Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:01:50.702Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Navigation bicoded as functions of x-y and time?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2013

James G. Phillips
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia. Jim.Phillips@monash.eduRowan.Ogeil@monash.edu
Rowan P. Ogeil
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia. Jim.Phillips@monash.eduRowan.Ogeil@monash.edu

Abstract

Evidence from egocentric space is cited to support bicoding of navigation in three-dimensional space. Horizontal distances and space are processed differently from the vertical. Indeed, effector systems are compatible in horizontal space, but potentially incompatible (or chaotic) during transitions to vertical motion. Navigation involves changes in coordinates, and animal models of navigation indicate that time has an important role.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avery, G. C. & Day, R. H. (1969) Basis of the horizontal–vertical illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology 81:376–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, G. C. & Gollub, J. G. (1996) Chaotic dynamics: An introduction. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L. & Phillips, J. G. (1992) Reappraising unilateral neglect. Australian Journal of Psychology 44:163–69.Google Scholar
Phillips, J. G. & Ogeil, R. P. (2010) Curved motions in horizontal and vertical orientations. Human Movement Science 29:737–50.Google Scholar
Phillips, J. G. Triggs, T. J. & Meehan, J. W. (2005) Forward/up directional incompatibilities during cursor placement within graphical user interfaces. Ergonomics 48:722–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reppert, S. M. (2006) A colorful model of the circadian clock. Cell 124:233–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shelton, P. A., Bowers, D. & Heilman, K. M. (1990) Peripersonal and vertical neglect. Brain 113:191205.Google Scholar
Worringham, C. J. & Beringer, D. B. (1989) Operator orientation and compatibility in visual-motor task performance. Ergonomics 32:387–99.Google Scholar
Zhu, H., Sauman, I., Yuan, Q., Casselman, A., Emery-Le, M., Emery, P. & Reppert, S. M. (2008) Cryptochromes define a novel circadian clock mechanism in Monarch butterflies that may underlie sun compass navigation. PLoS Biology 6:138–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed