Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T17:39:02.572Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behavioural inhibition and valuation of gain/loss are neurally distinct from approach/withdrawal

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2019

Neil McNaughton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealandneil.mcnaughton@otago.ac.nzhttp://www.otago.ac.nz/psychology/staff/neilmcnaughton.html
Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, City, University of London, London EC1R 0JD, United Kingdom. Philip.Corr.1@city.ac.ukhttps://www.city.ac.uk/people/academics/philip-corr

Abstract

Gain or omission/termination of loss produces approach; while loss or omission/termination of gain produces withdrawal. Control of approach/withdrawal motivation is distinct from valuation of gain/loss and does not entail learning – making “reward” and “punishment” ambiguous. Approach-withdrawal goal conflict engages a neurally distinct Behavioural Inhibition System, which controls “anxiety” (conflict/passive avoidance) but not “fear” (withdrawal/active avoidance).

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelman, H. M. & Maatsch, J. L. (1956) Learning and extinction based upon frustration, food reward, and exploratory tendency. Journal of Experimental Psychology 52:311–15.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1948) Gradients of approach and avoidance responses and their relation to level of motivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 41:450–65.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L. & Joormann, J. (2008) Serotonergic function, two-mode models of self-regulation, and vulnerability to depression: What depression has in common with impulsive aggression. Psychological Bulletin 134(6):912–43. doi:10.1037/a0013740.Google Scholar
Corr, P. J. (ed.) (2008) The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Corr, P. J. (2016) Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality Questionnaires: Structural survey with recommendations. Personality and Individual Differences 89:6064. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.045.Google Scholar
Corr, P. J., DeYoung, C. G. & McNaughton, N. (2013) Motivation and personality: A neuropsychological perspective. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 7:158–75.Google Scholar
Corr, P. J. & McNaughton, N. (2012) Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits: A two stage (valuation-motivation) approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36:2339–54. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.013.Google Scholar
Frost, R. & McNaughton, N. (2017) The neural basis of delay discounting: A review and preliminary model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 79:4865. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.022.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1965) Aggression in rats as a function of frustrative nonreward in a straight alley. Psychonomic Science 3:99100.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1977) Drug effects on fear and frustration: Possible limbic site of action of minor tranquilizers. In: Handbook of psychopharmacology: Vol. 8: Drugs, neurotransmitters and behaviour, ed. Iversen, L. L., Iversen, S. D. & Snyder, S. H., pp. 433529. Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. & McNaughton, N. (2000) The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system, 2nd edition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, P. J., Chong, W., McNaughton, N. & Corr, P. J. (2011) An economic perspective on the reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Personality and Individual Differences 51:242–47.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1979) Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47:263–91.Google Scholar
Kelly, J. F. & Hake, D. F. (1970) An extinction-induced increase in an aggressive response with humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 14:153–64.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N. (1989) Biology and emotion. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N. (2018) What do you mean “anxiety”? Developing the first anxiety syndrome biomarker. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand 48:177–90. doi: 10.1080/03036758.2017.1358184.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N. & Corr, P. J. (2004) A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: Fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28:285305. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.005.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N. & Corr, P. J. (2018) Survival circuits and risk assessment. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 24:1420. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.018.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N., DeYoung, C. G. & Corr, P. J. (2016) Approach/avoidance. In: Neuroimaging personality, social cognition and character, ed. Absher, J. R. & Cloutier, J., pp. 2549. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D. & LeDoux, J. E. (2018) Editorial overview: Survival behaviors and circuits. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 24:168–71. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.10.004.Google Scholar
Motta, S. C., Carobrez, A. P. & Canteras, N. S. (2017) The periaqueductal gray and primal emotional processing critical to influence complex defensive responses, fear learning and reward seeking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 76:3947. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.012.Google Scholar
Novemsky, N. & Kahneman, D. (2005) The boundaries of loss aversion. Journal of Marketing Research 42:119–28.Google Scholar
Paulus, M. P. & Stein, M. B. (2006) An insular view of anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 60(4):383–87. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042.Google Scholar
Renfrew, J. W. & Hutchinson, R. R. (1983) The motivation of aggression. In Satinoff, E. & Teitelbaum, P. (Eds.), Motivation (Vol. 6): Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Shadli, S. M., McIntosh, J., Glue, P. & McNaughton, N. (2015) An improved human anxiety process biomarker: Characterisation of frequency band, personality, and pharmacology. Translational Psychiatry 5:e699. doi:10.1038/tp.2015.188.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1991) Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference dependent model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 106:1039–61.Google Scholar