Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T13:29:17.513Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Integrating cultural evolution and behavioral genetics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2022

Ryutaro Uchiyama
Affiliation:
NTU-Cambridge Centre for Lifelong Learning and Individualised Cognition, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore639798ryutaro.uchiyama@ntu.edu.sg; https://www.uchiyamaryutaro.com
Rachel Spicer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, WC2A 2AE, UKr.a.spicer@lse.ac.ukhttps://www.lse.ac.uk/PBS/People/Rachel-Spicer m.muthukrishna@lse.ac.ukhttps://michael.muthukrishna.com
Michael Muthukrishna
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, WC2A 2AE, UKr.a.spicer@lse.ac.ukhttps://www.lse.ac.uk/PBS/People/Rachel-Spicer m.muthukrishna@lse.ac.ukhttps://michael.muthukrishna.com

Abstract

The 29 commentaries amplified our key arguments; offered extensions, implications, and applications of the framework; and pushed back and clarified. To help forge the path forward for cultural evolutionary behavioral genetics, we (1) focus on conceptual disagreements and misconceptions about the concepts of heritability and culture; (2) further discuss points raised about the intertwined relationship between culture and genes; and (3) address extensions to the proposed framework, particularly as it relates to cultural clusters, development, and power. These commentaries, and the deep engagement they represent, reinforce the importance of integrating cultural evolution and behavioral genetics.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, M. J. (1994). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT press.Google Scholar
Alesina, A., Giuliano, P., & Nunn, N. (2013). On the origins of gender roles: Women and the plough. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 128(2), 469530. https://doi.org/10/gctrzs.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apicella, C. L., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2020). Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 319329. https://doi.org/10/ghndmz.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, S. J., Bürger, R., Hohenlohe, P. A., Ajie, B. C., & Jones, A. G. (2008). Understanding the evolution and stability of the G-Matrix. Evolution, 62(10), 24512461. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00472.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsbai, T., Lukas, D., & Pondorfer, A. (2021). Local convergence of behavior across species. Science (New York, N.Y.), 371(6526), 292295. https://doi.org/10/ghxt79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, T. C., Lewis, G. J., & Weiss, A. (2013). Childhood socioeconomic status amplifies genetic effects on adult intelligence. Psychological Science, 24(10), 21112116. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchamp, J. P. (2016). Genetic evidence for natural selection in humans in the contemporary United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(28), 77747779. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600398113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, A., Chetty, R., Jaravel, X., Petkova, N., & Van Reenen, J. (2019). Who becomes an inventor in America? The importance of exposure to innovation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 134(2), 647713. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjy028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, T. (2015). Cultural studies and the culture concept. Cultural Studies, 29(4), 546568. https://doi.org/10.1080/09502386.2014.1000605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C., Gamson, D., Thorne, S., & Baker, D. (2005). Rising mean IQ: Cognitive demand of mathematics education for young children, population exposure to formal schooling, and the neurobiology of the prefrontal cortex. Intelligence, 33(1), 93106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Border, R., Johnson, E. C., Evans, L. M., Smolen, A., Berley, N., Sullivan, P. F., & Keller, M. C. (2019). No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(5), 376387. https://doi.org/10/gfwnhp.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(Supplement 2), 1091810925. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100290108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101(4), 568586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6(1), 130. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0601_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2009). Reading comprehension development from 8 to 14 years: The contribution of component skills and processes. In R. K. Wagner, C. Schatschneider, & C. Phythian-Sence (Eds.), Beyond decoding: The behavioral and biological foundations of reading comprehension (pp. 143175). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1973). Cultural versus biological inheritance: Phenotypic transmission from parents to children. (A theory of the effect of parental phenotypes on children's phenotypes). American Journal of Human Genetics, 25(6), 618637.Google Scholar
Chetty, R., Friedman, J. N., Hilger, N., Saez, E., Schanzenbach, D. W., & Yagan, D. (2011). How does your kindergarten classroom affect your earnings? Evidence from Project STAR. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(4), 15931660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 498508. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X08004998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, R. M., & Zubek, J. P. (1958). Effects of enriched and restricted early environments on the learning ability of bright and dull rats. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 12(3), 159164. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crow, J. F. (2010). On epistasis: Why it is unimportant in polygenic directional selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1544), 12411244. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culverhouse, R. C., Saccone, N. L., Horton, A. C., Ma, Y., Anstey, K. J., Banaschewski, T., … Bierut, L. J. (2018). Collaborative meta-analysis finds no evidence of a strong interaction between stress and 5-HTTLPR genotype contributing to the development of depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(1), 133142. https://doi.org/10/f9zprm.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56(2), 384398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review, 108(2), 346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelhardt, L. E., Briley, D. A., Mann, F. D., Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2015). Genes unite executive functions in childhood. Psychological Science, 26(8), 11511163. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615577209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, A., & Hermle, J. (2018). Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality. Science (New York, N.Y.), 362(6412), eaas9899. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aas9899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, S., Hansen, M. E. B., Lo, Y., & Tishkoff, S. A. (2016). Going global by adapting local: A review of recent human adaptation. Science (New York, N.Y.), 354(6308), 5459. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf5098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fosse, E., & Winship, C. (2019). Analyzing age-period-cohort data: A review and critique. Annual Review of Sociology, 45(1), 467492. https://doi.org/10/gf6rv7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, E. I. (2021). Studying mental health disorders as systems, not syndromes. PsyArXiv. doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/k4mhvGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galanter, J. M., Gignoux, C. R., Oh, S. S., Torgerson, D., Pino-Yanes, M., Thakur, N., … Zaitlen, N. (2017). Differential methylation between ethnic sub-groups reflects the effect of genetic ancestry and environmental exposures. ELife, 6, e20532. https://doi.org/10/gmh22v.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., von Rueden, C., Massenkoff, M., Kaplan, H., & Lero Vie, M. (2013). How universal is the Big Five? Testing the five-factor model of personality variation among forager–farmers in the Bolivian Amazon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 354370. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) and children's intelligence (IQ): In a UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e30320. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasson, U., Chen, J., & Honey, C. J. (2015). Hierarchical process memory: Memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(6), 304313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J. (2017). DNA is not destiny: The remarkable, completely misunderstood relationship between you and your genes. Norton.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2016). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2020). The WEIRDest people in the world: How the West became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2008). Five misunderstandings about cultural evolution. Human Nature, 19(2), 119137. https://doi.org/10/ffwks9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Chudek, M., & Boyd, R. (2015). The Big Man Mechanism: How prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1683), 20150013. https://doi.org/10/gfzktb.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 207240. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-081920-042106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, W. G., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2008). Data and theory point to mainly additive genetic variance for complex traits. PLoS Genetics, 4(2), e1000008. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1000008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hivert, V., Sidorenko, J., Rohart, F., Goddard, M. E., Yang, J., Wray, N. R., … Visscher, P. M. (2021). Estimation of non-additive genetic variance in human complex traits from a large sample of unrelated individuals. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 108(5), 786798. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2021.02.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Sage publications.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. (2016). Cultural epigenetics. The Sociological Review Monographs, 64(1), 4260. https://doi.org/10/ggxwsq.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonski, N. G., & Chaplin, G. (2017). The colours of humanity: The evolution of pigmentation in the human lineage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1724), 20160349. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M., De, S., & Fox, A. (2019). The loosening of American culture over 200 years is associated with a creativity–order trade-off. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(3), 244250. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0516-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M., & Ember, C. R. (2020). A global analysis of cultural tightness in non-industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1930), 20201036. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkin, F. (1867). The origin of species. North British Review, 46, 277318.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., King, A., Yoon, C., Tompson, S., Huff, S., & Liberzon, I. (2014). The dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) moderates cultural difference in independent versus interdependent social orientation. Psychological Science, 25(6), 11691177. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614528338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwiatkowski, D. P. (2005). How malaria has affected the human genome and what human genetics can teach us about malaria. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 77(2), 171192. https://doi.org/10.1086/432519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larøi, F., Luhrmann, T. M., Bell, V., Christian, W. A., Deshpande, S., Fernyhough, C., … Woods, A. (2014). Culture and hallucinations: Overview and future directions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(Suppl_4), S213–S220. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J. L. C., Nader, K., & Schiller, D. (2017). An update on memory reconsolidation updating. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(7), 531545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Treadway, M. T. (2016). Clashing diagnostic approaches: DSM-ICD versus RDoC. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 435463. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lippa, R. A. (2010). Gender differences in personality and interests: When, where, and why?: Gender differences in personality and interests. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(11), 10981110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00320.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipschuetz, M., Cohen, S. M., Ein-Mor, E., Sapir, H., Hochner-Celnikier, D., Porat, S., … Yagel, S. (2015). A large head circumference is more strongly associated with unplanned cesarean or instrumental delivery and neonatal complications than high birthweight. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 213(6), 833.e1-833.e12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.07.045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, M., Raninen, J., Slade, T., Swift, W., Lloyd, B., & Dietze, P. (2016). Understanding trends in Australian alcohol consumption – An age–period–cohort model. Addiction, 111(9), 15901598. https://doi.org/10/bmwr.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luhrmann, T. M., Padmavati, R., Tharoor, H., & Osei, A. (2015). Differences in voice-hearing experiences of people with psychosis in the USA, India and Ghana: Interview-based study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 206(1), 4144. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.139048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukaszewski, A. W., Gurven, M., von Rueden, C. R., & Schmitt, D. P. (2017). What explains personality covariation? A test of the socioecological complexity hypothesis. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(8), 943952. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617697175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathieson, I. (2021). The omnigenic model and polygenic prediction of complex traits. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 108, 15581563. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2021.07.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, K. J. (2018). Innate: How the wiring of our brains shapes who we are. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 112117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2003). The relationship between the structure of interindividual and intraindividual variability: A theoretical and empirical vindication of developmental systems theory. In Staudinger, U. M. & Lindenberger, U. (Eds.), Understanding human development (pp. 339360). Springer US. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moya, C., & Boyd, R. (2015). Different selection pressures give rise to distinct ethnic phenomena: A functionalist framework with illustrations from the Peruvian Altiplano. Human Nature, 26(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9224-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M., Bell, A. V., Henrich, J., Curtin, C. M., Gedranovich, A., McInerney, J., & Thue, B. (2020). Beyond Western, educated, industrial, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) psychology: Measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychological Science, 31(6), 678701. https://doi.org/10/ggxkjb.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M., Doebeli, M., Chudek, M., & Henrich, J. (2018). The cultural brain hypothesis: How culture drives brain expansion, sociality, and life history. PLoS Computational Biology, 14(11), e1006504. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2016). Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150192. https://doi.org/10/gfzkmd.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M., Henrich, J., & Slingerland, E. (2021). Psychology as a historical science. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 717749. https://doi.org/10/ghrnb6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niedzwiecki, M. M., Walker, D. I., Vermeulen, R., Chadeau-Hyam, M., Jones, D. P., & Miller, G. W. (2019). The exposome: Molecules to populations. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 59(1), 107127. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-010818-021315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14001356.Google ScholarPubMed
Oakhill, J. V. & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139(1), 2338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prado, E. L., Sebayang, S. K., Apriatni, M., Adawiyah, S. R., Hidayati, N., Islamiyah, A., … Shankar, A. H. (2017). Maternal multiple micronutrient supplementation and other biomedical and socioenvironmental influences on children's cognition at age 9–12 years in Indonesia: Follow-up of the SUMMIT randomised trial. The Lancet Global Health, 5(2), e217e228. https://doi.org/10/gdxwk6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reis, A., & Castro-Caldas, A. (1997). Illiteracy: A cause for biased cognitive development. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(5), 444450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C., & Deary, I. J. (2015). Is education associated with improvements in general cognitive ability, or in specific skills? Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 573582. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29(8), 13581369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, D. C., Jacobson, K. C., & Van den Oord, E. J. C. G. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as moderator. Child Development, 70(5), 11511162. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, C. B., Trinkaus, E., & Holliday, T. W. (1997). Body mass and encephalization in Pleistocene Homo. Nature, 387(6629), 173176. https://doi.org/10.1038/387173a0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauce, B., Bendrath, S., Herzfeld, M., Siegel, D., Style, C., Rab, S., … Matzel, L. D. (2018). The impact of environmental interventions among mouse siblings on the heritability and malleability of general cognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1756), 20170289. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science (New York, N.Y.), 174(4016), 12851295. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.174.4016.1285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmelpfennig, R., Razek, L., Schnell, E., & Muthukrishna, M. (2022). Paradox of diversity in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200316. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, D. P., Long, A. E., McPhearson, A., O'Brien, K., Remmert, B., & Shah, S. H. (2017). Personality and gender differences in global perspective. International Journal of Psychology, 52, 4556. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, J. F., Bahrami-Rad, D., Beauchamp, J. P., & Henrich, J. (2019). The Church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science (New York, N.Y.), 366(6466), eaau5141. https://doi.org/10/ggckxh.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwadel, P. (2011). Age, period, and cohort effects on religious activities and beliefs. Social Science Research, 40(1), 181192. https://doi.org/10/brb64c.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smaldino, P. E., Lukaszewski, A., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2019). Niche diversity can explain cross-cultural differences in personality structure. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(12), 12761283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tikhodeyev, O. N., & Shcherbakova, О. V. (2019). The problem of non-shared environment in behavioral genetics. Behavior Genetics, 49(3), 259269. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-019-09950-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (p. 60). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51(4), 407415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, B., Ringen, E. J., Tsiazonera, , Tombo, J., Hajasoa, P., Gérard, S., … Garçon, A. H. (2021). Ethnic markers without ethnic conflict: Why do interdependent Masikoro, Mikea, and Vezo of Madagascar signal their ethnic differences? Human Nature, 32(3), 529556. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09412-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Bates, T. C. (2016). Large cross-national differences in gene × socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychological Science, 27(2), 138149. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615612727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Briley, D. A., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Genetic and environmental influences on cognition across development and context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 349355. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413485087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(5), 160164. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turley, P., Meyer, M. N., Wang, N., Cesarini, D., Hammonds, E., Martin, A. R., … Visscher, P. M. (2021). Problems with using polygenic scores to select embryos. New England Journal of Medicine, 385(1), 7886. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsr2105065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uchiyama, R., & Muthukrishna, M. (in press). Cultural evolutionary neuroscience. In Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience and global mental health. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3pj8a.Google Scholar
Vogt, G. (2015). Stochastic developmental variation, an epigenetic source of phenotypic diversity with far-reaching biological consequences. Journal of Biosciences, 40(1), 159204. https://doi.org/10/f64xns.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waring, T. M., & Wood, Z. T. (2021). Long-term gene–culture coevolution and the human evolutionary transition. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 288(1952), 20210538. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wild, C. P. (2012). The exposome: From concept to utility. International Journal of Epidemiology, 41(1), 2432. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyr236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yi, X., Liang, Y., Huerta-Sanchez, E., Jin, X., Cuo, Z. X. P., Pool, J. E., … Wang, J. (2010). Sequencing of 50 human exomes reveals adaptation to high altitude. Science (New York, N.Y.), 329(5987), 7578. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1190371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed