number sense of resulting rationals is neither intuitive nor expedient in their manipulation.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Carey, S. (2001). Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: Evolution and ontogenisis. Mind & Language 16(1): 37–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (2009). Where our number concepts come from. The Journal of Philosophy 106(4): 220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S., & Barner, D. (2019). Ontogenetic origins of human integer representations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23(10): 823–835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M., & Narens, L. (1979). Fundamental unit structures: A theory of ratio scalability. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 20(3): 193–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Spelke, E., & Pica, P. (2008). Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science (New York, N.Y.) 320(5880): 1217–1220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (2002). Infants’ discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology 44(1): 33–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(7): 307–314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, L., Zhang, J., Zhou, T., & Chen, L. (2009). Connectedness affects dot numerosity judgment: Implications for configural processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 16(3): 509–517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krantz, D., Luce, D., Suppes, P., & Tversky, A. (1971). Foundations of measurement, Vol. I: Additive and polynomial representations.Google Scholar
Le Corre, M., & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition 105(2): 395–438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemer, C., Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., & Cohen, L. (2003). Approximate quantities and exact number words: Dissociable systems. Neuropsychologia 41(14): 1942–1958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D., & Narens, L. (1987). Measurement scales on the continuum. Science (New York, N.Y.) 236(4808): 1527–1532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narens, L. (1981). A general theory of ratio scalability with remarks about the measurement-theoretic concept of meaningfulness. Theory and Decision 13(1): 1–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narens, L., & Luce, R. D. (1986). Measurement: The theory of numerical assignments. Psychological Bulletin 99(2): 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., Cooperrider, K., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Number concepts without number lines in an indigenous group of Papua New Guinea. PLoS One 7(4): e35662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarnecka, B. W., & Carey, S. (2008). How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition 108(3): 662–674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1936). A scale for the measurement of a psychological magnitude: Loudness. Psychological Review 43(5): 405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1956). The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes: Loudness. The American journal of Psychology 69(1): 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review 64(3): 153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S., & Galanter, E. H. (1957). Ratio scales and category scales for a dozen perceptual continua. Journal of Experimental Psychology 54(6): 377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, F., Spelke, E. S., & Goddard, S. (2005). Number sense in human infants. Developmental Science 8(1): 88–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
The number sense represents (rational) numbers
Related commentaries (26)
A rational explanation for links between the ANS and math
Constructing rationals through conjoint measurement of numerator and denominator as approximate integer magnitudes in tradeoff relations
Contents of the approximate number system
Distinguishing the specific from the recognitional and the canonical, and the nature of ratios
Non-symbolic and symbolic number and the approximate number system
Not so rational: A more natural way to understand the ANS
Numbers in action
Numerical cognition needs more and better distinctions, not fewer
Numerical cognition: Unitary or diversified system(s)?
Numerosities are not ersatz numbers
Numerosity, area-osity, object-osity? Oh my
Perceived number is not abstract
Positing numerosities may be metaphysically extravagant; positing representation of numerosities is not
Ratio-based perceptual foundations for rational numbers, and perhaps whole numbers, too?
Real models: The limits of behavioural evidence for understanding the ANS
Representation of pure magnitudes in ANS
Second-order characteristics don't favor a number-representing ANS
Sizes, ratios, approximations: On what and how the ANS represents
The approximate number system represents magnitude and precision
The approximate number system represents rational numbers: The special case of an empty set
The number sense does not represent numbers, but cardinality comparisons
The number sense represents multitudes and magnitudes
The perception of quantity ain't number: Missing the primacy of symbolic reference
Unwarranted philosophical assumptions in research on ANS
Weighted numbers
What are we doing when we perceive numbers?
Author response
Numbers, numerosities, and new directions