No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The fire burns within: Individual motivations for self-sacrifice
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 December 2018
Abstract
Extreme self-sacrifice in intergroup conflict may be driven not only by situational factors generating “fusion,” but also by interindividual differences. Social value orientation is discussed as a potential contributor to self-harming behavior outside of intergroup conflicts and to the general propensity to participate in intergroup conflict. Social value orientation may therefore also be a person-specific determinant of extreme self-sacrifice in intergroup conflict.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018
References
Aaldering, H., Greer, L. L., Van Kleef, G. A. & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2013) Interest (mis)alignments in representative negotiations: Do pro-social agents fuel or reduce inter-group conflict? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 120(2):240–50. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.06.001.Google Scholar
Bélanger, J. J., Caouette, J., Sharvit, K. & Dugas, M. (2014) The psychology of martyrdom: Making the ultimate sacrifice in the name of a cause. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107(3):494–515. doi: 10.1037/a0036855.Google Scholar
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U. & Fehr, E. (2006) Parochial altruism in humans. Nature 442(7105):912–15. doi: 10.1038/nature04981.Google Scholar
Bobadilla, L. (2014) Martyrdom redefined: Self-destructive killers and vulnerable narcissism. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37(4):364–65. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13003361.Google Scholar
Bolton, G. E. & Ockenfels, A. (2000) ERC: A theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. The American Economic Review 90(1):166–93. doi: 10.1257/aer.90.1.166.Google Scholar
Choi, J.-K. & Bowles, S. (2007) The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318(5850):636–40. doi: 10.1126/science.1144237.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J., Wagner, C., du Plessis, I. & Birum, I. (2002) The psychological bases of ideology and prejudice: Testing a dual process model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83(1):75–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.75.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. & O'Brien, E. J. (1985) The person–situation debate in historical and current perspective. Psychological Bulletin 98(3):513–37. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.3.513.Google Scholar
Fehr, E. & Schmidt, K. M. (1999) A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 114(3):817–68. doi: 10.1162/003355399556151.Google Scholar
Fink, B. & Trivers, R. (2014) Cognitive simplicity and self-deception are crucial in martyrdom and suicide terrorism. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37(4):366–67. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13003385.Google Scholar
Gratz, K. L. (2003) Risk factors for and functions of deliberate self-harm: An empirical and conceptual review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 10(2):192–205. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg022.Google Scholar
Gratz, K. L., Conrad, S. D. & Roemer, L. (2002) Risk factors for deliberate self-harm among college students. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 72(1):128–40. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.72.1.128.Google Scholar
Griesinger, D. W. & Livingston, J. W. (1973) Toward a model of interpersonal motivation in experimental games. Behavioral Science 18(3):173–88. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830180305.Google Scholar
Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., Levin, S., Thomsen, L., Kteily, N. & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2012) Social dominance orientation: Revisiting the structure and function of a variable predicting social and political attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38(5):583–606. doi: 10.1177/0146167211432765.Google Scholar
Kranton, R. E. & Sanders, S. G. (2017) Groupy versus non-groupy social references: Personality, region, and political party. American Economic Review 107(5):65–69. doi: 10.1257/aer.p20171096.Google Scholar
Messick, D. M. & McClintock, C. G. (1968) Motivational bases of choice in experimental games. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 4(1):1–25. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(68)90046-2.Google Scholar
Perry, R., Sibley, C. G. & Duckitt, J. (2013) Dangerous and competitive worldviews: A meta-analysis of their associations with social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism. Journal of Research in Personality 47(1):116–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.10.004.Google Scholar
Post, J. M., Ali, F., Henderson, S. W., Shanfield, S., Victoroff, J. & Weine, S. (2009) The psychology of suicide terrorism. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 72(1):13–31. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2009.72.1.13.Google Scholar
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M. & Malle, B. F. (1994) Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67:741–63. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741.Google Scholar
Sidanius, J., Pratto, F. & Mitchell, M. (1994) In-group identification, social dominance orientation, and differential intergroup social allocation. The Journal of Social Psychology 134(2):151–67. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1994.9711378.Google Scholar
Thielmann, I. & Böhm, R. (2016) Who does (not) participate in intergroup conflict? Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(8):778–87. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616660160.Google Scholar
Thomsen, L., Green, E. G. T. & Sidanius, J. (2008) We will hunt them down: How social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism fuel ethnic persecution of immigrants in fundamentally different ways. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44(6):1455–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.06.011.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (2006) The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 907(1):114–31. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06619.x.Google Scholar
Vazire, S. & Funder, D. C. (2006) Impulsivity and the self-defeating behavior of narcissists. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10(2):154–65. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_4.Google Scholar
Target article
Dying for the group: Towards a general theory of extreme self-sacrifice
Related commentaries (29)
A potential explanation for self-radicalisation
Accumulative fusion and the issue of age: Reconciling the model with the data
Altruism, collective rationality, and extreme self-sacrifice
Communal sharing/identity fusion does not require reflection on episodic memory of shared experience or trauma – and usually generates kindness
Considering selection pressures for identity fusion and self-sacrifice in small-scale societies
Does identity fusion give rise to the group – or the reverse? Politics- versus community-based groups
Dying for your group or for your faith? On the power of belief
Extreme self-sacrifice beyond fusion: Moral expansiveness and the special case of allyship
Identity fusion and fitness interdependence
Identity fusion “in the wild”: Moving toward or away from a general theory of identity fusion?
Individual difference in acts of self-sacrifice
Individuals, traditions, and the righteous
Motivational (con)fusion: Identity fusion does not quell personal self-interest
Origins of social fusion
Segregation and belief polarization as boundary conditions for when fusion leads to self-sacrifice
Self-sacrifice as a social signal
Self-sacrifice for a cause: The role of ideas and beliefs in motivating human conflict
Self-sacrifice for in-group's history: A diachronic perspective
Strength in numbers: A survival strategy that helps explain social bonding and commitment
The analytic utility of distinguishing fighting from dying
The fire burns within: Individual motivations for self-sacrifice
The importance of environmental threats and ideology in explaining extreme self-sacrifice
The motivation to sacrifice for a cause reflects a basic cognitive bias
The power of norms to sway fused group members
The role of entitativity in perpetuating cycles of violence
Toward a more comprehensive theory of self-sacrificial violence
What fuses sports fans?
What motivates devoted actors to extreme sacrifice, identity fusion, or sacred values?
“Self-sacrifice” as an accidental outcome of extreme within-group mutualism
Author response
Four things we need to know about extreme self-sacrifice